Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-03T02:29:16.145Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Environments organize the verbal brain

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 December 2014

A. Charles Catania*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, Baltimore County (UMBC), Baltimore, MD 21250. [email protected]

Abstract

FOXP2 expression in the evolution of language derives from its role in allowing vocal articulation that is sensitive to its consequences. The discrete verbal discourse it allows must have evolved recently relative to affective features of vocal behavior such as tone of voice. Because all organ systems must have evolved in the service of behavior, attention is given to ways in which environments may have driven brain organization.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Catania, A. C. (1990) What good is five percent of a language competence? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13:729–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catania, A. C. (1995) Single words, multiple words, and the functions of language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18:184–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catania, A. C. (2001) Three varieties of selection and their implications for the origins of language. In: Language evolution: Biological, linguistic and philosophical perspectives, ed. Györi, G., pp. 5571. Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Catania, A. C. (2003) Why behavior should matter to linguists. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26:670–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catania, A. C. (2008) Brain and behavior: Which way does the shaping go? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31:516–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catania, A. C. (2009) Language evolution: Two tracks are not enough. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32:451–52.Google Scholar
Catania, A. C. (2013a) Learning, 5th edition. Sloan.Google Scholar
Catania, A. C. (2013b) A natural science of behavior. Review of General Psychology 17:133–39.Google Scholar
Catania, A. C. & Cerutti, D. (1986) Some nonverbal properties of verbal behavior. In: Analysis and integration of behavioral units, ed. Thompson, T. & Zeiler, M. D., pp. 185211. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lane, H. (1961) Operant control of vocalizing in the chicken. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 4:171–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madden, G. J., ed. (2012) APA handbook of behavior analysis. American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Provine, R. R. (2000) Laughter: A scientific investigation. Viking/Penguin Press.Google Scholar
Provine, R. R. (2012) Curious behavior: Yawning, laughing, hiccupping, and beyond. Belknap/Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Risley, T. R. (1977) The development and maintenance of language: An operant model. In: New developments in behavioral research, ed. Etzel, B. C., LeBlanc, J. M. & Baer, D. M., pp. 81101. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sacks, O. (1985) The president's speech. In: The man who mistook his wife for a hat and other clinical tales, by Sacks, O., pp. 8085. Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Schneider, S. M. (2012) The science of consequences. Prometheus.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1986) The evolution of verbal behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 45:115–22.Google Scholar
Verplanck, W. S. (2000) Glossary/thesaurus of behavioral terms (CD-ROM). CMS Software.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M. (1996) Phonological development: The origins of language in the child. Blackwell.Google Scholar