Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T19:13:05.211Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Phonation takes precedence over articulation in development as well as evolution of language

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 December 2014

D. Kimbrough Oller*
Affiliation:
School of Communication Sciences and Disorders, The University of Memphis, Memphis, TN 38105. [email protected]://umwa.memphis.edu/fcv/viewprofile.php?uuid=koller Institute for Intelligent Systems, The University of Memphis, Memphis, TN 38152 Konrad Lorenz Institute for Evolution and Cognition Research, A-3400 Klosterneuburg, Austria.

Abstract

Early human vocal development is characterized first by emerging control of phonation and later by prosodic and supraglottal articulation. The target article has missed the opportunity to use these facts in the characterization of evolution in language-specific brain mechanisms. Phonation appears to be the initial human-specific brain change for language, and it was presumably a key target of selection in early hominin evolution.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bertossa, R. C., ed. (2011) Theme issue: Evolutionary developmental biology (evo-devo) and behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 366(1574):2056–180. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0035.Google Scholar
Buder, E. H., Chorna, L., Oller, D. K. & Robinson, R. (2008) Vibratory regime classification of infant phonation. Journal of Voice 22:553–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crockford, C. & Boesch, C. (2003) Context-specific calls in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus: Analysis of barks. Animal Behaviour 66:115–25. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2003.2166.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. (1978) The facial action coding system. Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Franklin, B. S., Warlaumont, A. S., Messinger, D. S., Bene, E. R., Iyer, S. N., Lee, C.-C., Lambert, B. & Oller, D. K. (2014) Effects of parental interaction on infant vocalization rate, variability and vocal type. Language Learning and Development 10(3):279–96. doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2013.849176.Google Scholar
Goldstein, M. H., Schwade, J. A. & Bornstein, M. H. (2009) The value of vocalizing: Five-month-old infants associate their own noncry vocalizations with responses from adults. Child Development 80(3):636–44.Google Scholar
Griebel, U. & Oller, D. K. (2008) Evolutionary forces favoring contextual flexibility. In: Evolution of communicative flexibility: Complexity, creativity and adaptability in human and animal communication, ed. Oller, D. K. & Griebel, U., pp. 940. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Koopmans-van Beinum, F. J. & van der Stelt, J. M. (1986) Early stages in the development of speech movements. In: Precursors of early speech, ed. Lindblom, B. & Zetterstrom, R., pp. 3750. Stockton Press.Google Scholar
Kwon, K., Oller, D. K. & Buder, E. H. (2007) Evidence of systematic repetition in infant vocalizations. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, Boston, MA, November 2007.Google Scholar
Laporte, M. N. C. & Zuberbühler, K. (2010) Vocal greeting behaviour in wild chimpanzee females. Animal Behaviour 80:467–73.Google Scholar
Locke, J. L. (1993) The child's path to spoken language, First edition. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Nathani, S., Neal, A. R., Olds, H., Brill, J. & Oller, D. K. (2001) Canonical babbling and volubility in infants with moderate to severe hearing impairment. Paper presented at the International Child Phonology Conference, Boston, MA, April 2001.Google Scholar
Oller, D. K. (1980) The emergence of the sounds of speech in infancy. In: Child phonology, vol. 1: Production, ed. Yeni-Komshian, G., Kavanagh, J. & Ferguson, C., pp. 93112. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Oller, D. K. (2000) The emergence of the speech capacity. Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oller, D. K., Buder, E. H., Ramsdell, H. L., Chorna, L., Warlaumont, A. S. & Bakeman, R. (2013) Functional flexibility of infant vocalization and the emergence of language. In: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 110(16):6318–23. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1300337110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parr, L., Waller, B. M. & Heintz, M. (2008) Facial expression categorization by chimpanzees using standardized stimuli. Emotion 8(2):216–31. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.2.216.Google Scholar
Scheiner, E., Hammerschmidt, K., Jürgens, U. & Zwirner, P. (2006) Vocal expression of emotions in normally hearing and hearing-impaired infants. Journal of Voice 20(4):585604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stark, R. E. (1980) Stages of speech development in the first year of life. In: Child phonology, vol. 1: Production, ed. Yeni-Komshian, G., Kavanagh, J. & Ferguson, C., pp. 7390. Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stark, R. E., Bernstein, L. E. & Demorest, M. E. (1993) Vocal communication in the first 18 months of life. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 36:548–58.Google Scholar
Yale, M. E., Messinger, D. S., Cobo-Lewis, A. B., Oller, D. K. & Eilers, R. E. (1999) An event-based analysis of the coordination of early infant vocalizations and facial actions. Developmental Psychology 35(2):505–13.Google Scholar