Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T12:53:56.175Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognition blindness and cognitive gadgets

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 September 2019

Cecilia Heyes*
Affiliation:
All Souls College and Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 4AL, United Kingdom. [email protected]/~ascch/

Abstract

Responding to commentaries from psychologists, neuroscientists, philosophers, and anthropologists, I clarify a central purpose of Cognitive Gadgets – to overcome “cognition blindness” in research on human evolution. I defend this purpose against Brunerian, extended mind, and niche construction critiques of computationalism – that is, views prioritising meaning over information, or asserting that behaviour and objects can be intrinsic parts of a thinking process. I argue that empirical evidence from cognitive science is needed to locate distinctively human cognitive mechanisms on the continuum between gadgets and instincts. Focussing on that requirement, I also address specific challenges, and applaud extensions and refinements, of the evidence surveyed in my book. It has been said that “a writer's idea of sound criticism is ten thousand words of closely reasoned adulation.” I cannot disagree with this untraceable wag, but the 30 commentators on Cognitive Gadgets provided some 30,000 words of criticism that are of much greater scientific value than adulation. I am grateful to them all. The response that follows is V-shaped. It starts with the broadest conceptual and methodological issues and funnels down to matters arising from specific empirical studies.

Type
Author's Response
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Beisert, M., Zmyj, N., Liepelt, R., Jung, F., Prinz, W. & Daum, M. M. (2012) Rethinking ‘rational imitation’ in 14-month-old infants: A perceptual distraction approach. PLoS One 7(3):e32563.Google Scholar
Birch, J. (2017) The philosophy of social evolution. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Block, N. (1995) The mind as the software of the brain. In: Thinking, ed. Smith, E. E. & Osherson, D. N., pp. 377425. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1990) Acts of meaning. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Buiatti, M., Di Giorgio, E., Piazza, M., Polloni, C., Menna, G., Taddei, F., Baldo, E., and Vallortigara, G. (2019). Cortical route for facelike pattern processing in human newborns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 116(10):4625–30. Available at: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/?term=buiatti+vallortigara.Google Scholar
Buskell, A. (2018) Causes of cultural disparity: Switches, tuners, and the cognitive science of religion. Philosophical Psychology 31(8):1239–64.Google Scholar
Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2007) Enculturated chimpanzees imitate rationally. Developmental Science 10(4):3138.Google Scholar
Catmur, C. & Heyes, C. M. (2013) Is it what you do, or when you do it? The roles of contingency and similarity in pro-social effects of imitation. Cognitive Science 37(8):1541–52.Google Scholar
Clark, A. & Chalmers, D. (1998) The extended mind. Analysis 58(1):719.Google Scholar
Coltheart, M. (2002) Cognitive neuropsychology. In: Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology, ed. Wixted, J., pp. 139174. Wiley.Google Scholar
Coltheart, M. (2012) The cognitive level of explanation. Australian Journal of Psychology 64(1):1118.Google Scholar
Cook, R., Bird, G., Catmur, C., Press, C. & Heyes, C. (2014) Mirror neurons: From origin to function. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 37(2):177–92.Google Scholar
de Klerk, C. C., Lamy-Yang, I., & Southgate, V. (2018) The role of sensorimotor experience in the development of mimicry in infancy. Developmental Science, e12771. Available at: DOI: 10.1111/desc.12771.Google Scholar
Del Giudice, M., Manera, V. & Keysers, C. (2009) Programmed to learn? The ontogeny of mirror neurons. Developmental Science 12(2):350–63.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (2017) From bacteria to Bach and back: The evolution of minds. W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Feldman, M. W., Odling-Smee, J. & Laland, K. N. (2017) Why Gupta et al.’s critique of niche construction theory is off target. Journal of Genetics 96(3):505–08.Google Scholar
Feldman, M. W. & Ramachandran, S. (2018) Missing compared to what? Revisiting heritability, genes and culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 373(1743):20170064.Google Scholar
Gupta, M., Prasad, N. G., Dey, S., Joshi, A. & Vidya, T. N. C. (2017). Niche construction in evolutionary theory: The construction of an academic niche? Journal of Genetics 96(3):491504.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. (2018) Cognitive gadgets: The cultural evolution of thinking. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (2013) What can imitation do for cooperation? In: Cooperation and its evolution, ed. Sterelny, K., Joyce, R., Calcott, B. & Fraser, B., pp. 313–31. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (2016b) Born pupils? Natural pedagogy and cultural pedagogy. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(2), 280295.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (2016c) Homo imitans? Seven reasons why imitation couldn't possibly be associative. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1636),20150069.Google Scholar
Hughes, C., Jaffee, S. R., Happé, F., Taylor, A., Caspi, A. & Moffitt, T. E. (2005) Origins of individual differences in theory of mind: From nature to nurture? Child Development 76(2):356–70.Google Scholar
Hutto, D. D. (2007) The narrative practice hypothesis: Origins and applications of folk psychology. Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplement 60:4368. doi: 10.1017/S1358246107000033.Google Scholar
Hutto, D. D. & Myin, E. (2017) Evolving enactivism: Basic minds meet content. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Malafouris, L. (2016) On human becoming and incompleteness: A material engagement approach to the study of embodiment in evolution and culture. In: Embodiment in evolution and culture, ed. Etzelmüller, G. & Tewes, C., pp. 289305. Mohr Siebeck.Google Scholar
McEwen, F., Happé, F., Bolton, P., Rijsdijk, F., Ronald, A., Dworzynski, K. & Plomin, R. (2007) Origins of individual differences in imitation: Links with language, pretend play, and socially insightful behavior in two-year-old twins. Child Development 78(2):474–92.Google Scholar
Shallice, T. & Cooper, R. (2011) The organisation of mind. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shea, N., Boldt, A., Bang, D., Yeung, N., Heyes, C. M. & Frith, C. D. (2014) Supra-personal cognitive control and metacognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(4), 186193.Google Scholar
Stout, D., & Hecht, E. E. (2017) Evolutionary neuroscience of cumulative culture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114(30)7861–68.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
(2007/1549) The book of common prayer, p. 484. Church Publishing Incorporated.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2014) A natural history of human thinking. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2019) Becoming human: A theory of ontogeny. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar