No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Social and economic interdependence as a basis for peaceful between-group relationships in nonhuman primates and humans
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 15 January 2024
Abstract
Glowacki asserts that interdependent relationships beyond group boundaries are exceptionally rare among nonhuman mammals. However, rudimentary forms of interdependence can be seen in primate species that form multilevel societies, that is, core social units embedded within higher-level grouping categories. Studies of primate multilevel societies can enrich discussions about the evolutionary origins of peaceful between-group interactions in humans.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Alexander, R. D. (1990). How did humans evolve? Reflections on the uniquely unique species. Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Camerlenghi, E., McQueen, A., Delhey, K., Cook, C. N., Kingma, S. A., Farine, D. R., & Peters, A. (2022). Cooperative breeding and the emergence of multilevel societies in birds. Ecology Letters, 25, 766–777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camerlenghi, E., Nolazco, S., Farine, D. R., Magrath, R. D., & Peters, A. (2023). Multilevel social structure predicts individual helping responses in a songbird. Current Biology, 33, 1–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapais, B. (2008). Primeval kinship: How pair-bonding gave birth to human society. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drury, J., Cocking, C., & Reicher, S. (2009). Everyone for themselves? A comparative study of crowd solidarity among emergency survivors. British Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 487–506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forcina, G., Vallet, D., Le Gouar, P. J., Bernardo-Madrid, R., Illera, G., Molina-Vacas, G., … Bermejo, M. (2019). From groups to communities in western lowland gorillas. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 286, 20182019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fry, D. P. (2012). Life without war. Science (New York, N.Y.), 336, 879–884.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grueter, C. C., Chapais, B., & Zinner, D. (2012). Evolution of multilevel societies in nonhuman primates and humans. International Journal of Primatology, 33, 1002–1037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grueter, C. C., Qi, X., Zinner, D., Bergman, T., Li, B., Li, M., … Swedell, L. (2020). Multilevel organisation of animal sociality. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 35, 834–847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grueter, C. C., & van Schaik, C. P. (2010). Evolutionary determinants of modular societies in colobines. Behavioral Ecology, 21, 63–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grueter, C. C., & White, D. R. (2014). On the emergence of large-scale human social integration and its antecedents in primates. Structure and Dynamics, 7, 1–27.Google Scholar
Grueter, C. C., & Wilson, M. L. (2021). Do we need to reclassify the social systems of gregarious apes? Evolutionary Anthropology, 30, 316–326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, M. O., & Nei, S. (2015). Networks of military alliances, wars, and international trade. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112, 15277–15284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaeggi, A. V., Boose, K. J., White, F. J., & Gurven, M. (2016). Obstacles and catalysts of cooperation in humans, bonobos, and chimpanzees: Behavioural reaction norms can help explain variation in sex roles, inequality, war and peace. Behaviour, 153, 1015–1051.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kang, G. E. (1979). Exogamy and peace relations of social units: A cross-cultural test. Ethnology, 18, 85–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, R. L. (2013). The lifeways of hunter–gatherers: The foraging Spectrum. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kummer, H. (1968). Social organization of hamadryas baboons: A field study. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lévi-Strauss, C. (1949). Les structures élémentaires de la parenté. Paris Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Macdonald, D., & Johnson, D. (2015). Patchwork planet: The resource dispersion hypothesis, society, and the ecology of life. Journal of Zoology, 295, 75–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mirville, M. O., Ridley, A. R., Samedi, J. P. M., Vecellio, V., Ndagijimana, F., Stoinski, T. S., & Grueter, C. C. (2018). Low familiarity and similar “group strength” between opponents increase the intensity of intergroup interactions in mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 72, 178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pisor, A. C., & Gurven, M. (2016). Risk buffering and resource access shape valuation of out-group strangers. Scientific Reports, 6, 30435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reichard, U., & Sommer, V. (1997). Group encounters in wild gibbons (Hylobates lar): Agonism, affiliation, and the concept of infanticide. Behaviour, 134, 1135–1174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, E. J. H., & Barker, J. L. (2017). Inter-group cooperation in humans and other animals. Biology Letters, 13, 20160793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodrigues, A. M., Barker, J. L., & Robinson, E. J. (2023). The evolution of intergroup cooperation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 378(1874), 20220074.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodseth, L., Wrangham, R. W., Harrigan, A. M., & Smuts, B. B. (1991). The human community as a primate society. Current Anthropology, 32, 221–254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snyder-Mackler, N., Alberts, S. C., & Bergman, T. J. (2014). The socio-genetics of a complex society: Female gelada relatedness patterns mirror association patterns in a multilevel society. Molecular Ecology, 23, 6179–6191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tokuyama, N., Sakamaki, T., & Furuichi, T. (2019). Inter-group aggressive interaction patterns indicate male mate defense and female cooperation across bonobo groups at Wamba, Democratic Republic of the Congo. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 170, 535–550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiessner, P. (2002). Hunting, healing, and hxaro exchange: A long-term perspective on !Kung (Ju/'hoansi) large-game hunting. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 407–436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xiang, Z., Yang, B., Yu, Y., Yao, H., Grueter, C. C., Garber, P. A., & Li, M. (2014). Males collectively defend their one-male units against bachelor males in a multi-level primate society. American Journal of Primatology, 76, 609–617.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
You have
Access
Between-group peace and cooperation are critical for navigating pressing societal challenges such as security threats and climate change, and as attested by the timely target article by Glowacki, humans have an arsenal of strategies at their disposal to establish, maintain, and restore peace. I generally concur with Glowacki that stable interdependent relationships crossing group boundaries are the province of humans. However, are they really exceedingly rare among nonhuman mammals, including nonhuman primates? Here, I argue that they are not, and that by examining potential parallels between humans and nonhuman primates with multilevel societies we can better characterize the frequency and predictors of peace in primates.
As pointed out by Glowacki, some primate taxa exhibit peaceful encounters between groups (e.g., western gorillas, Forcina et al., Reference Forcina, Vallet, Le Gouar, Bernardo-Madrid, Illera, Molina-Vacas and Bermejo2019; white-handed gibbons, Reichard & Sommer, Reference Reichard and Sommer1997) and even cooperative interactions (bonobos, Tokuyama, Sakamaki, & Furuichi Reference Tokuyama, Sakamaki and Furuichi2019). However, as Glowacki notes, these tend to be transient (Grueter & Wilson, Reference Grueter and Wilson2021). Rudimentary forms of interdependence resembling durable positive-sum relationships do seem to occur in species that feature multilevel societies. Multilevel societies, that is, where multiple core social units coalesce in a semi-interactive manner to form higher-level “clans,” bands, and communities, are best known from snub-nosed monkeys, papionins, and humans (Grueter, Chapais, & Zinner, Reference Grueter, Chapais and Zinner2012; Grueter et al., Reference Grueter, Qi, Zinner, Bergman, Li, Li and Swedell2020; Rodseth, Wrangham, Harrigan, & Smuts, Reference Rodseth, Wrangham, Harrigan and Smuts1991). Mechanistically, a multilevel system emerges through coordinated behaviour among the core units and persistent mutual tolerance. The adaptive utility of this complex nexus of social interactions lies in its members being able to concurrently reap the benefits of multiple social levels, for example, access to cooperation partners at the band level and access to social services such as grooming or allocare at the core unit level (Grueter et al., Reference Grueter, Qi, Zinner, Bergman, Li, Li and Swedell2020).
The between-unit tolerance underlying multilevel societies is evolutionarily stabilized by several interactive forces, for example, the need for collective defence against external adversaries, the need to access patchily distributed resources and the existence of supragroup kin networks. Similar forms of social and economic interdependence can also explain peaceful intergroup relationships in humans.
In humans, between-group competition represents a major selective force for the evolution of intergroup peace and suprafamilial groupings (Alexander, Reference Alexander1990). The need for cooperative defence (or aggression) against shared external threats may suppress in-group favouritism and elicit amicable sentiments to neighbouring communities. A contemporary real-life example includes Russia's invasion of Ukraine which improved Ukraine's relationship with its allies, and the relationships among its allies (especially among European Nation states). Exposure to a common threat (such as a terrorist attack) can also cause strangers to identify themselves as belonging to a uniform social entity (Drury, Cocking, & Reicher, Reference Drury, Cocking and Reicher2009). In primate/animal multilevel systems, a similar catalyst for the emergence of tolerance between members of different units is an external threat which can originate from conspecifics or predators. Through aggregation for common defence, different groups can pool the risks of foreign threats (Camerlenghi, Nolazco, Farine, Magrath, & Peters, Reference Camerlenghi, Nolazco, Farine, Magrath and Peters2023; Grueter & van Schaik, Reference Grueter and van Schaik2010; Kummer, Reference Kummer1968; Xiang et al., Reference Xiang, Yang, Yu, Yao, Grueter, Garber and Li2014).
Economic and ecological interdependence such as trade of subsistence goods or mutually agreed-upon permission to use shared territories can also function against the waging of war in humans (Fry, Reference Fry2012; target article). Such symbiotic exchange networks between groups are critical in environments in which variance in resource access among groups is high because they can act as a buffer against resource failure in one's area (Kelly, Reference Kelly2013; Pisor & Gurven, Reference Pisor and Gurven2016; Wiessner, Reference Wiessner2002). Evidence from both small- and large-scale societies shows that a densification of trade alliances can effectively prevent intersociety and interstate war (Grueter & White, Reference Grueter and White2014; Jackson & Nei, Reference Jackson and Nei2015). Ecological interdependence may also explain intergroup tolerance in some primates, in particular those with multilevel societies. When critical resources across neighbouring ranges are heterogeneously distributed, gains to exclusive access could be reduced. The resulting relaxation in intergroup relations provides scope for intergroup tolerance (Jaeggi, Boose, White, & Gurven, Reference Jaeggi, Boose, White and Gurven2016; Robinson & Barker, Reference Robinson and Barker2017) and higher-level social structures such as multilevel systems, as long as these do not impose unsustainable costs on its constituents (Grueter & White, Reference Grueter and White2014; see also Macdonald & Johnson, Reference Macdonald and Johnson2015).
In humans, reciprocal exogamy, whereby individual dispersal for marriage suppresses latent violent tendencies and perpetuates alliances based on consanguineal and affinal kinship between groups, has been argued to be a strong determinant of nonwarring relations in small-scale societies (Chapais, Reference Chapais2008; Lévi-Strauss, Reference Lévi-Strauss1949; Rodseth et al., Reference Rodseth, Wrangham, Harrigan and Smuts1991; but see Grueter & White, Reference Grueter and White2014; Kang, Reference Kang1979). In primates, the buildup of kin networks encompassing multiple units and the opportunity for members of different units to interact regularly may also foster between-group tolerance. Shared descendance resulting from localized dispersal, social viscosity, and extragroup matings may be the source of reduced aggression between neighbouring groups and facilitate the formation of affiliative intergroup associations (Mirville et al., Reference Mirville, Ridley, Samedi, Vecellio, Ndagijimana, Stoinski and Grueter2018; Reichard & Sommer, Reference Reichard and Sommer1997; Rodrigues, Barker, & Robinson, Reference Rodrigues, Barker and Robinson2023; Snyder-Mackler, Alberts, & Bergman, Reference Snyder-Mackler, Alberts and Bergman2014; see also Camerlenghi et al., Reference Camerlenghi, McQueen, Delhey, Cook, Kingma, Farine and Peters2022).
A comparison between nonhuman primates and humans will help us understand if the prevalence of peace in human intergroup encounters can be explained with the canonical factors proposed for primates. It does seem that some of the same drivers can generate and stabilize between-group peace in humans and primates (i.e., mutual access to resources in ecologically complementary environments, collective group defence, and “exogamy”) although these take more elaborate forms in humans. However, what is unique to human peace systems is a cultural overlay that prescribes the adherence to peace norms (target article). This narrower definition of peace does not apply to nonhuman multilevel societies. However, if we use the less restrictive definition of tolerance and motivation to interact across group boundaries, then peace is present in nonhuman multilevel societies, albeit in more embryonic forms.
Acknowledgments
The author thanks Ettore Camerlenghi and Anne Pisor for feedback.
Financial support
This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
Competing interest
None.