Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T14:09:39.111Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Capacities for peace, and war, are old and related to Homo construction of worlds and communities

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 January 2024

Agustín Fuentes*
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, USA [email protected] https://anthropology.princeton.edu/people/faculty/agustin-fuentes
Nam Kim
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, USA [email protected] https://www.anthropology.wisc.edu/staff/kim-nam-c/
Marc Kissel
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, Appalachian State University, Boone, NC, USA [email protected] https://anthro.appstate.edu/directory/dr-marc-kissel
*
*Corresponding author.

Abstract

The capacities required for both peace and war predate 100,000 years ago in the genus Homo are deeply entangled in the modes by which humans physically and perceptually construct their worlds and communities, and may not be sufficiently captured by economic models.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

This is a robust contribution to the literature on the evolution of warfare and peacemaking. We found many of the arguments to be compelling and agree with much that Glowacki presents. Here we extend and contextualize the argument Glowacki offers regarding evolutionary dynamics, commenting on the extent and complexity of cultural dynamics in the genus Homo as it relates to the emergence of peacemaking. We suggest a broader engagement with the range of Pleistocene data offers a deeper time frame and a bit more nuance.

Glowacki argues that peace is the product of cultural technologies that depend on factors that have recently emerged in our species' history, including social institutions and cultural mechanisms for preventing and resolving conflicts. We agree with the core argument and have published on it and related themes (Fuentes, Reference Fuentes2017a; Kim & Kissel, Reference Kim and Kissel2018; Kissel & Kim, Reference Kissel and Kim2019). However, Glowacki implies that this suite of capacities to invent peace developed primarily within the last 100,000 years or so, drawing on, but different from, preexisting patterns of cooperation and conflict. This falls dangerously close to the refuted “behavioral modernity” versus “anatomical modernity” position (Kissel & Fuentes, Reference Kissel and Fuentes2021). We argue that the behavioral abilities that permitted the potential presence of warfare existed before 100,000 years ago and would also have allowed for the development of “peacemaking” (or “peacefare”; Kim & Kissel, Reference Kim and Kissel2018) as well. If members of the genus Homo could organize and cooperate in highly sophisticated ways, for example, hunting, material exchanges, cultural diffusion across large distances, complex care of injured and deceased, and other communal/joint activities (Brooks et al., Reference Brooks, Yellen, Potts, Behrensmeyer, Deino, Leslie and Clark2018; Dapschauskas, Göden, Sommer, & Kandel, Reference Dapschauskas, Göden, Sommer and Kandel2022; Hrdy & Burkart, Reference Hrdy and Burkart2020; MacDonald, Scherjona, van Veena, Vaesena, & Roebroeks, Reference MacDonald, Scherjona, van Veena, Vaesena and Roebroeks2021; Spikins, Dytham, French, & Seren, Reference Spikins, Dytham, French and Seren2021), and could use sophisticated communication and intergroup coordination in those endeavors, then populations of Homo could have started to develop and deploy capacities for peacefare well before 100,000 years ago. Glowacki acknowledges that underlying elements in these general patterns existed by ~300,000 years ago, but argues the key forms of social structure and cultural institutions of this earlier period did not resemble, sufficiently, the kinds he describes for later eras. Our view is that members of the genus Homo possessed the sufficient cognitive/physical/social toolkits, whether yesterday, 100,000 years ago, or 300,000 years, to develop the forms of cultural institutions necessary for peacefare (Fuentes, Reference Fuentes2017b; Kim & Kissel, Reference Kim and Kissel2018).

In relation to the complexities in intergroup dynamics that solidify in the last few hundred millennia of the Pleistocene, Glowacki writes that intergroup exchange in particular allowed humans to build the cultural technologies beneficial in meeting the challenges of the ecological and social environments. Citing Polly Wiessner, Glowacki writes that more recent periods of peace may have fueled increased social complexity due to an expansion of exchange between groups that would otherwise be in conflict. Clearly this is possible. We would add here that periods of conflict, with constructions of identity/solidarity against outside groups, cooperation for defense as well as policing, and sacrificial ceremonies, may have also fueled substantive changes in social and technological complexity. These dynamics may also predate 100,000 years ago. One of us has argued previously how violent competition between factions/groups, both intra- or intersocietal, is a key factor for development of sociopolitical complexity and innovations in cultural institutions (Kim, Reference Kim, Bondarenko, Kowalewski and Small2020).

Glowacki suggests that for contemporary small-scale societies, participation is risky and conflict is motivated by a range of private incentives. We think much more can be said about motivations for populations of the genus Homo participating in coalitionary cooperation and conflict, particularly if they are to result in “positive-sum outcomes,” which themselves would be highly complex and culturally contingent. Equally significant would have been cultural attitudes and perceptions about why violence is needed or ought to be actively avoided or restricted. This is, as partially noted by Glowacki, something that distinguishes human warfare and peacefare, even at small scales, from other kinds of coalitionary violence in other species (Kim & Kissel, Reference Kim and Kissel2018). Sometimes people participate because of a perceived attack or threat, and these perceptions could be related to beliefs that need not be physically manifest (Fuentes, Reference Fuentes2019; Whitehead, Reference Whitehead and Whitehead2004).

It is possible that Glowacki's focus on the core elements of game theory, economic models, and rational choice behaviors/incentives tends to elide much in actual motivations and belief systems, emphasizing instead the “rational” actor assessment. For humans, warfare and peacefare can be motivated by a perceived, and believed, collective good for the home community, even one that is not quantifiable. Arguably, participation in active warfare and peacefare can stem from a myriad of complex reasons and shared beliefs, much of which are simultaneously basis for, and derived from, cultural institutions and perceptions.

Glowacki's approach of creating positive-sum outcomes may not fully capture the variable nature of experiences in gains and losses for groups across the middle and terminal Pleistocene. Humans participate in warfare and peacefare for different reasons and in different contexts, many of which are not assessable primarily in an economic model, and these experiences may not be capable of being “summed.” We might perceive positive–positive outcomes on the aggregate, but peace as a lived dynamic is highly variable and subjective, and is not an absolute condition (it may be marked by perceived conditions of peace for some but losses/injustices, unfair treatment for others). A generally positive-sum outcome may run the risk of overlooking negative conditions for some within a society. Peace for some might come at a cost for others. For instance, some might claim that American society today is generally at peace, but many community members might feel very differently (and rightfully so) given various kinds of conflict, injustice, and an absence of peace as related to outcomes from forms of cultural or structural violence (Kim, Reference Kim2012).

In sum, we applaud Glowacki's exploration of the conditions required for peace and war and welcome more discussions on how these are not opposites but epiphenomenal of deeper issues of how humans construct their worlds and communities.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Competing interest

None.

References

Brooks, A. S., Yellen, J., Potts, R., Behrensmeyer, A. L., Deino, A. L., Leslie, D. E., … Clark, J. B. (2018). Long-distance stone transport and pigment use in the earliest Middle Stone Age. Science (New York, N.Y.), 360(6384), 9094.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dapschauskas, R., Göden, M. B., Sommer, C., & Kandel, A. W. (2022). The emergence of habitual ochre use in Africa and its significance for the development of ritual behavior during the Middle Stone Age. Journal of World Prehistory, 35, 233319. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10963-022-09170-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuentes, A. (2017a). Human niche, human behaviour, human nature. Interface Focus, 7, 20160136. http://doi.org/10.1098/rsfs.2016.0136CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuentes, A. (2017b). The creative spark: How imagination made humans exceptional Dutton/Penguin. Dutton Books/Penguin Press.Google Scholar
Fuentes, A. (2019). Why we believe: Evolution and the human way of being. Yale University Press/Templeton Press.Google Scholar
Hrdy, S. B., & Burkart, J. M. (2020). The emergence of emotionally modern humans: Implications for language and learning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 375(1803), 20190499. http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0499CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, N. C. (2012). Angels, illusions, hydras, and chimeras: Violence and humanity. Reviews in Anthropology, 41(4), 239272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, N. C. (2020). A pathway to emergent social complexity and state power: A view from southeast Asia. In Bondarenko, D. M., Kowalewski, S., & Small, D. (Eds.), The evolution of social institutions (pp. 225253). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, N. C., & Kissel, M. (2018). Emergent warfare in our evolutionary past. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kissel, M., & Fuentes, A. (2021). The ripples of modernity: How we can extend paleoanthropology with the extended evolutionary synthesis. Evolutionary Anthropology, 30(1), 8498. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1002/evan.21883CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kissel, M., & Kim, N. C. (2019). The emergence of human warfare: Current perspectives. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 168, 141163. http://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.23751CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacDonald, K., Scherjona, F., van Veena, E., Vaesena, K., & Roebroeks, W. (2021). Middle Pleistocene fire use: The first signal of widespread cultural diffusion in human evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 118(31), e2101108118. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2101108118CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spikins, P., Dytham, C., French, J., & Seren, J.-W. (2021). Theoretical and methodological approaches to ecological changes, social behaviour and human intergroup tolerance 300,000 to 30,000 BP. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 28, 5375. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10816-020-09503-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitehead, N. L. (2004). On the poetics of violence. In Whitehead, N. L. (Ed.), Violence (pp. 5577). School of American Research Press.Google Scholar