Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T03:22:20.506Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Thinking through other minds: A variational approach to cognition and culture

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 May 2019

Samuel P. L. Veissière
Affiliation:
Division of Social and Transcultural Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, CanadaH3A [email protected]@[email protected] Culture, Mind, and Brain Program, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, CanadaH3A 1A1 Department of Anthropology, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, CanadaH3A 2T7
Axel Constant
Affiliation:
Culture, Mind, and Brain Program, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, CanadaH3A 1A1 Charles Perkins Centre, The University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, [email protected] Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, University College London, LondonWC1N 3AR, UK. [email protected]
Maxwell J. D. Ramstead
Affiliation:
Division of Social and Transcultural Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, CanadaH3A [email protected]@[email protected] Culture, Mind, and Brain Program, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, CanadaH3A 1A1 Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, University College London, LondonWC1N 3AR, UK. [email protected]
Karl J. Friston
Affiliation:
Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, University College London, LondonWC1N 3AR, UK. [email protected]
Laurence J. Kirmayer
Affiliation:
Division of Social and Transcultural Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, CanadaH3A [email protected]@[email protected] Culture, Mind, and Brain Program, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, CanadaH3A 1A1 Department of Anthropology, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, CanadaH3A 2T7

Abstract

The processes underwriting the acquisition of culture remain unclear. How are shared habits, norms, and expectations learned and maintained with precision and reliability across large-scale sociocultural ensembles? Is there a unifying account of the mechanisms involved in the acquisition of culture? Notions such as “shared expectations,” the “selective patterning of attention and behaviour,” “cultural evolution,” “cultural inheritance,” and “implicit learning” are the main candidates to underpin a unifying account of cognition and the acquisition of culture; however, their interactions require greater specification and clarification. In this article, we integrate these candidates using the variational (free-energy) approach to human cognition and culture in theoretical neuroscience. We describe the construction by humans of social niches that afford epistemic resources called cultural affordances. We argue that human agents learn the shared habits, norms, and expectations of their culture through immersive participation in patterned cultural practices that selectively pattern attention and behaviour. We call this process “thinking through other minds” (TTOM) – in effect, the process of inferring other agents’ expectations about the world and how to behave in social context. We argue that for humans, information from and about other people's expectations constitutes the primary domain of statistical regularities that humans leverage to predict and organize behaviour. The integrative model we offer has implications that can advance theories of cognition, enculturation, adaptation, and psychopathology. Crucially, this formal (variational) treatment seeks to resolve key debates in current cognitive science, such as the distinction between internalist and externalist accounts of theory of mind abilities and the more fundamental distinction between dynamical and representational accounts of enactivism.

Type
Target Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2019. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, R. A., Huys., Q. J. M. & Roiser, J. P. (2016) Computational psychiatry: Towards a mathematically informed understanding of mental illness. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 87(1):5363.Google ScholarPubMed
Andrews, P. W., Gangestad, S. W. & Matthews, D. (2002) Adaptationism–how to carry out an exaptationist program. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25(4): 489504, discussion 504–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Apperly, I. A. & Butterfill, S. A. (2009) Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological Review 116(4):953–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asch, S. E. (1956) Studies of independence and conformity: I. A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied 70(9):170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Astuti, R. & Bloch, M. (2015) The causal cognition of wrong doing: Incest, intentionality, and morality. Frontiers in Psychology 6:136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Badcock, P. B. (2012) Evolutionary systems theory: A unifying meta-theory of psychological science. Review of General Psychology 16(1):1023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Badcock, P. B., Davey, C. G., Whittle, S., Allen, N. B. & Friston, K. J. (2017) The depressed brain: An evolutionary systems theory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 21(3):182–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Badcock, P. B., Friston, K. J. & Ramstead, M. J. (2019) The hierarchically mechanistic mind: A free-energy formulation of the human psyche. Physics of Life Reviews 31:104–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, M. W. (1992) Relational schemas and the processing of social information. Psychological Bulletin 112(3):461–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bengio, Y. (2014) Evolving culture versus local minima. In: Growing adaptive machines: Combining development and learning in artificial neural networks, eds. Kowaliw, T., Bredeche, N. & Doursat, R., pp. 109138. Studies in Computational Intelligence. Springer.Google Scholar
Bertolotti, T. & Magnani, L. (2017) Theoretical considerations on cognitive niche construction. Synthese 194(12):4757–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berwick, R. C., Chomsky, N. & Piattelli-Palmarini, M. (2013) Poverty of the stimulus stands: Why recent challenges fail. In: Rich Languages from Poor Inputs, eds. Piattelli-Palmarini, M. & Berwick, R. C., pp. 1942. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bijleveld, E., Scheepers, D. & Ellemers, N. (2012) The cortisol response to anticipated intergroup interactions predicts self-reported prejudice. PLoS ONE 7(3):e33681.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, P. (2005) Descartes’ baby: How the science of child development explains what makes us human. Random House.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2017) Against empathy: The case for rational compassion. Random House.Google Scholar
Bolis, D., Balsters, J., Wenderoth, N., Becchio, C. & Schilbach, L. (2017) Beyond autism: Introducing the dialectical misattunement hypothesis and a Bayesian account of intersubjectivity. Psychopathology 50(6):355–72. https://doi.org/10.1159/000484353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolis, D. & Schilbach, L. (2018a) Observing and participating in social interactions: Action perception and action control across the autistic spectrum. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 29:168–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.01.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bourdieu, P. (1977) Equisse D'une Théorie de La Pratique. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bourdieu, P. (1984) Distinction: A social critique of the judgement of taste. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. (2005) The origin and evolution of cultures. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (2018) Minds make societies: How cognition explains the world humans create. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Brown, D. E. (2004) Human universals, human nature & human culture. Daedalus 133(4):4754. https://doi.org/10.1162/0011526042365645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruineberg, J. & Rietveld, E. (2014) Self-organization, free energy minimization, and optimal grip on a field of affordances. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8:599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruineberg, J., Rietveld, E., Parr, T., van Maanen, L. & Friston, K. J. (2018b) Free-energy minimization in joint agent-environment systems: A niche construction perspective. Journal of Theoretical Biology 455:161–78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2018.07.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burkart, J. M., Hrdy, S. B. & Van Schaik, C. P. (2009) Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews 18(5):175–86.Google Scholar
Carruthers, P. & Smith, P. K. (1996) Theories of theories of mind. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chemero, A. (2009) Radical embodied cognitive science. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., Foulsham, T., Kingstone, A. & Henrich, J. (2013) Two ways to the top: Evidence that dominance and prestige are distinct yet viable avenues to social rank and influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 104(1):103–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chomsky, N. (1996) Studies on semantics in generative grammar. Walter de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, W. & Michael, J. (2016) From two systems to a multi-systems architecture for mindreading. New Ideas in Psychology 40:4864.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chudek, M., McNamara, R., Burch, S., Bloom, P. & Henrich, J. (2013) Developmental and cross-cultural evidence for intuitive dualism. Unpublished manuscript, University of British Columbia.Google Scholar
Cialdini, R. B. & Goldstein, N. J. (2004) Social influence: Compliance and conformity. Annual Review of Psychology 55:591621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, A. (2006) Language, embodiment, and the cognitive niche. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10(8):370–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, A. (2008) Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A. (2013a) Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36(3):181204. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12000477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A. & Chalmers, D. (1998) The extended mind. Analysis 58(1):719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, K. B. (1988) Prejudice and your child. Wesleyan University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, K. B. & Clark, M. K. (1939) The development of consciousness of self and the emergence of racial identification in Negro preschool children. Journal of Social Psychology 10(4):591–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Constant, A., Bervoets, J., Hens, K. & Van de Cruys, S. (2018a) Precise worlds for certain minds: An ecological perspective on the relational self in autism. Topoi 112. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-018-9546-4.Google Scholar
Constant, A., Ramstead, M. J. D., Veissière, S. P. L., Campbell, J. O. & Friston, K. (2018b) A variational approach to niche construction. Journal of the Royal Society Interface 15(141):20170685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Constant, A., Ramstead, M. J. D., Veissière, S. P. L. & Friston, K. J. (2019b) Regimes of expectations: An active inference model of social conformity and decision making. Frontiers in Psychology 10:679. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00679.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coopersmith, J. (2017) The lazy universe: An introduction to the principle of least action. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Csibra, G. & Gergely, G. (2009) Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13(4):148–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Csibra, G. & Gergely, G. (2011) Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B: Biological Sciences 366(1567):1149–57. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0319.Google Scholar
Cullen, M., Davey, B., Friston, K. J. & Moran, R. J. (2018) Active inference in OpenAI Gym: A paradigm for computational investigations into psychiatric illness. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging 3(9):809–18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.06.010.Google ScholarPubMed
De Castro, E. V. (2009) Métaphysiques Cannibales: Lignes D'anthropologie Post-Structurale. Presses universitaires de France.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S. & Cohen, L. (2007) Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron 56(2):384–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunbar, R. I. M. (2003) The social brain: Mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology 32(1):163–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (2004) Gossip in evolutionary perspective. Review of General Psychology 8(2):100–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duranti, A. (2015) The anthropology of intentions. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durkheim, E. (1985/2014) The rules of sociological method: And selected texts on sociology and its method. Simon and Schuster. (Original work published in 1985).Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S. (2013) Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Taylor & Francis. https://content.taylorfrancis.com/books/download?dac=C2009-0-07336-6&isbn=9781317710332&format=googlePreviewPdf.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Einarsson, A. & Ziemke, T. (2017) Exploring the multi-layered affordances of composing and performing interactive music with responsive technologies. Frontiers in Psychology 8:1701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabry, R. E. (2018) Betwixt and between: The enculturated predictive processing approach to cognition. Synthese 195:2483–518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feinman, S. (1982) Social referencing in infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 28:445–70. https://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/23086154.pdf.Google Scholar
Feldman, H. & Friston, K. J. (2010) Attention, uncertainty, and free-energy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 4:215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, J. (2013) Tuning your priors to the world. Topics in Cognitive Science 5(1):1334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, K. (2005) A theory of cortical responses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 360(1456):815–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, K. J. (2010) The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11(2):127–38. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2787.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, K. (2011) Embodied inference: Or “I think therefore I am, if I am what I think.” In: Implications of Embodiment: Cognition and Communication, eds. Tschacher, W. & Bergomi, C., pp. 89125.Google Scholar
Friston, K. (2013) Life as we know it. Journal of the Royal Society Interface 10(86):20130475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friston, K. J., FitzGerald, T., Rigoli, F., Schwartenbeck, P., O'Doherty, J. & Pezzulo, G. (2016) Active inference and learning. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 68:862–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friston, K., FitzGerald, T., Rigoli, F., Schwartenbeck, P. & Pezzulo, G. (2017a) Active inference: A process theory. Neural Computation 29(1):149. https://doi.org/10.1162/NECO_a_00912.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friston, K. J. & Frith, C. D. (2015b) Active inference, communication and hermeneutics. Cortex 68:129–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.03.025.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friston, K. J., Kilner, J. & Harrison, L. (2006) A free energy principle for the brain. Journal of Physiology, Paris 100(1–3):7087.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, K. J., Lin, M., Frith, C. D., Pezzulo, G., Hobson, J. A. & Ondobaka, S. (2017b) Active inference, curiosity and insight. Neural Computation 29(10):2633–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friston, K., Rigoli, F., Ognibene, D., Mathys, C., Fitzgerald, T. & Pezzulo, G. (2015) Active inference and epistemic value. Cognitive Neuroscience 6(4):187214. https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2015.1020053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, K. J., Schwartenbeck, P., FitzGerald, T., Moutoussis, M., Behrens, T. & Dolan, R. J. (2014a) The anatomy of choice: Dopamine and decision-making. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 369(1655):20130481. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friston, K. J. & Stephan, K. E. (2007) Free-energy and the brain. Synthese 159(3):417–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, K. J., Stephan, K. E., Montague, R. & Dolan, R. J. (2014b) Computational psychiatry: The brain as a phantastic organ. Lancet Psychiatry 1(2):148–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friston, K., Thornton, C. & Clark, A. (2012a) Free-energy minimization and the dark-room problem. Frontiers in Psychology 3:130. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fulda, F. C. (2017) Natural agency: The case of bacterial cognition. Journal of the American Philosophical Association 3(1):6990.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, S. (2017) Enactivist interventions: Rethinking the mind. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, S. & Allen, M. (2018) Active inference, enactivism and the hermeneutics of social cognition. Synthese 195(6):2627–48. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-016-1269-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallese, V. & Goldman, A. (1998) Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2(12):493501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gavrilets, S. & Vose, A. (2006) The dynamics of Machiavellian intelligence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103(45):16823–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geertz, C. (1973) The interpretation of culture. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Goffman, E. (2009) Relations in public. Transaction.Google Scholar
Gold, J. & Gold, I. (2015) Suspicious minds: How culture shapes madness. Free Press.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. I. (2006) Simulating minds: The philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience of mindreading. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldstein, J., Davidoff, J. & Roberson, D. (2009) Knowing color terms enhances recognition: Further evidence from English and Himba. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 102(2):219–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gopnik, A. & Wellman, H. M. (2012) Reconstructing constructivism: Causal models, Bayesian learning mechanisms, and the theory theory. Psychological Bulletin 138(6):10851108. doi:10.1037/a0028044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hacking, I. (1998) Mad travelers: Reflections on the reality of transient mental illnesses. University of Virginia Press.Google Scholar
Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2008) Emulation and mimicry for social interaction: A theoretical approach to imitation in autism. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 61(1):101–15. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210701508798.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heine, S. J., Takemoto, T., Moskalenko, S., Lasaleta, J. & Henrich, J. (2008) Mirrors in the head: Cultural variation in objective self-awareness. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin 34(7):879–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J. (2015) The secret of our success: How culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J. & Gil-White, F. J. (2001) The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior 22(3):165–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hewlett, B. S. (1994) Intimate fathers: The nature and context of Aka pygmy paternal infant care. University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Hewlett, B. S. (2017) Hunter-gatherer childhoods: Evolutionary, developmental, and cultural perspectives. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heyes, C. (2018b) Cognitive gadgets: The cultural evolution of thinking. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heyes, C. M. & Frith, C. D. (2014) The cultural evolution of mind reading. Science 344(6190):1243091.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hohwy, J. (2013) The predictive mind. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howes, D. (2011) Reply to Tim Ingold. Social Anthropology 19(3):318–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (2011) Mothers and others. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Huneman, P. & Machery, E. (2015) Evolutionary psychology: Issues, results, debates. In: Handbook of evolutionary thinking in the sciences, eds. Heams, T., Huneman, P., Lecointre, G. & Silberstein, M., pp. 647–57. Springer.Google Scholar
Hutto, D. & Myin, E. (2013) Radical enactivism: Basic minds without content. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hutto, D. & Myin, E. (2017) Evolving enactivism: Basic minds meet content. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutto, D. & Satne, G. (2015) The natural origins of content. Philosophia 43(3):521–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutto, D. D. (2012) Folk psychological narratives: The sociocultural basis of understanding reasons. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hutto, D. D., Kirchhoff, M. D. & Myin, E. (2014) Extensive enactivism: Why keep it all in? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8:706.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huys, Q. J. M., Maia, T. V. & Frank, M. J. (2016) Computational psychiatry as a bridge from neuroscience to clinical applications. Nature Neuroscience 19(3):404–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ignatow, G. (2009) Why the sociology of morality meeds Bourdieu's habitus. Sociological Inquiry 79:98114. http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1475-682X.2008.00273.x/full.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ingold, T. (2001) From the transmission of representations to the education of Attention. In: Debated Mind: Evolutionary Psychology versus Ethnography, ed. Whitehouse, H., pp. 113–53. Berg.Google Scholar
Ingold, T. (2016) Lines: A brief history. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jack, A. I. (2014) A scientific case for conceptual dualism: The problem of consciousness and the opposing domains hypothesis. Oxford Studies in Experimental Philosophy 1:132.Google Scholar
Joffily, M. & Coricelli, G. (2013) Emotional valence and the free-energy principle. PLoS Computational Biology 9(6):e1003094. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003094.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, S. C., Dweck, C. S. & Chen, F. S. (2007) Evidence for infants’ internal working models of attachment. Psychological Science 18(6):501502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. (2011) Thinking, fast and slow. Macmillan.Google Scholar
Kaplan, R. & Friston, K. J. (2018) Planning and navigation as active inference. Biological Cybernetics 112:323–43. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00422-018-0753-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufmann, L. & Clément, F. (2014) Wired for society: Cognizing pathways to society and culture. Topoi. An International Review of Philosophy 33(2):459–75.Google Scholar
Keane, W. (2015) Varieties of ethical stance. In: Four lectures on ethics: Anthropological perspectives, eds. Lambek, M., Das, V., Fassin, D. & Hau, W. Keane..Google Scholar
Kelly, D., Faucher, L. & Machery, E. (2010) Getting rid of racism: Assessing three proposals in light of psychological evidence. Journal of Social Philosophy 41(3):293322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kiebel, S. J., Daunizeau, J. & Friston, K. J. (2008) A hierarchy of time-scales and the brain. PLoS Computational Biology 4(11):e1000209. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiebel, S. J. & Friston, K. J. (2011) Free energy and dendritic self-organization. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 5:80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E. & Spelke, E. S. (2007) The native language of social cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104(30):12577–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kinzler, K. D. & Spelke, E. S. (2011) Do infants show social preferences for people differing in race? Cognition 119(1):19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirmayer, L. J. (1989) Cultural variations in the response to psychiatric disorders and emotional distress. Social Science & Medicine 29(3):327–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirmayer, L. J. (2015) Re-visioning psychiatry: Toward an ecology of mind in health and illness. In: Re-visioning psychiatry: Cultural phenomenology, critical neuroscience and global mental health, eds. Kirmayer, L. J., Lemelson, R. & Cummings, C. A., pp. 622–60. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirmayer, L. J. & Gold, I. (2011) Re-socializing psychiatry: Critical neuroscience and the limits of reductionism. In: Critical Neuroscience, eds. Choudhury, S. and Slaby, J., pp. 305–30. Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirmayer, L. J., Gomez-Carrillo, A. & Veissière, S. P. L. (2017) Culture and depression in global mental health: An ecosocial approach to the phenomenology of psychiatric disorders. Social Science and Medicine 183:163–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirmayer, L. J., Lemelson, R. & Cummings, C. A. (2015) Re-visioning psychiatry: Cultural phenomenology, critical neuroscience, and global mental health. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirmayer, L. J. & Ramstead, M. J. D. (2017) Embodiment and enactment in cultural psychiatry. In: Embodiment, enaction, and culture: Investigating the constitution of the shared world, eds. Durt, C., Fuchs, T., & Tewes, C., pp. 397422. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kirmayer, L. J. & Young, A. (1998) Culture and somatization: Clinical, epidemiological, and ethnographic perspectives. Psychosomatic Medicine 60(4):420–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiverstein, J., Miller, M. & Rietveld, E. (2019) The feeling of grip: Novelty, error dynamics and the predictive brain. Synthese 196(7):2847–69. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-017-1583-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurzban, R. & Neuberg, S. (2005) Managing ingroup and outgroup relationships. In: The handbook of evolutionary psychology, ed. Buss, D. M., pp. 653–75. Wiley.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. & Johnson, M. (1980) The metaphorical structure of the human conceptual system. Cognitive Science 4(2):195208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laland, K. N. (2018) Darwin's unfinished symphony: How culture made the human mind. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N., Uller, T., Feldman, M. W., Sterelny, K., Müller, G. B., Moczek, A., Jablonka, E. & Odling-Smee, J. (2015) The extended evolutionary synthesis: Its structure, assumptions and predictions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 282(1813):20151019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lebois, L. A. M., Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Simmons, W. K., Feldman Barrett, L. & Barsalou, L. W. (in press) Learning situated emotions. Neuropsychologia.Google Scholar
Levy, R. I. (1975) Tahitians: Mind and experience in the society islands. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Levy, R. I. (1984) Emotion, knowing and culture. In: Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion, eds. Shweder, R. & LeVine, R., pp. 214–37. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Luhrmann, T. (2011) Toward an anthropological theory of mind. Suomen Antropologi: Journal of the Finnish Anthropological Society 36(4):569.Google Scholar
Luo, S., Li, B., Ma, Y., Zhang, W., Rao, Y. & Han, S. (2015) Oxytocin receptor gene and racial ingroup bias in empathy-related brain activity. NeuroImage 110:2231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luo, Y. & Baillargeon, R. (2005) Can a self-propelled box have a goal? Psychological reasoning in 5-month-old infants. Psychological Science 16(8):601608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lutz, A., Brefczynski-Lewis, J., Johnstone, T. & Davidson, R. J. (2008) Regulation of the neural circuitry of emotion by compassion meditation: Effects of meditative expertise. PLoS ONE 3(3):e1897.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Machery, E. (2016) De-Freuding implicit attitudes. In: Implicit bias and philosophy. Vol. 1. Metaphysics and epistemology, eds. Brownstein, M & Saul, J., pp. 104–29. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Machery, E. & Faucher, L. (2017) Why do we think racially? Culture, evolution, and cognition. In: Handbook of categorization in cognitive science, 2nd edition, eds. Cohen, H. and Lefebvre, C., pp. 1135–75. Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madoka, M. (2003) Haiku. In: Far beyond the field: Haiku by Japanese women, ed. Ueda, M., p. 232. Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Mahajan, N. & Woodward, A. (2009) Seven-month-old infants selectively reproduce the goals of animate but not inanimate agents. Infancy 14(6):667–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malafouris, L. (2015) Metaplasticity and the primacy of material engagement. Time and Mind 8(4):351–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mameli, M. (2001) Mindreading, mindshaping, and evolution. Biology and Philosophy 16(5):595626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mauss, M. (1973) Techniques of the body. Economy and Society 2(1):7088.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCauley, R. N. & Henrich, J. (2006) Susceptibility to the Müller-Lyer illusion, theory-neutral observation, and the diachronic penetrability of the visual input system. Philosophical Psychology 19(1):79101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGeer, V. (2007) The regulative dimension of folk psychology. In: Folk psychology re-assessed, eds. Hutto, D. D. & Ratcliffe, M., pp. 137–56. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4020-5558-4_8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menary, R. (2010) The extended mind and cognitive integration. In: The extended mind, ed. Menary, R., pp. 267–88. MIT Press,CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mercier, H. & Sperber, D. (2017) The enigma of reason. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michael, J., Christensen, W. & Overgaard, S. (2014) Mindreading as social expertise. Synthese 191(5):817–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milgram, S. (1963) Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 67:371–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miresco, M. J. & Kirmayer, L. J. (2006) The persistence of mind-brain dualism in psychiatric reasoning about clinical scenarios. American Journal of Psychiatry 163(5):913–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mirza, M. B., Adams, R. A., Mathys, C. D. & Friston, K. J. (2016) Scene construction, visual foraging, and active inference. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience 10(56):116. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncom.2016.00056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montague, P. R., Dolan, R. J., Friston, K. J. & Dayan, P. (2012) Computational psychiatry. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16(1):7280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, T. J. H. & Laland, K. N. (2012) The biological bases of conformity. Frontiers in Neuroscience 6:87. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2012.00087.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Navarrete, C. D. & Fessler, D. M. T. (2005) Normative bias and adaptive challenges: A relational approach to coalitional psychology and a critique of terror management theory. Evolutionary Psychology 3(1):297325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Odling-Smee, J., Laland, K. N. & Feldman, M. W. (2003) Niche construction: The neglected process in evolution. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Olson, K. R. & Spelke, E. S. (2008) Foundations of cooperation in young children. Cognition 108(1):222–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Onishi, K. H. & Baillargeon, R. (2005) Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science 308(5719):255–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oudeyer, P.-Y. & Kaplan, F. (2007) What is intrinsic motivation? A typology of computational approaches. Frontiers in Neurorobotics 1:6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parr, T. & Friston, K. J. (2017a) Uncertainty, epistemics and active inference. Journal of the Royal Society Interface 14(136):20170376. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2017.0376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parr, T. & Friston, K. J. (2017b) Working memory, attention, and salience in active inference. Scientific Reports 7(1):14678.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parr, T. & Friston, K. J. (2019) Attention or salience? Current Opinion in Psychology 29:15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.10.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pauker, K., Williams, A. & Steele, J. R. (2016) Children's racial categorization in context. Child Development Perspectives 10(1):3338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pezzulo, G., Cartoni, E., Rigoli, F., Pio-Lopez, L. & Friston, K. J. (2016) Active inference, epistemic value, and vicarious trial and error. Learning & Memory 23(7):322–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pezzulo, G. & Cisek, P. (2016) Navigating the affordance landscape: Feedback control as a process model of behavior and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20(6):414–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, M. L., Young, A. W., Senior, C., Brammer, M., Andrew, C., Calder, A. J., Bullmore, E. T., Perrett, D. I., Rowland, D., Williams, S. C. R., Gray, J. A. & David, A. S. (1997) A specific neural substrate for perceiving facial expressions of disgust. Nature 389(6650):495–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, S. (1999) How the mind works. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 882:119–27, discussion 128–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, S. (2003) Language as an adaptation to the cognitive niche. In: Language evolution: States of the art, eds. Kirby, S. & Christiansen, M. H., pp. 1637. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poerio, G. L. & Smallwood, J. (2016) Daydreaming to navigate the social world: What we know, what we don't know, and why it matters. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 10(11):605–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramsey, W. M. (2007) Representation reconsidered. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramstead, M. J. D., Badcock, P. B. & Friston, K. J. (2018) Answering Schrödinger's question: A free-energy formulation. Physics of Life Reviews 24:116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plrev.2017.09.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramstead, M. J. D., Constant, A., Badcock, P. B. & Friston, K. (2019a) Variational ecology and the physics of sentient systems. Physics of Life Reviews 31:188205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramstead, M. J. D., Veissière, S. P. L. & Kirmayer, L. J. (2016) Cultural affordances: Scaffolding local worlds through shared intentionality and regimes of attention. Frontiers in Psychology 7:1090.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rietveld, E. & Brouwers, A. A. (2017) Optimal grip on affordances in architectural design practices: An ethnography. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 16(3):545–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rietveld, E. & Kiverstein, J. (2014) A rich landscape of affordances. Ecological Psychology 26(4):325–52. https://doi.org/10.1080/10407413.2014.958035.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robbins, J. (2008) On not knowing other minds: Confession, intention, and linguistic exchange in a Papua New Guinea community. Anthropological Quarterly 81(2):421–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robbins, J., Cassaniti, J. & Luhrmann, T. M. (2011) The constitution of mind: What's in a mind? Interiority and boundedness. Suomen Antropologi 36(4):1520.Google Scholar
Robbins, J. & Rumsey, A. (2008) Introduction: Cultural and linguistic anthropology and the opacity of other minds. Anthropological Quarterly 81(2):407–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roepstorff, A., Niewöhner, J. & Beck, S. (2010) Enculturing brains through patterned practices. Neural Networks 23(8–9):1051–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosaldo, M. Z. (1982) The things we do with words: Ilongot speech acts and speech act theory in philosophy. Language in Society 11(2):203–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P., Haidt, J. & Fincher, K. (2009) Psychology: From oral to moral. Science 323(5918):1179–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rumsey, A. (2013) Intersubjectivity, deception and the “opacity of other minds”: Perspectives from Highland New Guinea and beyond. Language & Communication 33(3):326–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schilbach, L. (2016) Towards a second-person neuropsychiatry. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 371(1686):20150081. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidhuber, J. (2006) Developmental robotics, optimal artificial curiosity, creativity, music, and the fine arts. Connection Science 18(2):173–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartenbeck, P. & Friston, K. (2016) Computational phenotyping in psychiatry: A worked example. eNeuro 3(4):ENEURO.0049-16.2016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seligman, R., Choudhury, S. & Kirmayer, L. J. (2016) Locating culture in the brain and in the world: From social categories to the ecology of mind. In Handbook of cultural neuroscience, eds. Chiao, J. Y., Li, S. C., Seligman, R., & Turner, R., pp. 320. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Seth, A. K. & Friston, K. J. (2016) Active interoceptive inference and the emotional brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 371(1708):20160007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shapiro, L. (2010) Embodied cognition. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shelley-Tremblay, J. F. & Rosén, L. A. (1996) Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: An evolutionary perspective. Journal of Genetic Psychology 157(4):443–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spelke, E. S. & Kinzler, K. D. (2007) Core knowledge. Developmental Science 10(1):8996.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sperber, D. (1996) Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Wiley.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1997) Intuitive and reflective beliefs. Mind & Language 12(1):6783.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stasch, R. (2009) Society of others kinship and mourning in a West Papuan place. University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephan, K. E., Kasper, L., Harrison, L. M., Daunizeau, J., den Ouden, H. E. M., Breakspear, M. & Friston, K. J. (2008) Nonlinear dynamic causal models for fMRI. NeuroImage 42(2):649–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sterelny, K. (2012) The evolved apprentice. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stotz, K. (2017) Why developmental niche construction is not selective niche construction: And why it matters. Interface Focus 7(5):20160157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stotz, K. & Griffiths, P. E. (2017) A developmental systems account of human nature. In: Why we disagree about human nature, eds. Lewens, T. & Hannon, E., pp. 5875. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stout, D. & Chaminade, T. (2007) The evolutionary neuroscience of tool making. Neuropsychologia 45(5):1091–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stout, D., Toth, N., Schick, K. & Chaminade, T. (2008) Neural correlates of early Stone Age toolmaking: Technology, language and cognition in human evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 363(1499):1939–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutton, J. (2010) Exograms and interdisciplinarity: History, the extended mind, and the civilizing process. In: The extended mind, ed. Menary, R., pp. 189225. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swami, V., Frederick, D. A., Aavik, T., Alcalay, L., Allik, J., Anderson, D., Andrianto, S., Arora, A., Brännström, A., Cunningham, J., Danel, D., Doroszewicz, K., Forbes, G. B., Furnham, A., Greven, C. U., Halberstadt, J., Hao, S., Haubner, T., Hwang, C. S., Inman, M., Jaafar, J. L., Johansson, J., Jung, J., Keser, A., Kretzschmar, U., Lachenicht, L., Li, N. P., Locke, K., Lönnqvist, J. E., Lopez, C., Loutzenhiser, L., Maisel, N. C., McCabe, M. P., McCreary, D. R., McKibbin, W. F., Mussap, A., Neto, F., Nowell, C., Alampay, L. P., Pillai, S. K., Pokrajac-Bulian, A., Proyer, R. T., Quintelier, K., Ricciardelli, L. A., Rozmus-Wrzesinska, M., Ruch, W., Russo, T., Schütz, A., Shackelford, T. K., Shashidharan, S., Simonetti, F., Sinniah, D., Swami, M., Vandermassen, G., van Duynslaeger, M., Verkasalo, M., Voracek, M., Yee, C. K., Zhang, E. X., Zhang, X. & Zivcic-Becirevic, I. (2010) The attractive female body weight and female body dissatisfaction in 26 countries across 10 world regions: Results of the International Body Project I. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin 36(3):309–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, J., Moyzis, R., Fossella, J., Fan, J. & Posner, M. I. (2002) Adaptationism and molecular biology: An example based on ADHD. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25(4):530–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, C. (2016) The language animal. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Timmermans, B., Schilbach, L., Pasquali, A. & Cleeremans, A. (2012) Higher order thoughts in action: Consciousness as an unconscious re-description process. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367(1594):1412–23. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (2009) Why we cooperate. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2014) A natural history of human thinking. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T. & Moll, H. (2005) Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28(5):675–91, discussion 691–735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tovo-Rodrigues, L., Rohde, L. A., Menezes, A. M. B., Polanczyk, G. V., Kieling, C., Genro, J. P., Anselmi, L. & Hutz, M. H. (2013) DRD4 rare variants in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Further evidence from a birth cohort study. PLoS ONE 8(12):e85164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trivers, R. (2000) The elements of a scientific theory of self-deception. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 907:114–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tschacher, W. & Haken, H. (2007) Intentionality in non-equilibrium systems? The functional aspects of self-organized pattern formation. New Ideas in Psychology 25(1):115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., Kurzban, R. & DeScioli, P. (2013) Disgust: Evolved function and structure. Psychological Review 120(1):6584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Dijk, L. & Rietveld, E. (2017) Foregrounding sociomaterial practice in our understanding of affordances: The skilled intentionality framework. Frontiers in Psychology 7:1969. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01969.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veissière, S. (2016) Varieties of tulpa experiences: The hypnotic nature of human sociality, personhood, and interphenomenality. In: Hypnosis and Meditation: Towards an Integrative Science of Conscious Planes, eds. Raz, A. & Lifshitz, M., pp. 5576. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Veissière, S. (2018) Cultural Markov blankets? Mind the other minds gap! Comment on “Answering Schrödinger's question: A free-energy formulation” by Maxwell James Désormeau Ramstead et al. Physics of Life Reviews 24:4749.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
von der Lühe, T., Manera, V., Barisic, I., Becchio, C., Vogeley, K. & Schilbach, L. (2016) Interpersonal predictive coding, not action perception, is impaired in autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 371(1693):20150373. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, Y.-G., Wang, Y.-Q., Chen, S.-L., Zhu, C.-Y. & Wang, K. (2008) Theory of mind disability in major depression with or without psychotic symptoms: A componential view. Psychiatry Research 161(2):153–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A. & Erdal, D. (2012) The human socio-cognitive niche and its evolutionary origins. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367(1599):2119–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, B. (2011) Ethics and the limits of philosophy. Taylor & Francis.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, L. T., Nancarrow, C. & Kwok, P. M. H. (2001) Food taste preferences and cultural influences on consumption. British Food Journal 103(5):348–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zatzick, D. F. & Dimsdale, J. E. (1990) Cultural variations in response to painful stimuli. Psychosomatic Medicine 52(5):544–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zawidzki, T. W. (2008) The function of folk psychology: Mind reading or mind shaping? Philosophical Explorations: An International Journal for the Philosophy of Mind and Action 11(3):193210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zawidzki, T. W. (2013) Mindshaping: A new framework for understanding human social cognition. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar