Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T19:05:54.620Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Subjectivity may hinder the application of Kline's teaching framework in comparative contexts

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 June 2015

Lewis G. Dean
Affiliation:
Centre for Social Learning and Cognitive Evolution, School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, Fife, KY16 9JP, United Kingdom. [email protected]/profile/lgd1
Rachel L. Kendal
Affiliation:
Centre for Coevolution of Biology and Culture, Department of Anthropology, Durham University, Durham DH1 3LE, United Kingdom. [email protected]/research/directory/staff/?mode=staff&id=5444

Abstract

We welcome Kline's attempt to develop an overarching framework to allow much needed collaboration between fields in the study of teaching. While we see much utility in this enterprise, we are concerned that there is too much focus on the behavior of the teacher, without examining results or costs, and the categories within the framework are not sufficiently distinct.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Burkart, J. M. & van Schaik, C. P. (2010) Cognitive consequences of cooperative breeding in primates? Animal Cognition 13(1):119. doi:10.1007/s10071-009-0263-7.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. & Whiten, A., eds. (1988) Machiavellian intelligence. Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes and humans. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Caldwell, C. A. & Whiten, A. (2003) Scrounging facilitates social learning in common marmosets, Callithrix jacchus . Animal Behaviour 65(6):1085–92. doi:10.1006/anbe.2003.2145.Google Scholar
Caro, T. M. & Hauser, M. D. (1992) Is there teaching in nonhuman animals? The Quarterly Review of Biology 67(2):151–74. Available at: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.2307/2831436 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coussi-Korbel, S. & Fragaszy, D. M. (1995) On the relation between social dynamics and social learning. Animal Behaviour 50:1441–53.Google Scholar
De Waal, F. B. M. & Luttrell, L. M. (1989) Toward a comparative socioecology of the genus Macaca: Different dominance styles in rhesus and stumptail monkeys. American Journal of Primatology 19:83109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fogarty, L., Strimling, P. & Laland, K. N. (2011) The evolution of teaching. Evolution 65(10):2760–70. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2011.01370.x.Google Scholar
Giraldeau, L. & Lefebvre, L. (1987) Scrounging prevents cultural transmission of food-finding behaviour in pigeons. Animal Behaviour 35(2):387–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoppitt, W. J. E., Brown, G. R., Kendal, R., Rendell, L., Thornton, A., Webster, M. M. & Laland, K. N. (2008) Lessons from animal teaching. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 23(9):486–93. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2008.05.008.Google Scholar
Ottoni, E., Dogo de Resende, B. & Izar, P. (2005) Watching the best nutcrackers: What capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) know about others' tool-using skills. Animal Cognition 24:215–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sapolsky, R. (2006) Culture in animals: The case of a non-human primate culture of low aggression and high affiliation. Social Forces 85(1):217–33.Google Scholar
Sueur, C., Petit, O., De Marco, A., Jacobs, A. T., Watanabe, K. & Thierry, B. (2011) A comparative network analysis of social style in macaques. Animal Behaviour 82(4):845–52. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.07.020.Google Scholar
Thornton, A. & Raihani, N. J. (2010) Identifying teaching in wild animals. Learning and Behavior 38(3):297309.Google Scholar
Whiten, A. & Ham, R. (1992) On the nature and evolution of imitation in the animal kingdom: Reappraisal of a century of research. Advances in the Study of Behavior 21:239–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar