No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Competition elicits arousal and affect
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 05 January 2017
Abstract
The emotion–cognition integration in Mather et al. can be extended by specifying the relationship between competition and arousal in the reverse direction. According to affective monitoring, competition raises arousal, which, when sustained, results in negative affect, evoking theta oscillations, and when resolved, in positive affect, evoking gamma oscillations. Competition should be considered a core process in both cognition and emotion.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016
References
Bauer, F., Cheadle, S. W., Parton, A., Müller, H. J. & Usher, M. (2009) Gamma flicker triggers attentional selection without awareness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
106:1666–71.Google Scholar
Desimone, R. & Duncan, J. (1995) Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience
18:193–222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dreisbach, G. & Fischer, R. (2012) Conflicts as aversive signals. Brain and Cognition
78:94–98.Google Scholar
Duncan, J. (1996) Cooperating brain systems in selective perception and action. In: Attention and Performance XVI, ed. Inui, T. & McClelland, J. L., pp. 549–78. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fazendeiro, T., Chenier, T. & Winkielman, P. (2007) How dynamics of thinking create affective and cognitive feelings: Psychology and neuroscience of the connection between fluency, liking, and memory. In:
Social neuroscience, ed. Harmon-Jones, E. & Winkielman, P., pp. 271–89. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Goolsby, B. A., Shapiro, K. L., Silvert, L., Kiss, M., Fragopanagos, N., Taylor, J. G., Eimer, M., Nobre, A. C. & Raymond, J. E. (2009) Feature-based inhibition underlies the affective consequences of attention. Visual Cognition
17:500–30.Google Scholar
Heerebout, B. T. & Phaf, R. H. (2010a) Emergent oscillations in evolutionary simulations: Oscillating networks increase switching efficacy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
22:807–23.Google Scholar
Heerebout, B. T. & Phaf, R. H. (2010b) Good vibrations switch attention: An affective function for network oscillations in evolutionary simulations. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience
10:217–29.Google Scholar
Heerebout, B. T., Tap, A. E. Y., Rotteveel, M. & Phaf, R. H. (2013) Gamma flicker elicits positive affect without awareness. Consciousness and Cognition
22:281–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murre, J., Phaf, R. & Wolters, G. (1992) CALM: Categorizing and learning module. Neural Networks
5:55–82.Google Scholar
Pessoa, L. (2008) On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nature
9:148–58.Google ScholarPubMed
Phaf, R. H. & Rotteveel, M. (2005) Affective modulation of recognition bias. Emotion
5:309–18.Google Scholar
Phaf, R. H. & Rotteveel, M. (2012) Affective monitoring: A generic mechanism for affect elicitation. Frontiers in Psychology
3:47. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00047.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phaf, R. H., Van der Heijden, A. H. C. & Hudson, P. T. (1990) SLAM: A connectionist model for attention in visual selection tasks. Cognitive Psychology
22:273–341.Google Scholar
Raymond, J. E., Fenske, M. J. & Tavassoli, N. T. (2003) Selective attention determines emotional responses to novel visual stimuli. Psychological Science
14:537–42.Google Scholar
Rotteveel, M. & Phaf, R. H. (2007) Mere exposure in reverse: Mood and motion modulate memory bias. Cognition and Emotion
21:1323–46.Google Scholar
Srinivasan, N. & Hanif, A. (2010) Global-happy and local-sad: Perceptual processing affects emotion identification. Cognition and Emotion
24:1062–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sutherland, M. R. & Mather, M.
(under review)
Both positive and negative arousing sounds increase the impact of visual salience.Google Scholar
Target article
Norepinephrine ignites local hotspots of neuronal excitation: How arousal amplifies selectivity in perception and memory
Related commentaries (27)
Adaptive memory systems for remembering the salient and the seemingly mundane
Amplified selectivity in cognitive processing implements the neural gain model of norepinephrine function
Are there “local hotspots?” When concepts of cognitive psychology do not fit with physiological results
Arousal-biased preferences for sensory input: An agent-centered and multisource perspective
Bidirectional synaptic plasticity can explain bidirectional retrograde effects of emotion on memory
Bodily arousal differentially impacts stimulus processing and memory: Norepinephrine in interoception
Cognitive control, dynamic salience, and the imperative toward computational accounts of neuromodulatory function
Competition elicits arousal and affect
Contemplating the GANE model using an extreme case paradigm
Dentate gyrus and hilar region revisited
Does arousal enhance apical amplification and disamplification?
Effect of arousal on perception as studied through the lens of the motor correlates of sexual arousal
Emotional memory: From affective relevance to arousal
Emotionally arousing context modulates the ERP correlates of neutral picture processing: An ERP test of the GANE model
For better or worse, or for a change?
GANEing on emotion and emotion regulation
Glutamate and norepinephrine interaction: Relevance to higher cognitive operations and psychopathology
Importance of amygdala noradrenergic activity and large-scale neural networks in regulating emotional arousal effects on perception and memory1
Interactions of noradrenaline and cortisol and the induction of indelible memories
Locus coeruleus reports changes in environmental contingencies
Once more with feeling: On the explanatory limits of the GANE model and the missing role of subjective experience
The Fluency Amplification Model supports the GANE principle of arousal enhancement
The role of arousal in predictive coding
What BANE can offer GANE: Individual differences in function of hotspot mechanisms
What do we GANE with age?
Why we forget our dreams: Acetylcholine and norepinephrine in wakefulness and REM sleep
“What have we GANEd?” A theoretical construct to explain experimental evidence for noradrenergic regulation of sensory signal processing
Author response
GANEing traction: The broad applicability of NE hotspots to diverse cognitive and arousal phenomena