Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T05:56:05.595Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Shared intentionality and the representation of groups; or, how to build a socially adept robot

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 July 2022

Ben Phillips*
Affiliation:
Department of Philosophy, School of History, Philosophy and Religious Studies, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ 85281, [email protected]://www.bensphillips.com/

Abstract

Pietraszewski provides a compelling case that representations of certain interaction-types are the “cognitive primitives” that allow all tokens of group-in-conflict to be represented within the mind. Here, I argue that the folk concept GROUP encodes shared intentions and goals as more central than these interaction-types, and that providing a computational theory of social groups will be more difficult than Pietraszewski envisages.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ahn, W., Kim, N. S., Lassaline, M. E., & Dennis, M. J. (2000). Causal status as a determinant of feature centrality. Cognitive Psychology, 41(4), 361416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bratman, M. (1999). Faces of intention: Selected essays on intention and agency, Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (2007). Baboon metaphysics: The evolution of a social mind. The University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ip, G. W. M., Chiu, C. Y., & Wan, C. (2006). Birds of a feather and birds flocking together: Physical versus behavioral cues may lead to trait- versus goal-based group perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 368381.Google ScholarPubMed
Lakens, D., & Stel, M. (2011). If they move in sync, they must feel in sync: Movement synchrony leads to attributions of rapport and entitativity. Social Cognition, 29, 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClung, J., Placì, S., Bangerter, A., Clément, F., & Bshary, R. (2017). The language of cooperation: Shared intentionality drives variation in helping as a function of group membership. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 284.Google ScholarPubMed
Phillips, B. (2021a). The roots of racial categorization. Review of Philosophy and Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-021-00525-w.Google Scholar
Phillips, B. (2021b). Entitativity and implicit measures of social cognition. Mind & Language. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloman, S. A., Love, B. C., & Ahn, W.-K. (1998). Feature centrality and conceptual coherence. Cognitive Science, 22(2), 189228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ting, F., He, Z., & Baillargeon, R. (2019). Toddlers and infants expect individuals to refrain from helping an ingroup victim's aggressor. PNAS, 116(13), 60256034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(5), 675691. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, S., & Gos, C. (2019). Perceiving social cohesion: Movement synchrony and task demands both matter. Perception, 48(4), 316329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed