Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T19:59:05.151Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Learning about teaching requires thinking about the learner

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 June 2015

Kathleen H. Corriveau*
Affiliation:
School of Education, Boston University, Boston, MA 02215. [email protected]/learninglab

Abstract

Kline argues for an expanded taxonomy of teaching focusing on the adaptive behaviors needed to solve learning problems. Absent from her analysis is an explicit definition of learning, or a discussion of the iterative nature of the relationship between teaching and learning. Including the learner in the discussion may help to distinguish among the adaptive values of different teaching behaviors.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bates, E. (1976) Language and context: Studies in the acquisition of pragmatics. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (1998) Cognitive tracks of cultural inheritance: How evolved intuitive ontology governs cultural transmission. American Anthropologist 100:876–89. doi: 10.1525/aa.1998.100.4.876.Google Scholar
Butterworth, G. & Jarrett, N. (1991) What minds have in common is space: Spatial mechanisms serving joint visual attention in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 9:5572. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.1991.tb00862.x.Google Scholar
Caro, T. M. & Hauser, M. D. (1992) Is there teaching in nonhuman animals? The Quarterly Review of Biology 67(2):151–74. Available at: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.2307/2831436 Google Scholar
Chouinard, M. (2007) Children's questions: A mechanism for cognitive development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Serial No. 286, Vol. 72, No. 1, pp. 1129.Google Scholar
Corriveau, K. H., Min, G. & Kurkul, K. (2014) Cultural differences in children's learning from others. In: Trust and skepticism: Children's selective learning from testimony, ed. Einav, S. & Robinson, E., pp. 95109. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Csibra, G. & Gergely, G. (2006) Social learning and social cognition: The case for pedagogy. In: Attention and Performance XX1: Processes of change in brain and cognitive development, ed. Munakata, Y. & Johnson, M. H., pp. 249274. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Csibra, G. & Gergely, G. (2009) Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13(4):148–53.Google Scholar
Csibra, G. & Gergely, G. (2011) Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 366(1567):1149–57. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0319.Google Scholar
Flynn, E. & Whiten, A. (2008) Cultural transmission of tool use in young children: A diffusion chain study. Social Development 17:699718.Google Scholar
Flynn, E. & Whiten, A. (2012) Experimental “microcultures” in young children: Identifying biographic, cognitive, and social predictors of information transmission. Child Development 83:911–25. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01747.x.Google Scholar
Franks, N. & Richardson, T. (2006) Teaching in tandem-running ants. Nature 439(7073):153.Google Scholar
Gelman, S. A., Ware, E. A., Manczak, E. M. & Graham, S. A. (2013) Children's sensitivity to the knowledge expressed in pedagogical and non-pedagogical contexts. Developmental Psychology 49:491504. doi: 10.1037/a0027901.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. (2012) Trusting what you're told: How children learn from others. Belknap Press/Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. & Corriveau, K. H. (2011) Young children's selective trust in informants. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 366:1179–87. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0321.Google Scholar
Keil, F. C., Stein, C., Webb, L., Billings, V. D. & Rozenblit, L. (2008) Discerning the division of cognitive labor: An emerging understanding of how knowledge is clustered in other minds. Cognitive Science 32:259300. doi: 10.1080/03640210701863339.Google Scholar
Lyons, D. E., Young, A. G. & Keil, F. C. (2007) The hidden structure of overimitation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 104(50):19751–56. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704452104.Google Scholar
Mills, C. M. & Keil, F. C. (2004) Knowing the limits of one's understanding: The development of an awareness of an illusion of explanatory depth. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 87:132. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2003.09.003.Google Scholar
Richardson, T. O., Sleeman, P. A., McNamara, J. M., Houston, A. I. & Franks, N. R. (2007) Teaching with evaluation in ants. Current Biology 17(17):1520–26. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.032.Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J. & Boyd, R. (2005) Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Sobel, D. M. & Kushnir, T. (2013) Knowledge matters: How children evaluate the reliability of testimony as a process of rational inference. Psychological Review 120:779–97. doi: 10.1037/a0034191.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1996) Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Strauss, S., Ziv, M. & Stein, A. (2002) Teaching as a natural cognition and its relations to preschoolers' developing theory of mind. Cognitive Development 17:1473–87. doi: 10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00128-4.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1995) Joint attention as social cognition. In: Joint attention: Its origins and role in development, ed. Moore, C. & Dunham, P. J., pp. 103–31. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2009) The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C. & Ratner, H. H. (1993) Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16:495552.Google Scholar
Warneken, F. (2013) Young children proactively remedy unnoticed accidents. Cognition 126(1):101108. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.09.011.Google Scholar
Wood, L., Kendal, R. & Flynn, E. (2012) Context-dependent model-based biases in cultural transmission: Children's imitation is affected by model age over model knowledgeable state. Evolution and Human Behavior 33:387–94. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.11.010.Google Scholar