Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T23:53:19.844Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Shared intentionality shapes humans' technical know-how

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 August 2020

Henrike Moll
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, [email protected]
Ryan Nichols
Affiliation:
Department of Philosophy, California State University Fullerton, Fullerton, CA92831.
Ellyn Pueschel
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, [email protected]

Abstract

Osiurak and Reynaud argue that cumulative technological culture is made possible by a “non-social cognitive structure” (sect. 1, para. 1) and they offer an account that aims “to escape from the social dimension” (sect. 1, para. 2) of human cognition. We challenge their position by arguing that human technical rationality is unintelligible outside of our species' uniquely social form of life, which is defined by shared intentionality (Kern & Moll 2017, Philosophical Psychology30(3):319–37; Tomasello 2019a, Becoming human: A theory of ontogeny. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press).

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (1995) Use of social information in the problem solving of orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) and human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology 109(3):308. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.109.3.308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanus, D., Mendes, N., Tennie, C. & Call, J. (2011) Comparing the performances of apes (Gorilla gorilla, Pan troglodytes, Pongo pygmaeus) and human children (Homo sapiens) in the floating peanut task. PLoS One 6(6):e19555. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kern, A. & Moll, H. (2017) On the transformative character of collective intentionality and the uniqueness of the human. Philosophical Psychology 30(3):319–37. doi: 10.1080/09515089.2017.1295648.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyons, D. E., Young, A. G. & Keil, F. C. (2007) The hidden structure of overimitation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 104(50):19751–56. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704452104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGuigan, N., Whiten, A., Flynn, E. F. & Horner, V. (2007) Imitation of causally opaque versus causally transparent tool use by 3- and 5-year-old children. Cognitive Development 22(3):353–64. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.01.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moll, H. (2018) The transformative cultural intelligence hypothesis: Evidence from young children's problem-solving. Review of Philosophy and Psychology 9(1):161–75. doi: 10.1007/s13164-017-0342-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2019a) Becoming human: A theory of ontogeny. Belknap Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar