No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Adopt process-oriented models (if they're more useful)
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 31 January 2025
Abstract
Though we see the potential for benefits from the development of process-oriented approaches, we argue that it falls prey to many of the same critiques raised about the existing construct level of analysis. The construct-level approach will likely dominate motivation research until we develop computational models that are not only accurate, but also broadly usable.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Ackerman, R. (2014). The diminishing criterion model for metacognitive regulation of time investment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(3), 1349–1368. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035098CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, J. R. (2002). Spanning seven orders of magnitude: A challenge for cognitive modeling. Cognitive Science, 26(1), 85–112. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2601_3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aubé, M. (1997). Toward computational models of motivation: A much needed foundation for social sciences and education. Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Learning, 8(1), 43–75.Google Scholar
Bjork, R. A. (1973). Why mathematical models? American Psychologist, 28(5), 426–433. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0034623CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1963). A model of school learning. Teachers College Record, 64(8), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1177/0161468163064008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97(1), 19–35. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.97.1.19CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donoghue, G. M., & Horvath, J. C. (2016). Translating neuroscience, psychology and education: An abstracted conceptual framework for the learning sciences. Cogent Education, 3(1), 1267422. https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2016.1267422CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elder-Vass, D. (2022). Pragmatism, critical realism and the study of value. Journal of Critical Realism, 21(3), 261–287. https://doi.org/10.1080/14767430.2022.2049088CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guest, O., & Martin, A. E. (2021). How computational modeling can force theory building in psychological science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(4), 789–802. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620970585CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harnischfeger, A., & Wiley, D. E. (1978). Conceptual issues in models of school learning. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 10(3), 215–231. https://doi.org/10.1080/0022027780100304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (1907/2001). What pragmatism means. In Delbanco, A. (Ed.), Writing new England (pp. 80–93). Harvard University Press. https://doi.org/10.4159/harvard.9780674335486.c18.Google Scholar
Schuetze, B. A. (2024). A computational model of school achievement. Educational Psychology Review, 36, 18. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-024-09853-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thiede, K. W., & Dunlosky, J. (1999). Toward a general model of self-regulated study: An analysis of selection of items for study and self-paced study time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(4), 1024–1037. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.25.4.1024Google Scholar
van Rooij, I., & Blokpoel, M. (2020). Formalizing verbal theories: A tutorial by dialogue. Social Psychology, 51(5), 285–298. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000428CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yan, V. X., Sana, F., & Carvalho, P. F. (2024). No simple solutions to complex problems: Cognitive science principles can guide but not prescribe educational decisions. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11(1), 59–66. https://doi.org/10.1177/23727322231218906CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Target article
A critique of motivation constructs to explain higher-order behavior: We should unpack the black box
Related commentaries (25)
Adopt process-oriented models (if they're more useful)
Almost, but not quite there: Research into the emergence of higher-order motivated behavior should fully embrace the dynamic systems approach
Beyond reductionism: Understanding motivational energization requires higher-order constructs
Connecting theories of personality dynamics and mental computational processes
Definitional devils and detail: On identifying motivation as an animating dynamic
Don't throw motivation out with the black box: The value of a good theory revisited
Endogenous reward is a bridge between social/cognitive and behavioral models of choice
Expectancy value theory's contribution to unpacking the black box of motivation
Exploring novelty to unpack the black-box of motivation
Higher-order motivational constructs as personal-level fictions: A solution in search of a problem
Human motivation is organized hierarchically, from proximal (means) to ultimate (ends)
It's bigger on the inside: mapping the black box of motivation
Mental computational processes have always been an integral part of motivation science
Motivation needs cognition but is not just about cognition
Motivational constructs: Real, causally powerful, not psychologically constructed
Motivational whack-a-mole: Foundational boxes cannot be unpacked
Needed: Clear definition and hierarchical integration of motivation constructs
Postcard from inside the black box
Predictive processing: Shedding light on the computational processes underlying motivated behavior
Resurrecting the “black-box” conundrum
The ins and outs of unpacking the black box: Understanding motivation using a multi-level approach
The role of metacognitive feelings in motivation
The unboxing has already begun: One motivation construct at a time
There's no such thing as a free lunch: A computational perspective on the costs of motivation
When unpacking the black box of motivation invites three forms of reductionism
Author response
Response to the critiques (and encouragements) on our critique of motivation constructs