Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T03:17:14.610Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Supporting the weight of the elephant in the room: Technical intelligence propped up by social cognition and language

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 August 2020

Alex Thornton
Affiliation:
Centre for Ecology and Conservation, University of Exeter, PenrynTR10 9FE, [email protected]://www.wildcognitionresearch.com/
Francesca Happé
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King's College London, LondonSE5 8AF, [email protected]://www.kcl.ac.uk/people/francesca-happe
Christine A. Caldwell
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Stirling, StirlingFK9 4LA, UK. [email protected]://sites.google.com/site/christineannacaldwell/

Abstract

We consider the evolutionary plausibility of Osiurak and Reynaud's (O&R) arguments. We argue that technical reasoning is not quite the magic bullet that O&R assume, and instead propose a co-evolutionary account of the interplay between technical reasoning and social learning, with language emerging as a vital issue neglected in O&R's account.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Beck, S. R., Apperly, I. A., Chappell, J., Guthrie, C. & Cutting, N. (2011) Making tools isn't child's play. Cognition 119(2):301306. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.01.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. (1996) Why culture is common, but cultural evolution is rare. Proceedings of the British Academy 88:7793.Google Scholar
Caldwell, C. A. & Millen, A. E. (2009) Social learning mechanisms and cumulative cultural evolution: Is imitation necessary? Psychological Science 20(12):1478–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caldwell, C. A., Renner, E. & Atkinson, M. (2018) Human teaching and cumulative cultural evolution. Review of Philosophy and Psychology 9(4):751–70. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-017-0346-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendal, R. L., Boogert, N. J., Rendell, L., Laland, K. N., Webster, M. & Jones, P. L. (2018) Social learning strategies: Bridge-building between fields. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 22:651–65. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.04.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laland, K. N. (2017b) The origins of language in teaching. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 24:225–31. Available at: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1077-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesoudi, A. & Thornton, A. (2018) What is cumulative cultural evolution? Proceedings of the Royal Society B 285:20180712. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.0712.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, T. J. H., Uomini, N. T., Rendell, L. E., Chouinard-Thuly, L., Street, S. E., Lewis, H. M., Cross, C. P., Evans, C., Kearney, R., de la Torre, I., Whiten, A. & Laland, K. N. (2015) Experimental evidence for the co-evolution of hominin tool-making teaching and language. Nature Communications 6:18. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms7029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nielsen, M. (2013) Young children's imitative and innovative behaviour on the floating object task. Infant and Child Development 22(6):4452. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1765.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saldana, C., Fagot, J., Kirby, S., Smith, K. & Claidière, N. (2019) High-fidelity copying is not necessarily the key to cumulative cultural evolution: A study in monkeys and children. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 286:20190729. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2019.0729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sasaki, T. & Biro, D. (2017) Cumulative culture can emerge from collective intelligence in animal groups. Nature Communications 8(1):15049. doi: 10.1038/ncomms15049.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thornton, A. & Clutton-Brock, T. (2011) Social learning and the development of individual and group behaviour in mammal societies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 366:978–87. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thornton, A. & Raihani, N. (2008) The evolution of teaching. Animal Behaviour 75:1823–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C. & Ratner, H. H. (1993) Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16:495552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A. (2019a) Cultural evolution in animals. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics 50:122. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-110218-025040.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zwirner, E. & Thornton, A. (2015) Cognitive requirements of cumulative culture: Teaching is useful but not essential. Scientific Reports 5:16781. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1038/srep16781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar