Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T19:10:05.309Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Enhanced action control as a prior function of episodic memory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 January 2018

Philipp Rau
Affiliation:
Department of Philosophy, University of Sheffield, Sheffield S3 7QB, United Kingdom. [email protected]@sheffield.ac.ukhttps://sheffield.academia.edu/rauhttp://www.sheffield.ac.uk/philosophy/staff/profiles/botterill
George Botterill
Affiliation:
Department of Philosophy, University of Sheffield, Sheffield S3 7QB, United Kingdom. [email protected]@sheffield.ac.ukhttps://sheffield.academia.edu/rauhttp://www.sheffield.ac.uk/philosophy/staff/profiles/botterill

Abstract

Improved control of agency is likely to be a prior and more important function of episodic memory than the epistemic-communicative role pinpointed by Mahr & Csibra (M&C). Taking the memory trace upon which scenario construction is based to be a stored internal model produced in past perceptual processing promises to provide a better account of autonoetic character than metarepresentational embedding.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Clark, A. (2013) Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36(3):181204. Available at: doi:10.1017/S0140525X12000477.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2014) Perceiving as predicting. In: Perception and its modalities, ed. Stokes, D., Matthen, M. & Biggs, S., pp. 2343. Oxford University Press. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199832798.001.0001.Google Scholar
Clayton, N. S. & Dickinson, A. (1998) Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays. Nature 395(6699):272–74. Available at: http://www.nature.com/nature/journal/v395/n6699/abs/395272a0.html.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L. (1989) The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition 31(3):187276. Available at: doi:10.1016/0010-0277(89)90023-1.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (1992) Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In: The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture, ed. Barkow, J., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J., pp. 163228. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. & Lewontin, R. C. (1979) The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 205(1161):581–98. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1979.0086.Google Scholar
Petro, L. S. & Muckli, L. (2016) The brain's predictive prowess revealed in primary visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 113(5):1124–25. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1523834113.Google Scholar
Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z. & Jordan, M. I. (1995) An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science 269(5232):1880–82.Google Scholar
Wolpert, D. M., Goodbody, S. J. & Husain, M. (1998) Maintaining internal representations: The role of the human superior parietal lobe. Nature Neuroscience 1(6):529–33. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1038/2245.Google Scholar