Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T02:47:20.288Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Let us be careful with the evidence on mentalizing, cognitive biases, and religious beliefs

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 March 2016

Marjaana Lindeman
Affiliation:
Institute of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsinki, FI-00014 University of Helsinki, [email protected]@helsinki.fihttp://blogs.helsinki.fi/everyday-thinking/
Annika M. Svedholm-Häkkinen
Affiliation:
Institute of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsinki, FI-00014 University of Helsinki, [email protected]@helsinki.fihttp://blogs.helsinki.fi/everyday-thinking/

Abstract

Norenzayan et al.’s theoretical synthesis is highly plausible and commendable. However, the empirical evidence for the arguments on mentalizing, cognitive biases, and religious belief is currently not as strong as the writers suggest. Although certainly abundant and compelling, this evidence is indirect, contradictory, and weak and must be acknowledged as such. More direct studies are needed to support the theory.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allen, J. G. (2006) Mentalizing in practice. In: Handbook of mentalization-based treatment, ed. Allen, E. & Fonagy, P., pp. 330. Wiley.Google Scholar
Banerjee, K. & Bloom, P. (2014) Why did this happen to me? Religious believers' and non-believers' teleological reasoning about life events. Cognition 133:277303. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.06.017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnes, K. & Gibson, N. J. (2013) Supernatural agency: Individual difference predictors and situational correlates. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 23:4262. doi: 10.1080/10508619.2013.739066.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. & Wheelwright, S. (2004) The empathy quotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 34:163–75. doi: 10.1023/B:JADD.0000022607.19833.00.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y. & Plumb, I. (2001) The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 42:241–51. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00715.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, J. L. (2004) Why would anyone believe in God? AltaMira Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2012) Religion, morality, evolution. Annual Review of Psychology 63:179–99. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100334.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (2001) Religion explained. The evolutionary origins of religious thought. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Diduca, D. & Joseph, S. (1997) Schizotypal traits and dimensions of religiosity. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 36:635–38. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1997.tb01270.x.Google Scholar
Frith, U. & Frith, C. D. (2003) Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 358:459–73. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1218.Google Scholar
Guthrie, S. (1993) Faces in the clouds. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kelemen, D., Rottman, J. & Seston, R. (2013) Professional physical scientists display tenacious teleological tendencies: Purpose-based reasoning as a cognitive default. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 42:1074–83. doi: 10.1037/a0030399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirkpatrick, L. A. (1999) Toward an evolutionary psychology of religion and personality. Journal of Personality 67:921–52. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00078.Google Scholar
Lawson, E. T. & McCauley, R. N. (1993) Rethinking religion: Connecting cognition and culture. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lindeman, M., Svedholm-Häkkinen, A. M. & Lipsanen, J. (2015) Ontological confusions but not mentalizing abilities predict religious belief, paranormal belief, and belief in supernatural purpose. Cognition 134:6376. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.09.008.Google Scholar
Lombrozo, T., Kelemen, D. & Zaitchik, D. (2007) Inferring design: Evidence of a preference for teleological explanations in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Psychological Science 18:9991006. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02015.x.Google Scholar
Maltby, J., Garner, I., Alan Lewis, C. & Day, L. (2000) Religious orientation and schizotypal traits. Personality and Individual Differences 28:143–51. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00090-2.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A., Gervais, W. M. & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2012) Mentalizing deficits constrain belief in a personal God. PLOS ONE 7:e36880. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036880.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A. & Shariff, A. F. (2008) The origin and evolution of religious prosociality. Science 322:5862. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1158757.Google Scholar
Schaap-Jonker, H., Sizoo, B., van Schothorst-van Roekel, J. & Corveleyn, J. (2013) Autism spectrum disorders and the image of God as a core aspect of religiousness. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 23:145–60. doi: 10.1080/10508619.2012.688005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valla, J. M., Ganzel, B. L., Yoder, K. J., Chen, G. M., Lyman, L. T., Sidari, A. P., Wong, S. K. (2010) More than maths and mindreading: Sex differences in empathizing/systemizing covariance. Autism Research 3:174–84. doi: 10.1002/aur.143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waytz, A., Gray, K., Epley, N. & Wegner, D. M. (2010) Causes and consequences of mind perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14:383–88. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.006.Google Scholar
Willard, A. K. & Norenzayan, A. (2013) Cognitive biases explain religious belief, paranormal belief, and belief in life's purpose. Cognition 129:379–91. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.07.016.Google Scholar