Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T17:11:48.202Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Recognizing religion's dark side: Religious ritual increases antisociality and hinders self-control

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 March 2016

Nicholas M. Hobson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Scarborough, Toronto, Ontario M1C 1A4Canada. [email protected]@utoronto.cawww.michaelinzlicht.com
Michael Inzlicht
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Scarborough, Toronto, Ontario M1C 1A4Canada. [email protected]@utoronto.cawww.michaelinzlicht.com

Abstract

The target article develops an account of religious prosociality that is driven by increases in self-control. We suggest this account is incomplete. Although religion might increase prosociality to the in-group, it decreases it to the much larger out-group. Rituals, for example, lead to out-group derogation. We also challenge the link between religion and improved self-control, offering evidence that religion hinders self-control.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allport, G. W. & Ross, M. J. (1967) Personal religious orientation and prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 5:432–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atran, S. & Ginges, J. (2012) Religious and sacred imperatives in human conflict. Science 336:855–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bulbulia, J. (2004) Religious costs as adaptations that signal altruistic intention. Evolution and Cognition 10:938.Google Scholar
Choi, J. K. & Bowles, S. (2007) The coevolution of parochial altruism and war. Science 318:636–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, R. (1992) Learned industriousness. Psychological Review 99:248–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ginges, J., Atran, S., Medin, D. & Shikaki, K. (2007) Sacred bounds on rational resolution of violent political conflict. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104:7357–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ginges, J., Hansen, I. & Norenzayan, A. (2009) Religion and support for suicide attacks. Psychological Science 20:224–30.Google Scholar
Good, M., Inzlicht, M. & Larson, M. J. (2015) God will forgive: Reflecting on God's love decreases neurophysiological response to errors. Social Cognitive Affective Neuroscience 10:357–63.Google Scholar
Hirsh, J. B., DeYoung, C. G., Xu, X. & Peterson, J. B. (2010) Compassionate liberals and polite conservatives: Associations of agreeableness with political ideology and moral values. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 36:655–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, N. M. (2013) The binding and blinding effects of collective ritual: Intergroup biases and processes. Master's thesis. University of Toronto: Canada.Google Scholar
Hobson, N. M., Norton, M. I., Gino, F. & Inzlicht, M. (2015) Mock ritual leads to intergroup biases in behavior and neurophysiology, Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Association for Psychological Science, New York, NY.Google Scholar
Hunsberger, B., Jackson, L. M. (2005) Religion, meaning, and prejudice. Journal of Social Issues 61:807–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inzlicht, M., McGregor, I., Hirsh, J. B. & Nash, K. (2009) Neural markers of religious conviction. Psychological Science 20:385–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inzlicht, M. & Tullett, A. M. (2010) Reflecting on god: Religious primes can reduce neuropsychological response to errors. Psychological Science 21:1181–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inzlicht, M., Tullett, A. & Good, M. (2011) The need to believe: A neuroscience account of religion as a motivated process. Religion, Brain and Behavior 1:192212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCullough, M. E. & Willoughby, B. L. B. (2009) Religion, self-regulation, and self-control: Associations, explanations, and implications. Psychological Bulletin 135:6993. doi: 10.1037/a0014213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neuberg, S. L., Warner, C. Mistler, S. Berlin, A., Hill, E., Johnson, J., Filip-Crawford, G., Millsap, R., Thomas, G. M., Winkelman, M., Broome, B., Taylor, T. & Schober. J. (2014) Religion and intergroup conflict: Findings from the Global Group Relations Project. Psychological Science 25:198206.Google Scholar
Rounding, K., Lee, A., Jacobson, J. & Ji, L.-J. (2012) Religion replenishes self-control. Psychological Science 23:635–42.Google Scholar
Schjoedt, U., Sørensen, J., Nielbo, K. L., Xygalatas, D., Mitkidis, P. & Bulbulia, J. (2013) Cognitive depletion in religious interactions. Religion, Brain and Behavior 3:3986.Google Scholar