Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-fwgfc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-08T11:09:54.187Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Adaptive narratives and fantastical falsehoods?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 May 2023

Aaron D. Lightner*
Affiliation:
Department of the Study of Religion, Aarhus University, Jens Chr. Skous Vej 3, 8000 Aarhus C, Denmark. [email protected]

Abstract

Johnson et al. make a strong case for Conviction Narrative Theory, but it remains unclear why so many adaptive narratives include supernatural causes and other falsehoods. Focusing on religions, I argue that an adaptive decision-making system might include supernatural falsehoods because they simplify complex problems, they are sensitive to long-term incentives, and they evoke strong emotions in a communicative context.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Atran, S., Medin, D., Ross, N., Lynch, E., Vapnarsky, V., Ek, E., … Baran, M. (2002). Folkecology, cultural epidemiology, and the spirit of the commons: A garden experiment in the Maya lowlands, 19912001. Current Anthropology, 43(3), 421450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bendixen, T., Apicella, C., Atkinson, Q., Cohen, E., Henrich, J., McNamara, R. A., … Purzycki, B. G. (2021). Appealing to the minds of gods: A novel cultural evolutionary account of religious appeals and an empirical assessment using ethnographic data from eight diverse societies.Google Scholar
Bendixen, T., & Purzycki, B. G. (2017). Peering into the minds of gods: What cross-cultural variation in gods’ concerns can tell us about the evolution of religion. Journal for the Cognitive Science of Religion, 5(2), 142165.Google Scholar
Bird, D. W., Bird, R. B., Codding, B. F., & Taylor, N. (2016). A landscape architecture of fire: Cultural emergence and ecological pyrodiversity in Australia's western desert. Current Anthropology, 57(S13), S65S79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bird, R. B., Tayor, N., Codding, B. F., & Bird, D. W. (2013). Niche construction and dreaming logic: Aboriginal patch mosaic burning and varanid lizards (Varanus gouldii) in Australia. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 280(1772), 20132297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brighton, H., & Gigerenzer, G. (2012). Homo heuristicus and the bias-variance dilemma. In Schulkin, J. (Ed.), Action, perception and the brain (pp. 6891). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delton, A. W., Krasnow, M. M., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2011). Evolution of direct reciprocity under uncertainty can explain human generosity in one-shot encounters. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(32), 1333513340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennett, D. C. (1987). The intentional stance. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Douglas, M. (2013). Risk and blame: Essays in cultural theory. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1937). Witchcraft, oracles, and magic among the Azande. Clarendon Press, abridged with an introd. by eva gillies edition.Google Scholar
Filotas, E., Parrott, L., Burton, P. J., Chazdon, R. L., Coates, K. D., Coll, L., … Messier, C. (2014). Viewing forests through the lens of complex systems science. Ecosphere, 5(1), 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitouchi, L., & Singh, M. (2021). Supernatural punishment beliefs as cognitively compelling tools of social control. Current Opinion in Psychology, 44, 252257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerstenberg, T., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2017). Intuitive theories. Oxford handbook of causal reasoning (pp. 515548). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Howell, S. (2012). Knowledge, morality, and causality in a “luckless” society: The case of the Chewong in the Malaysian rain forest. Social Analysis, 56(1), 133147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ingold, T. (2006). Rethinking the animate, re-animating thought. Ethnos, 71(1), 920.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, S. G. B., Matiashvili, T., & Tuckett, D. (2019). Explaining the past, predicting the future: How attributions for past price changes affect price expectations. Manuscript in preparation.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2013). Thinking, fast and slow. Farrar, Straus and Giroux, 1st pbk. ed edition.Google Scholar
Kim, J. H., & Ryoo, H. H. (2011). Common stocks as a hedge against inflation: Evidence from century-long us data. Economics Letters, 113(2), 168171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lightner, A. D., & Hagen, E. (2022). All models are wrong, and some are religious: Supernatural explanations as abstract and useful falsehoods about complex realities. Human Nature, 33, 425462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lightner, A. D., & Purzycki, B. G. (2023). Game theoretical aspects of the minds of gods. In Purzycki, B. G. & Bendixen, T. (Eds.), The minds of gods: A cross-disciplinary survey (pp. 133147). Bloomsbury Publishing.Google Scholar
Malinowski, B. (1932). Argonauts of the Western Pacific. George Routledge and Sons, Limited.Google Scholar
Morin, O., & Sobchuk, O. (2022). Why monsters are dangerous.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
ojalehto mays, B., Seligman, R., & Medin, D. L. (2020). Cognition beyond the human: Cognitive psychology and the new animism. Ethos, 48(1), 5073.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purzycki, B. G., Bendixen, T., Lightner, A. D., & Sosis, R. (2022). Gods, games, and the socioecological landscape. Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology, 3, 100057.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quillien, T. (2020). When do we think that x caused y? Cognition, 205, 104410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sperber, D., Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. (1995). Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate. Clarendon Press Oxford.Google Scholar
Sugiyama, M. S., & Sugiyama, L. S. (2011). Once the child is lost he dies: Monster stories vis-a-vis the problem of errant children. In Slingerland, E. & Collard, M. (Eds.), Creating consilience: Integrating the sciences and the humanities (pp. 351371). Oxford Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tucker, B. (2017). From risk and time preferences to cultural models of causality: On the challenges and possibilities of field experiments, with examples from rural southwestern Madagascar. In Stevens, Jeffrey R. (Ed.), Impulsivity: How time and risk influence decision making (pp. 61114). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodward, J. (2006). Sensitive and insensitive causation. The Philosophical Review, 115(1), 150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zinn, J. O. (2016). “In-between” and other reasonable ways to deal with risk and uncertainty: A review article. Health, Risk & Society, 18(7–8), 348366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed