Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-03T02:56:44.977Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Beyond cry and laugh: Toward a multilevel model of language production

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 December 2014

Marc H. Bornstein
Affiliation:
Child and Family Research, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, Rockledge I, Bethesda, MD 20892-7971, USA. [email protected]://www.cfr.nichd.nih.gov/index.html
Gianluca Esposito
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Cognitive Sciences, University of Trento, Trento, 38068, Italy. [email protected]://polorovereto.unitn.it/~esposito/ Division of Psychology, Nanyang Technological University, 639798, Singapore.

Abstract

Language production is a multilevel phenomenon, and human capacities to communicate vocally progress from early forms, based on projections of motor cortex to brainstem nuclei, to complex elaborations, mediated by high-order cognition and fostered by socially mediated feedback.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bornstein, M. H., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Haynes, O. M. (1999) First words in the second year: Continuity, stability, and models of concurrent and predictive correspondence in vocabulary and verbal responsiveness across age and context. Infant Behavior and Development 22:6585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Esposito, G., del Carmen Rostagno, M., Venuti, P., Haltigan, J. D. & Messinger, D. S. (2013) Brief Report: Atypical expression of distress during the separation phase of the strange situation procedure in infant siblings at high risk for ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 44(4):975–80.Google Scholar
Esposito, G. & Venuti, P. (2009) Comparative analysis of crying in children with autism, developmental delays, and typical development. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities 24(4):240–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hage, S. R. & Nieder, A. (2013) Single neurons in monkey prefrontal cortex encode volitional initiation of vocalizations. Nature Communications 4:2409.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Tsao, F. M. & Liu, H. M. (2003) Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 100(15):9096–101.Google Scholar
Roseberry, S., Hirsh-Pasek, K. & Golinkoff, R. M. (2014) Skype me! Socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language. Child Development 85(3):956–70.Google Scholar
Sheinkopf, S. J., Iverson, J. M., Rinaldi, M. L. & Lester, B. M. (2012) Atypical cry acoustics in 6-month-old infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research 5(5):331–39.Google Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., Baumwell, L. & Damast, A. M. (1996) Responsive parenting in the second year: Specific influences on children's language and play. Early Development and Parenting 5:173–83.3.0.CO;2-V>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Venuti, P., Caria, A., Esposito, G., De Pisapia, N., Bornstein, M. H. & de Falco, S. (2012) Differential brain responses to cries of infants with autistic disorder and typical development: An fMRI study. Research in Developmental Disabilities 33(6):2255–64.Google Scholar