Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T14:49:23.988Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The evolution of coordinated vocalizations before language

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 December 2014

Gregory A. Bryant*
Affiliation:
Department of Communication, Center for Behavior, Evolution, and Culture, University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA), Los Angeles, CA 90095-1563. [email protected]://gabryant.bol.ucla.edu/

Abstract

Ackermann et al. briefly point out the potential significance of coordinated vocal behavior in the dual pathway model of acoustic communication. Rhythmically entrained and articulated pre-linguistic vocal activity in early hominins might have set the evolutionary stage for later refinements that manifest in modern humans as language-based conversational turn-taking, joint music-making, and other behaviors associated with prosociality.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bailenson, J. N., Yee, N., Patel, K. & Beall, A. C. (2008) Detecting digital chameleons. Computers in Human Behavior 24:6687.Google Scholar
Bispham, J. (2006) Rhythm in music: What is it? Who has it? And why? Music Perception 24:125–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryant, G. A. (2012) Shared laughter in conversation as coalition signaling. Paper presented at the XXI Biennial International Conference on Human Ethology, Vienna, Austria, August 13, 2012.Google Scholar
Bryant, G. A. (2013) Animal signals and emotion in music: Coordinating affect across groups. Frontiers in Psychology 4(Article 990):113.Google Scholar
Bryant, G. A. & Aktipis, A. (2014) The animal nature of spontaneous human laughter. Evolution and Human Behavior 35(4):327–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davila-Ross, M., Owren, M. & Zimmermann, E. (2009) Reconstructing the evolution of laughter in great apes and humans. Current Biology 19:1106–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hagen, E. H. & Bryant, G. A. (2003) Music and dance as a coalition signaling system. Human Nature 14(1):2151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hagen, E. H. & Hammerstein, P. (2009) Did Neanderthals and other early humans sing? Seeking the biological roots of music in the territorial advertisements of primates, lions, hyenas, and wolves. Musicae Scientiae 13(Suppl. 2):291320.Google Scholar
Kirschner, S. & Tomasello, M. (2010) Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4- year-old children. Evolution and Human Behavior 31:354–64.Google Scholar
Manson, J. E., Bryant, G. A., Gervais, M. & Kline, M. (2013) Convergence of speech rate in conversation predicts cooperation. Evolution and Human Behavior 34(6):419–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perlman, M., Patterson, F. G. & Cohn, R. H. (2012) The human-fostered gorilla Koko shows breath control in play with wind instruments. Biolinguistics 6:433–44.Google Scholar
Phillips-Silver, J., Aktipis, A. & Bryant, G. A. (2010) The ecology of entrainment: Foundations of coordinated rhythmic movement. Music Perception 28(1):314.Google Scholar
Provine, R. R. (1993) Laughter punctuates speech: Linguistic, social and gender contexts of laughter. Ethology 95:291–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Provine, R. R. (2000) Laughter: A scientific investigation. Viking/Penguin Press.Google Scholar
Repp, B. H. (2005) Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of the tapping literature. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 12:969–92.Google Scholar
Richman, B. (1976) Some vocal distinctive features used by gelada monkeys. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 60:718–24.Google Scholar
Wilson, M. & Wilson, T. P. (2005) An oscillator model of the timing of turn-taking. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 12:957–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiltermuth, S. S. & Heath, C. (2009) Synchrony and cooperation. Psychological Science 20:15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed