Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-mwx4w Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-30T19:15:04.045Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Where do the hypotheses come from? Data-driven learning in science and the brain

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 December 2023

Barton L. Anderson
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia [email protected]
Katherine R. Storrs
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand [email protected]
Roland W. Fleming
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Justus Liebig University of Giessen, Giessen, Germany [email protected] Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior, Universities of Marburg and Giessen, Giessen, Germany

Abstract

Everyone agrees that testing hypotheses is important, but Bowers et al. provide scant details about where hypotheses about perception and brain function should come from. We suggest that the answer lies in considering how information about the outside world could be acquired – that is, learned – over the course of evolution and development. Deep neural networks (DNNs) provide one tool to address this question.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, B. L., & Marlow, P. J. (2023). Perceiving the shape and material properties of 3D surfaces. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 27(1), 98110. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2022.10.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golan, T., Raju, P. C., & Kriegeskorte, N. (2020). Controversial stimuli: Pitting neural networks against each other as models of human cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 117(47), 2933029337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ho, Y. X., Landy, M. S., & Maloney, L. T. (2008). Conjoint measurement of gloss and surface texture. Psychological Science, 19, 196204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marlow, P., & Anderson, B. (2021). The cospecification of the shape and material properties of light permeable materials. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 118(14), e2024798118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marlow, P., Kim, J., & Anderson, B. (2012). The perception and misperception of specular surface reflectance. Current Biology, 22(20), 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marlow, P., Mooney, S., & Anderson, B. (2019). Photogeometric cues to perceived surface shading. Current Biology, 29(2), 306311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Storrs, K. R., Anderson, B. L., & Fleming, R. W. (2021b). Unsupervised learning predicts human perception and misperception of gloss. Nature Human Behavior, 5, 14021417. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01097-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Storrs, K. R., Kietzmann, T. C., Walther, A., Mehrer, J., & Kriegeskorte, N. (2021a). Diverse deep neural networks all predict human inferior temporal cortex well, after training and fitting. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 33(10), 20442064.Google ScholarPubMed
Wang, Z., & Simoncelli, E. P. (2008). Maximum differentiation (MAD) competition: A methodology for comparing computational models of perceptual quantities. Journal of Vision, 8(12), 8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yamins, D., & DiCarlo, J. (2016). Using goal-driven deep learning models to understand sensory cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 19, 356365. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4244CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed