Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T21:11:47.982Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Beyond neonatal imitation: Aerodigestive stereotypies, speech development, and social interaction in the extended perinatal period

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 December 2017

Nazim Keven
Affiliation:
Department of Philosophy, Bilkent University, TR-06800 Bilkent, Ankara, Turkey. [email protected]://sci-phi.com/
Kathleen A. Akins
Affiliation:
Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC V5A 1S6, Canada. [email protected]://www.sfu.ca/~kathleea/

Abstract

In our target article, we argued that the positive results of neonatal imitation are likely to be by-products of normal aerodigestive development. Our hypothesis elicited various responses on the role of social interaction in infancy, the methodological issues about imitation experiments, and the relation between the aerodigestive theory and the development of speech. Here we respond to the commentaries.

Type
Authors' Response
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abravanel, E. & Sigafoos, A. D. (1984) Exploring the presence of imitation during early infancy. Child Development 55(2):381–92.Google Scholar
Anisfeld, M. (1996) Only tongue protrusion modeling is matched by neonates. Developmental Review 16(2):149–61. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1006/drev.1996.0006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bahrick, L. E., Lickliter, R. & Flom, R. (2004) Intersensory redundancy guides the development of selective attention, perception, and cognition in infancy. Current Directions in Psychological Science 13(3):99102. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00283.x.Google Scholar
Bartrip, J., Morton, J. & Schonen, S. (2001) Responses to mother's face in 3-week to 5-month-old infants. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 19(2):219–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertin, E. & Striano, T. (2006) The still-face response in newborn, 1.5-, and 3-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development 29(2):294–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bigelow, A. E. & Power, M. (2012) The effect of mother-infant skin-to-skin contact on infants' response to the Still Face Task from newborn to three months of age. Infant Behavior and Development 35(2):240–51. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.12.008.Google Scholar
Borjon, J. I., Takahashi, D. Y., Cervantes, D. C. & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2016) Arousal dynamics drive vocal production in marmoset monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology 116(2):753–64. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00136.2016.Google Scholar
Cecchini, M., Baroni, E., Di Vito, C., Piccolo, F. & Lai, C. (2011) Newborn preference for a new face versus a previously seen communicative or motionless face. Infant Behavior and Development 34(3):424–33.Google Scholar
Chandrasekaran, C., Lemus, L., Trubanova, A., Gondan, M. & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2011) Monkeys and humans share a common computation for face/voice integration. PLoS Computational Biology 7(9):e1002165.Google Scholar
Chandrasekaran, C., Trubanova, A., Stillittano, S., Caplier, A. & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2009) The natural statistics of audiovisual speech. PLoS Computational Biology 5(7):e1000436. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000436.Google Scholar
Choi, J. Y., Takahashi, D. Y. & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2015) Cooperative vocal control in marmoset monkeys via vocal feedback. Journal of Neurophysiology 114(1):274–83. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00228.2015.Google Scholar
Dai, S.-Y. & Hata, T. (2006) Four-dimensional sonographic assessment of fetal facial expression early in the third trimester. Gynecology and Obstetrics 94(2):108–13. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijgo.2006.05.004.Google Scholar
Fagan, M. K. (2014) Frequency of vocalization before and after cochlear implantation: Dynamic effect of auditory feedback on infant behavior. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 126:328–38. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.05.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fontaine, R. (1984) Imitative skills between birth and six months. Infant Behavior and Development 7(3):323–33.Google Scholar
Ghazanfar, A. A., Chandrasekaran, C. & Logothetis, N. K. (2008) Interactions between the superior temporal sulcus and auditory cortex mediate dynamic face/voice integration in rhesus monkeys. The Journal of Neuroscience 28(17):4457–469. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0541-08.2008.Google Scholar
Ghazanfar, A. A., Takahashi, D. Y., Mathur, N. & Fitch, W. T. (2012) Cineradiography of monkey lip-smacking reveals putative precursors of speech dynamics. Current Biology 22(13):1176–182. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.04.055.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghazanfar, A. A. & Zhang, Y. S. (2016) The autonomic nervous system is the engine for vocal development through social feedback. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 40:155–60. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2016.07.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ham, J. & Tronick, E. D. (2006) Infant resilience to the stress of the still-face: Infant and maternal psychophysiology are related. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1094(1):297302. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1376.038.Google Scholar
Heimann, M., Nelson, K. E. & Schaller, J. (1989) Neonatal imitation of tongue protrusion and mouth opening: Methodological aspects and evidence of early individual differences. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 30(2):90101.Google Scholar
Jacobson, S. W. (1979) Matching behavior in the young infant. Child Development 50(2):425–30. Available at: https://doi.org/10.2307/1129418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, M. H., Senju, A. & Tomalski, P. (2015) The two-process theory of face processing: Modifications based on two decades of data from infants and adults. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 50:169–79. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaas, J. H. (2015) Blindsight: Post-natal potential of a transient pulvinar pathway. Current Biology 25(4):R155–57. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.12.053.Google Scholar
Kanenishi, K., Hanaoka, U., Noguchi, J., Marumo, G. & Hata, T. (2013) 4D ultrasound evaluation of fetal facial expressions during the latter stages of the second trimester. International Journal of Gynaecology and Obstetrics: The Official Organ of the International Federation of Gynaecology and Obstetrics 121(3):257–60. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijgo.2013.01.018.Google Scholar
Kugiumutzakis, G. (1999) Genesis and development of early infant mimesis to facial and vocal models. In: Imitation in infancy, ed. Nadel, L. & Butterworth, G., pp. 3659. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K. & Meltzoff, A. N. (1984) The intermodal representation of speech in infants. Infant Behavior and Development 7(3):361–81. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(84)80050-8.Google Scholar
Kurjak, A., Azumendi, G., Andonotopo, W. & Salihagic-Kadic, A. (2007) Three- and four-dimensional ultrasonography for the structural and functional evaluation of the fetal face. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology 196(1):1628. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajog.2006.06.090.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewkowicz, D. J. (2014) Early experience and multisensory perceptual narrowing. Developmental Psychobiology 56(2):292315. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21197.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. (2007) ‘Like Me’: A foundation for social cognition. Developmental Science 10(1):126–34. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00574.x.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. & Moore, M. K. (1983) Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development 54(3):702–9. Available at: https://doi.org/10.2307/1130058.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. & Moore, M. K. (1989) Imitation in newborn infants: Exploring the range of gestures imitated and the underlying mechanisms. Developmental Psychology 25(6):954–62.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. & Moore, M. K. (1992) Early imitation within a functional framework: The importance of person identity, movement, and development. Infant Behavior and Development 15(4):479505.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. & Moore, M. K. (1994) Imitation, memory, and the representation of persons. Infant Behavior and Development 17:8399. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/0163638394900248.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. & Moore, M. K. (2002) Imitation, memory, and the representation of persons. Infant Behavior and Development 25(1):3961.Google Scholar
Moore, G. A. & Calkins, S. D. (2004) Infants' vagal regulation in the still-face paradigm is related to dyadic coordination of mother-infant interaction. Developmental Psychology 40(6):1068.Google Scholar
Morton, J. & Johnson, M. H. (1991) CONSPEC and CONLERN: A two-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review 98(2):164. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oostenbroek, J., Slaughter, V., Nielsen, M. & Suddendorf, T. (2013) Why the confusion around neonatal imitation? A review. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology 31(4):328–41.Google Scholar
Oostenbroek, J., Suddendorf, T., Nielsen, M., Redshaw, J., Kennedy-Costantini, S., Davis, J., Clark, S. & Slaughter, V. (2016) Comprehensive longitudinal study challenges the existence of neonatal imitation in humans. Current Biology 26(10):1334–38. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.03.047.Google Scholar
Patterson, M. L. & Werker, J. F. (2003) Two-month-old infants match phonetic information in lips and voice. Developmental Science 6(2):191–96. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00271.Google Scholar
Reissland, N., Francis, B. & Mason, J. (2012) Development of fetal yawn compared with non-yawn mouth openings from 24–36 weeks gestation. PLoS ONE 7(11):e50569. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0050569.Google Scholar
Reissland, N., Francis, B. & Mason, J. (2013) Can healthy fetuses show facial expressions of “pain” or “distress”? PLoS ONE 8(6):e65530. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0065530.Google Scholar
Reissland, N., Francis, B., Mason, J. & Lincoln, K. (2011) Do facial expressions develop before birth? PLoS ONE 6(8):e24081. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0024081.Google Scholar
Sato, M., Kanenishi, K., Hanaoka, U., Noguchi, J., Marumo, G. & Hata, T. (2014) 4D ultrasound study of fetal facial expressions at 20–24 weeks of gestation. International Journal of Gynaecology & Obstetrics 126(3):275–79. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijgo.2014.03.036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shepherd, S. V., Lanzilotto, M. & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2012) Facial muscle coordination in monkeys during rhythmic facial expressions and ingestive movements. Journal of Neuroscience 32(18):6105–116. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6136-11.2012.Google Scholar
Simpson, E. A., Murray, L., Paukner, A. & Ferrari, P. F. (2014a) The mirror neuron system as revealed through neonatal imitation: Presence from birth, predictive power and evidence of plasticity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 369(1644):20130289. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0289.Google Scholar
Striano, T. (2004) Direction of regard and the still-face effect in the first year: Does intention matter? Child Development 75(2):468–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thelen, E. (1981b) Rhythmical behavior in infancy: An ethological perspective. Developmental Psychology 17(3):237–57. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.17.3.237.Google Scholar
Volpe, J. (2008) Neurology of the newborn infant, 5th ed. Saunders/Elsevier.Google Scholar
Warner, C. E., Kwan, W. C. & Bourne, J. A. (2012) The early maturation of visual cortical area MT is dependent on input from the retinorecipient medial portion of the inferior pulvinar. Journal of Neuroscience 32(48):17073–7085.Google Scholar
Yigiter, A. B. & Kavak, Z. N. (2006) Normal standards of fetal behavior assessed by four-dimensional sonography. Journal of Maternal-Fetal Neonatal Medicine 19(11):707–21. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1080/14767050600924129.Google Scholar