Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-10-28T05:13:01.227Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Moral disciplining: The cognitive and evolutionary foundations of puritanical morality

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 September 2022

Léo Fitouchi
Affiliation:
Département d’études cognitives, Institut Jean Nicod, ENS, EHESS, PSL University, CNRS, Paris, France. [email protected]; https://sites.google.com/view/leofitouchi [email protected]; http://jb.homepage.free.fr/ [email protected]; https://nicolasbaumards.org/
Jean-Baptiste André
Affiliation:
Département d’études cognitives, Institut Jean Nicod, ENS, EHESS, PSL University, CNRS, Paris, France. [email protected]; https://sites.google.com/view/leofitouchi [email protected]; http://jb.homepage.free.fr/ [email protected]; https://nicolasbaumards.org/
Nicolas Baumard
Affiliation:
Département d’études cognitives, Institut Jean Nicod, ENS, EHESS, PSL University, CNRS, Paris, France. [email protected]; https://sites.google.com/view/leofitouchi [email protected]; http://jb.homepage.free.fr/ [email protected]; https://nicolasbaumards.org/
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Why do many societies moralize apparently harmless pleasures, such as lust, gluttony, alcohol, drugs, and even music and dance? Why do they erect temperance, asceticism, sobriety, modesty, and piety as cardinal moral virtues? According to existing theories, this puritanical morality cannot be reduced to concerns for harm and fairness: It must emerge from cognitive systems that did not evolve for cooperation (e.g., disgust-based “purity” concerns). Here, we argue that, despite appearances, puritanical morality is no exception to the cooperative function of moral cognition. It emerges in response to a key feature of cooperation, namely that cooperation is (ultimately) a long-term strategy, requiring (proximately) the self-control of appetites for immediate gratification. Puritanical moralizations condemn behaviors which, although inherently harmless, are perceived as indirectly facilitating uncooperative behaviors, by impairing the self-control required to refrain from cheating. Drinking, drugs, immodest clothing, and unruly music and dance are condemned as stimulating short-term impulses, thus facilitating uncooperative behaviors (e.g., violence, adultery, free-riding). Overindulgence in harmless bodily pleasures (e.g., masturbation, gluttony) is perceived as making people slave to their urges, thus altering abilities to resist future antisocial temptations. Daily self-discipline, ascetic temperance, and pious ritual observance are perceived as cultivating the self-control required to honor prosocial obligations. We review psychological, historical, and ethnographic evidence supporting this account. We use this theory to explain the fall of puritanism in western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic (WEIRD) societies, and discuss the cultural evolution of puritanical norms. Explaining puritanical norms does not require adding mechanisms unrelated to cooperation in our models of the moral mind.

Type
Target Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. Introduction

Brueghel's painting, The Fight Between Carnival and Lent (1559), contrasts the vices and virtues of puritanical moral standards. On one side of the painting, “men and women dance, they crowd into a tavern, get drunk, play games, watch street theatre, ignore beggars, sneak inside for sex, play cruel tricks on others, gamble, eat, join masked processions, and make music; in short, they are unruly, profane, sexually promiscuous, spontaneous, and concerned with immediate gratification” (Martin, Reference Martin2009, p. 9). On the other side of the painting, other “men and women work, attend church, give alms to beggars and the poor; in short they are orderly, sober, devout, and disciplined” (Martin, Reference Martin2009, pp. 9–10). This side represents the values of “decency, diligence, gravity, modesty, orderliness, prudence, reason, self-control, sobriety, and thrift” (Burke, Reference Burke1978, p. 213).

In recent decades, moral psychologists have identified a related cluster of moral norms. They have noted that many human societies praise chastity, temperance, and piety; condemn the immoderate enjoyment of sensual pleasures; and disapprove of the lack of religious and ritual observance (Graham et al., Reference Graham, Haidt, Koleva, Motyl, Iyer, Wojcik and Ditto2013; Haidt, Reference Haidt2007, Reference Haidt2012; Haidt & Joseph, Reference Haidt and Joseph2007; Shweder, Mahapatra, & Miller, Reference Shweder, Mahapatra, Miller, Kagan and Lamb1987, Reference Shweder, Much, Mahapatra and Park1997). This constellation of moral values gained popularity in psychological and evolutionary approaches to morality as part of the so-called “purity” moral concerns (Graham et al., Reference Graham, Haidt, Koleva, Motyl, Iyer, Wojcik and Ditto2013; Haidt, Reference Haidt2007, Reference Haidt2012; Haidt & Joseph, Reference Haidt and Joseph2004, Reference Haidt and Joseph2007). The purity category, however, is a murky concept with no clear definition (Crone, Reference Crone2022; Gray, DiMaggio, Schein, & Kachanoff, Reference Gray, DiMaggio, Schein and Kachanoff2022; Kollareth, Brownell, Duran, & Russell, Reference Kollareth, Brownell, Duran and Russell2022). Reviewing 158 papers of the purity literature, Gray et al. (Reference Gray, DiMaggio, Schein and Kachanoff2022) show that moral psychologists understand purity in about nine different ways, often mixing distinct meanings in their definitions and operationalizations. To avoid confusions, we here use the term puritanical morality (or “puritanism”) to refer to the ascetic, austere moralization of apparently victimless pleasures that humans crave for, such as eating, drinking, feasting, dancing, gambling, taking drugs, dressing indecently, having sex, or masturbating.

Puritanical morality, more precisely, comprises the following constellation of moral norms:

  1. (1) A moral condemnation of bodily pleasures. Christian morality, for example, condemns excessive indulgence in food and sex as the deadly sins of gluttony and lust (Adamson, Reference Adamson2004; Dabhoiwala, Reference Dabhoiwala2012; Hill, Reference Hill2011). Psychologists have observed that many participants moralize unrestrained or unhealthy eating (Fitouchi, André, Baumard, & Nettle, Reference Fitouchi, André, Baumard and Nettle2022; Mooijman et al., Reference Mooijman, Meindl, Oyserman, Monterosso, Dehghani, Doris and Graham2018; Ringel & Ditto, Reference Ringel and Ditto2019; Steim & Nemeroff, Reference Steim and Nemeroff1995), as well as sexual indulgences such as masturbation or oral sex (Fitouchi et al., Reference Fitouchi, André, Baumard and Nettle2022; Haidt & Hersh, Reference Haidt and Hersh2001; Haidt, Koller, & Dias, Reference Haidt, Koller and Dias1993; Schein, Ritter, & Gray, Reference Schein, Ritter and Gray2016).

  2. (2) A strong valorization of temperance and self-discipline. In various countries, a substantial share of participants moralize lack of self-control (Mooijman et al., Reference Mooijman, Meindl, Oyserman, Monterosso, Dehghani, Doris and Graham2018), general hedonism (Saroglou & Craninx, Reference Saroglou and Craninx2021), and reluctance to needless effort (Celniker et al., Reference Celniker, Gregory, Koo, Piff, Ditto and Shariff2023; Tierney et al., Reference Tierney, Hardy, Ebersole, Viganola, Clemente, Gordon and Uhlmann2021), whereas similar values are preached across world religions (see sect. 1.1). In medieval and early modern Western societies, for instance,

self-discipline in all spheres of life was prized as the ultimate mark of civilization … Only beasts and savages gave “unrestrained liberty” to “the cravings of nature” – civilized Christians were rather “to bring under the flesh; bring nature under the government of reason, and, in short bring the body under the command of the soul.” The mental and physical government of fleshly appetites was the very foundation of the whole culture of discipline. (Dabhoiwala, Reference Dabhoiwala2012, pp. 26–27)

  1. (3) Condemnations of entertainments such as alcohol, drug use, gambling, and certain forms of music and dances. These are widespread both in cross-national surveys (Lugo, Cooperman, Bell, O'Connell, & Stencel, Reference Lugo, Cooperman, Bell, O'Connell and Stencel2013; Poushter, Reference Poushter2014; Weeden & Kurzban, Reference Weeden and Kurzban2013), and in the explicit moral codes of various societies (e.g., Buddhism: Najjar, Young, Leasure, Henderson, & Neighbors, Reference Najjar, Young, Leasure, Henderson and Neighbors2016; Sterckx, Reference Sterckx2005, pp. 223–224; Hinduism: Doniger, Reference Doniger2014, pp. 263–270; traditional Europe: Burke, Reference Burke1978; Martin, Reference Martin2009; Partridge & Moberg, Reference Partridge and Moberg2017; Wagner, Reference Wagner1997; Arab-Muslim societies: Michalak & Trocki, Reference Michalak and Trocki2006; Otterbeck & Ackfeldt, Reference Otterbeck and Ackfeldt2012).

  2. (4) Moral demands of modesty, which regulate decency in clothing, speech, and attitudes. In traditional Arab-Muslim societies, for example, when entering the public sphere, women must be veiled, lower their gaze, and avoid body ornaments (Antoun, Reference Antoun1968; Beckmann, Reference Beckmann2010; Mernissi, Reference Mernissi2011), whereas similar restrictions appear in other puritanical cultures (e.g., Puritans' austere clothing: Bremer, Reference Bremer2009; Hindu India: Stephens, Reference Stephens1972, p. 4; Jewish Tznihut dress: Andrews, Reference Andrews2010; ancient Christian veiling: Tariq, Reference Tariq2014).

  3. (5) Moral prescriptions of a pious lifestyle, requiring the diligent observance of religious rituals, such as fasting, daily prayers, meditations, effortful pilgrimages, or dietary restrictions (see sect. 1.1).

This definition of puritanism comprises many core elements of purity, which is often defined by moral psychologists as including moralizations of lust, gluttony (Haidt & Graham, Reference Haidt and Graham2007; Mcadams et al., Reference Mcadams, Albaugh, Farber, Daniels, Logan and Olson2008), clothing, prayer, meditation, temperance, self-control (Graham, Haidt, & Nosek, Reference Graham, Haidt and Nosek2009; Horberg, Oveis, Keltner, & Cohen, Reference Horberg, Oveis, Keltner and Cohen2009; Koleva, Graham, Iyer, Ditto, & Haidt, Reference Koleva, Graham, Iyer, Ditto and Haidt2012), drugs, alcohol, and certain kinds of music (Helzer & Pizarro, Reference Helzer and Pizarro2011; Horberg et al., Reference Horberg, Oveis, Keltner and Cohen2009) – all of which are central to puritanism as defined here. However, because of its heterogeneity (Kollareth et al., Reference Kollareth, Brownell, Duran and Russell2022), the purity category is broader than puritanism as we define it. In particular, purity also includes concerns for physical contamination, often operationalized by weird or abnormal behaviors (Gray & Keeney, Reference Gray and Keeney2015; Kupfer, Inbar, & Tybur, Reference Kupfer, Inbar and Tybur2020), such as pouring urine on oneself (Chakroff, Russell, Piazza, & Young, Reference Chakroff, Russell, Piazza and Young2017), touching poop barehanded (Dungan, Chakroff, & Young, Reference Dungan, Chakroff and Young2017), or eating pizza off a dead body (Clifford, Iyengar, Cabeza, & Sinnott-Armstrong, Reference Clifford, Iyengar, Cabeza and Sinnott-Armstrong2015). We do not include this subclass of purity in our definition of puritanism because, as researchers have noted, these contamination-related concerns appear distinct from those labeled here as puritanical (see Crone, Reference Crone2022; though see sect. 6.1.1).

Why, then, do many societies develop puritanical values? Answering this question requires resolving two puzzles.

1.1. The puzzle of association

First, puritanism consists of apparently heterogenous moral concerns, governing domains as various as sex, food, clothing, self-discipline, entertainments, and ritual observance. Yet despite their heterogeneity, these moral concerns tend to co-occur and cohere in the most culturally successful moral traditions, which cover almost 80% of the world's population (Hackett & McClendon, Reference Hackett and McClendon2017) – from Hinduism (Doniger, Reference Doniger2014, pp. 363–370; Hatcher, Reference Hatcher2017; Menon, Reference Menon2013; Nag, Reference Nag1972, p. 236; Stephens, Reference Stephens1972, p. 4; Vatuk & Vatuk, Reference Vatuk and Vatuk1967, pp. 108–112) to Christianity (Bremer, Reference Bremer2009; Burke, Reference Burke1978; Dabhoiwala, Reference Dabhoiwala2012; Gaca, Reference Gaca2003; Gorski, Reference Gorski2003; Partridge & Moberg, Reference Partridge and Moberg2017; Spiegel, Reference Spiegel2020; Wagner, Reference Wagner1997) to Buddhism (Harvey, Reference Harvey2000; Keown, Reference Keown2003, pp. 78, 93; Mann, Reference Mann2011; Sterckx, Reference Sterckx2005, pp. 223–224; Stunkard, LaFleur, & Wadden, Reference Stunkard, LaFleur and Wadden1998) to Chinese religions (Brokaw, Reference Brokaw2014; Csikszentmihalyi, Reference Csikszentmihalyi, Lagerwey and Kalinowski2009; Slingerland, Reference Slingerland2014, p. 76; Suiming, Reference Suiming1998; Tiwald, Reference Tiwald and Zalta2020; Yü, Reference Yü2021, pp. 36–40, 82, 216) to Arab-Muslim societies (Garden, Reference Garden2014, pp. 83, 89, 76; Mernissi, Reference Mernissi2011; Michalak & Trocki, Reference Michalak and Trocki2006; Otterbeck & Ackfeldt, Reference Otterbeck and Ackfeldt2012, pp. 231–233; Rehman, Reference Rehman2019) and ancient Greco-Roman spiritualities (Gaca, Reference Gaca2003; Langlands, Reference Langlands2006).

This association, suggested by qualitative data, is consistent with psychological evidence. Studies find that moralizations of gluttony, sexual indulgences, lack of self-control, intoxicant use, and certain types of music, are intercorrelated (Fitouchi et al., Reference Fitouchi, André, Baumard and Nettle2022; Kurzban, Dukes, & Weeden, Reference Kurzban, Dukes and Weeden2010; Lynxwiler & Gay, Reference Lynxwiler and Gay2000; Mooijman et al., Reference Mooijman, Meindl, Oyserman, Monterosso, Dehghani, Doris and Graham2018; Quintelier, Ishii, Weeden, Kurzban, & Braeckman, Reference Quintelier, Ishii, Weeden, Kurzban and Braeckman2013; Steim & Nemeroff, Reference Steim and Nemeroff1995; Tierney et al., Reference Tierney, Hardy, Ebersole, Viganola, Clemente, Gordon and Uhlmann2021). Condemnations of lack of self-control, intoxicant use, hedonism, sexual indulgences, and immodesty are all associated with religiosity (Grubbs, Exline, Pargament, Hook, & Carlisle, Reference Grubbs, Exline, Pargament, Hook and Carlisle2015; Jacquet et al., Reference Jacquet, Pazhoohi, Findling, Mell, Chevallier and Baumard2021; Mooijman et al., Reference Mooijman, Meindl, Oyserman, Monterosso, Dehghani, Doris and Graham2018; Moon, Wongsomboon, & Sevi, Reference Moon, Wongsomboon and Sevi2021; Najjar et al., Reference Najjar, Young, Leasure, Henderson and Neighbors2016; Saroglou & Craninx, Reference Saroglou and Craninx2021; Stylianou, Reference Stylianou2004; Weeden & Kurzban, Reference Weeden and Kurzban2013), which is itself related, across countries, to the moralization of piety (Abrams, Jackson, Vonasch, & Gray, Reference Abrams, Jackson, Vonasch and Gray2020; Tamir, Connaughton, & Salazar, Reference Tamir, Connaughton and Salazar2020). At a deeper level, experimental evidence indicates that different puritanical norms are intuitively intertwined in people's mind. Experimental studies of “implicit puritanism” across cultures (N > 6,000) show that Indian, American, Australian, and English participants all implicitly associate violation of one puritanical norm (work-related self-discipline) with violations of another puritanical norm (sexual restraint), by misremembering individuals described as violating one norm as also violating the other (Tierney et al., Reference Tierney, Hardy, Ebersole, Viganola, Clemente, Gordon and Uhlmann2021).

Hence the first puzzle of puritanism: Why do moralizations of bodily pleasures, self-discipline, entertainments, clothing, and piety often develop in concert?

1.2. The puzzle of morality without cooperation

The second puzzle of puritanical morality concerns its relation to cooperation. Most evolutionary theories of morality share the ultimate hypothesis that moral cognition is an adaptation to the challenges of cooperation recurrent in human social life (Alexander, Reference Alexander1987; André, Fitouchi, Debove, & Baumard, Reference André, Fitouchi, Debove and Baumard2022; Baumard, André, & Sperber, Reference Baumard, André and Sperber2013; Boehm, Reference Boehm2012; Curry, Reference Curry, Shackelford and Hansen2016; Haidt, Reference Haidt2012; Stanford, Reference Stanford2018; Tomasello, Reference Tomasello2020). This hypothesis explains the vast majority of moral intuitions and norms found across human societies, such as condemnations of theft, murder, violence, unfairness, and the promotion of justice, loyalty, reciprocity, or respect for property and authority (Baumard, Reference Baumard2016; Boehm, Reference Boehm2012; Curry, Jones Chesters, & Van Lissa, Reference Curry, Jones Chesters and Van Lissa2019a; Hofmann, Wisneski, Brandt, & Skitka, Reference Hofmann, Wisneski, Brandt and Skitka2014; Purzycki et al., Reference Purzycki, Pisor, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, Henrich and Xygalatas2018).

In this context, puritanical morality appears as an “odd corner of moral life” (Haidt & Joseph, Reference Haidt and Joseph2004, p. 60). If moral cognition evolved for cooperation, moral condemnations should only target cheating behaviors, such as lying, theft, free-riding, betrayal of coalition partners, or suffering inflicted on innocent people. Yet the behaviors typically moralized by puritanical values are not, at first sight, clearly related to cooperation. For example, in medieval Christianity (Dabhoiwala, Reference Dabhoiwala2012), Neo-Confucian China (Suiming, Reference Suiming1998, p. 16), or Victorian England (Seidman, Reference Seidman1990), indulgence in sexual pleasure is condemned not only when it amounts to cheating other people in a cooperative interaction, such as in adultery,Footnote 1 but also in a range of victimless manifestations, such as in masturbation, or too frequent or licentious sex within marriage:

A huge body of teaching grew up in support of the notion that bodily desire was inherently shameful and sinful … Even in marriage, men and women had to be constantly on their guard against sinning through immoderate, unchaste, or unprocreative sex. (Dabhoiwala, Reference Dabhoiwala2012, pp. 7–8)

In moral psychology, famous vignette studies have echoed this apparent harmlessness of sexual sins, finding that American and Brazilian participants condemn “purity violations,” such as masturbating in a chicken carcass, even though these actions do not in themselves cause any harm to other individuals (Haidt et al., Reference Haidt, Koller and Dias1993; Haidt & Hersh, Reference Haidt and Hersh2001; Horberg et al., Reference Horberg, Oveis, Keltner and Cohen2009).

Similarly, puritanical values condemn immoderate indulgence in food pleasure, even when gluttony doesn't involve failing in one's duty to share food, or to respect others' property (e.g., medieval Christianity: Adamson, Reference Adamson2004; Hill, Reference Hill2011; India: Vatuk & Vatuk, Reference Vatuk and Vatuk1967; European antiquity: Coveney, Reference Coveney2006; Gaca, Reference Gaca2003). The Christian sin of gluttony, for example, condemns the failure to control the food appetite in itself – whether by eating too much, failing to wait for the proper time to eat, eating too eagerly, or craving foods that are too tasty (Adamson, Reference Adamson2004; Hill, Reference Hill2007, Reference Hill2011). Psychologists, too, observe that participants moralize inherently harmless eating practices, such as eating fatty rather than healthy foods (Fitouchi et al., Reference Fitouchi, André, Baumard and Nettle2022; Mooijman et al., Reference Mooijman, Meindl, Oyserman, Monterosso, Dehghani, Doris and Graham2018; Oakes & Slotterback, Reference Oakes and Slotterback2004; Steim & Nemeroff, Reference Steim and Nemeroff1995).

Besides food and sex, puritanical values prescribe industrious self-discipline even when idleness would be harmless and effort unproductive (Indian, American, Australian, and English participants: Tierney et al., Reference Tierney, Hardy, Ebersole, Viganola, Clemente, Gordon and Uhlmann2021; Uhlmann, Poehlman, Tannenbaum, & Bargh, Reference Uhlmann, Poehlman, Tannenbaum and Bargh2011; American, French, and South-Korean participants: Celniker et al., Reference Celniker, Gregory, Koo, Piff, Ditto and Shariff2023; early modern China: Yü, Reference Yü2021). They condemn alcohol, drugs, and gambling, when the latter are widely considered “victimless crimes” (Boyd & Richerson, Reference Boyd, Richerson, Gigerenzer and Selten2001; Ellis, Reference Ellis1988; Stylianou, Reference Stylianou2010). And their strict regulations of mundane activities, such as music, dance, or clothing appear, to our modern eyes, as needlessly austere restrictions (see Moon et al., Reference Moon, Wongsomboon and Sevi2021).

Hence the second puzzle of puritanism: If the function of morality is cooperation – as the prominence of cooperative norms in the human moral landscape suggests (Curry, Mullins, & Whitehouse, Reference Curry, Mullins and Whitehouse2019b; Hofmann et al., Reference Hofmann, Wisneski, Brandt and Skitka2014; Purzycki et al., Reference Purzycki, Pisor, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, Henrich and Xygalatas2018) – why do humans moralize victimless lifestyle choices with respect to sex, food, drinking, clothing, self-discipline, and ritual observance?

This apparent disconnect between puritanical morality and cooperation has sparked intense debates about the cognitive architecture of morality, opposing unitary models of moral cognition to theories dividing morality into distinct cognitive domains (Beal, Reference Beal2020; Graham et al., Reference Graham, Haidt, Koleva, Motyl, Iyer, Wojcik and Ditto2013; Schein & Gray, Reference Schein and Gray2018). Unitary theories argue that all moral judgments are produced by a single, functionally unified cognitive mechanism. In particular, the theory of dyadic morality maintains that all moral judgments stem from perceptions of dyadic harm – that is, from perceptions that an “agent” intentionally causes suffering to a “patient” (Gray, Waytz, & Young, Reference Gray, Waytz and Young2012, Reference Gray, Schein and Ward2014; Schein & Gray, Reference Schein and Gray2015, Reference Schein and Gray2018). Other unitary theories argue that all moral judgments are outputs of fairness computations, tracking violations of mutual benefit between cooperative partners (André et al., Reference André, Fitouchi, Debove and Baumard2022; Baumard, Reference Baumard2016; Fitouchi, André, & Baumard, Reference Fitouchi, André, Baumard, Al-Shawaf and Shackelfordin press).Footnote 2 By contrast, theories based on distinct cognitive domains – such as moral foundations theory – maintain that moral cognition is composed of multiple, functionally distinct, domain-specific mechanisms, some of which track stimuli unrelated to harm or fairness (Graham et al., Reference Graham, Haidt, Koleva, Motyl, Iyer, Wojcik and Ditto2013). In these debates, purity moralizations have appeared as critical arguments against unitary theories. If harmless behaviors can be morally condemned, scholars have argued, there must be in the mind some mechanisms that generate moral judgments despite not functioning for cooperation (Haidt, Reference Haidt2012). Accordingly, puritanical morality has so far been explained by psychological mechanisms unrelated to cooperation, such as pathogen avoidance and conflicts of reproductive interests (sect. 2).

1.3. The moral disciplining theory of puritanism

Here, we propose that puritanical morality does target cooperation, and is reducible to concerns for harm or fairness. Psychologists and evolutionary scientists have long noted that reciprocal and reputation-based cooperation require self-control – the ability to delay gratification, by resisting temptations of immediate rewards (Ainslie, Reference Ainslie2013; Axelrod, Reference Axelrod1984; Hofmann, Meindl, Mooijman, & Graham, Reference Hofmann, Meindl, Mooijman and Graham2018; Manrique et al., Reference Manrique, Zeidler, Roberts, Barclay, Walker, Samu and Raihani2021; Stevens, Cushman, & Hauser, Reference Stevens, Cushman and Hauser2005). Historians and social scientists, meanwhile, have repeatedly stressed that puritanical groups seem obsessed with the cultivation of self-control (Csikszentmihalyi, Reference Csikszentmihalyi, Lagerwey and Kalinowski2009; Dabhoiwala, Reference Dabhoiwala2012; Eisner, Reference Eisner2014; Gaca, Reference Gaca2003; Gorski, Reference Gorski2003; Luttmer, Reference Luttmer2000; Menon, Reference Menon2013; Oestreich, Oestreich, & Koenigsberger, Reference Oestreich, Oestreich and Koenigsberger1982; Rehman, Reference Rehman2019; Seidman, Reference Seidman1990; Spiegel, Reference Spiegel2020; Walzer, Reference Walzer1963, Reference Walzer1982; Weber, Reference Weber1968; Yü, Reference Yü2021). Connecting these insights, we propose that puritanism develops from folk-psychological beliefs that restraining indulgence in victimless pleasures would improve people's self-control, thus facilitating cooperative behaviors.

Our argument goes as follows. People perceive that meeting prosocial obligations often requires self-control (sect. 3.1). Refraining from violent behaviors, they perceive, sometimes requires resisting aggressive impulses. Abstaining from adultery sometimes requires resisting sexual temptations. In collective work, doing one's fair share of effort can require overcoming lazy desires. Meanwhile, people perceive, not only that cooperation requires self-control, but also that some behaviors alter self-control (sect. 3.2). They perceive that alcohol and drugs make people impulsive, precipitating antisocial behaviors – such as adultery, violence, or lazy free-riding – by impeding abilities to resist impulses. They see carnal pleasures – lust, gluttony, intoxicants, gambling – as dangerously addictive behaviors, overindulgence in which would make people slave to their urges, and unable to resist uncooperative temptations. By contrast, daily self-discipline, ascetic temperance, and regular ritual observance are perceived as improving people's self-control, thus ensuring that they remain peaceful neighbors, faithful husbands and wives, industrious workers, responsible family members, or conscientious followers. Thus, although inherently harmless, hedonistic behaviors are perceived as indirectly socially harmful. As such, they are naturally tagged as morally wrong by cognitive systems biologically evolved to detect and condemn uncooperative behaviors or threats to cooperation (sect. 3.3). Puritanical moral judgments are thus generated by the same, cooperation-based cognitive systems producing the rest of human morality.

These intuitive psychological processes, in turn, shape the cultural evolution of puritanical norms (sect. 3.4). In environments where many people want to prevent perceived antisocial effects of hedonistic impulses, people gradually invent and refine cultural technologies they perceive as efficient for disciplining other individuals to ensure social order. These technologies of moral self-discipline include ascetic rituals, modest clothes, legal regulations of entertainment such as drinking and feasting, as well as mental techniques for the self-monitoring of impulses (see sect. 6.1.2). Our account, importantly, is agnostic as to whether puritanical norms are objectively effective in improving self-control and cooperation – it rather insists on people's perceptions that they are.

In the following, we first review and examine existing accounts of puritanical morality (sect. 2). Section 3 lays out the evolutionary and psychological foundations of the moral disciplining theory (MDT) of puritanism, and reviews evidence for its assumptions. We then derive predictions from this account, review current evidence supporting them, and outline avenues for further testing (sect. 4). We finally use this theory to explain the fall of puritanism in western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic (WEIRD) societies (sect. 5), and discuss extensions and outstanding questions for the study of puritanical values (sect. 6).

2. Existing accounts of puritanical morality

2.1. Moral foundations theory and disgust-based accounts

Moral foundations theory considers puritanical morality as an exception to the cooperative function of moral cognition (Graham et al., Reference Graham, Haidt, Koleva, Motyl, Iyer, Wojcik and Ditto2013; Haidt, Reference Haidt2012; Haidt & Joseph, Reference Haidt and Joseph2007). According to this framework, moral cognition is composed of several domain-specific cognitive systems, most which have evolved for cooperative adaptive challenges – including harm/care, fairness/reciprocity, loyalty/betrayal, and authority/respect. The last moral system, purity/sanctity, is instead proposed to function for the nonsocial challenge of pathogen avoidance, and to emerge from disgust at the proximate level (Haidt & Joseph, Reference Haidt and Joseph2007). People driven by carnal impulses rather than spiritual motivations would be detected by this disgust-related system as “impure” and “less than human,” and thus morally condemned (Haidt & Graham, Reference Haidt and Graham2007, p. 9). Although this theory is consistent with the frequent cultural depiction of pleasure-seeking behaviors as “impure,” it seems insufficient to explain puritanical morality, for at least two reasons.

First, it is unclear that violations of puritanical standards actually trigger disgust. Initial support for this hypothesis came from “purity violation” studies, in which participants find apparently harmless behaviors (e.g., masturbating in a chicken carcass) both disgusting and morally wrong (e.g., Haidt et al., Reference Haidt, Koller and Dias1993; Haidt & Hersh, Reference Haidt and Hersh2001; Horberg et al., Reference Horberg, Oveis, Keltner and Cohen2009; Rozin, Lowery, Imada, & Haidt, Reference Rozin, Lowery, Imada and Haidt1999). However, containing more pathogen cues than the typical targets of puritanical moralizations, such vignettes appear disconnected from real-world puritanical concerns. As such, they are likely to overestimate the extent to which real-world violations of puritanical standards evoke disgust. For example, in widely cited studies, sexual lust takes the form of masturbating in a dead chicken (Haidt et al., Reference Haidt, Koller and Dias1993; Horberg et al., Reference Horberg, Oveis, Keltner and Cohen2009) or corpse-sexing (Haidt & Hersh, Reference Haidt and Hersh2001). Gluttony becomes eating one's dead dog (Haidt et al., Reference Haidt, Koller and Dias1993), or rotten meat (Rozin et al., Reference Rozin, Lowery, Imada and Haidt1999). By contrast, historical and anthropological data make clear that puritanical societies are less morally obsessed by disgusting sexualities with dead animals than with the sin of lust as the unbridled craving for sexual pleasure (Dabhoiwala, Reference Dabhoiwala2012; Gaca, Reference Gaca2003; Le Goff, Reference Le Goff1984; Mernissi, Reference Mernissi2011; Seidman, Reference Seidman1990). For example, medieval Christian moralists (e.g., Augustine, fourth to fifth centuries, Aquinas, thirteenth century) regarded the peculiar problem of the sin of lust to be “the intensity of the pleasure it offers” (Sweeney, Reference Sweeney, Newhauser and Ridyard2012, p. 96), and its resulting “unparalleled power to overwhelm reason and human will” (Dabhoiwala, Reference Dabhoiwala2012, p. 8). In the same vein, the sin of gluttony condemns the immoderate indulgence in the pleasure of eating (Adamson, Reference Adamson2004; Bynum, Reference Bynum2000; Hill, Reference Hill2007, Reference Hill2011), not the eating of rotten aliments or animal corpses. In fact, pleasurable aliments (affording gluttony) are almost by definition aliments that are not disgusting. In line with these ideas, when participants are asked to themselves generate sinful or lustful scenarios, they overwhelmingly mention pathogen-free behaviors (e.g., stripping) (Gray & Keeney, Reference Gray and Keeney2015).

Aside from lust and gluttony, moralizations of intemperance, lack of self-discipline, and impiety also seem unrelated to disgust. These behaviors do not involve pathogen cues at all. Accordingly, maintaining that disgust explains their condemnation has required to argue that this emotion is triggered not only by pathogen cues, but also by “spiritually” impure behaviors, that “degrade” the elevated nature of the human soul or remind humans of their “animal nature” (Haidt & Graham, Reference Haidt and Graham2007; Rozin et al., Reference Rozin, Lowery, Imada and Haidt1999, Reference Rozin, Haidt, McCauley, Lewis, Haviland-Jones and Barrett2008; Rozin & Haidt, Reference Rozin and Haidt2013) – an often contested theory (Bloom, Reference Bloom2013; Royzman & Sabini, Reference Royzman and Sabini2001; Tybur, Lieberman, Kurzban, & DeScioli, Reference Tybur, Lieberman, Kurzban and DeScioli2013). As many researchers have noted, self-reports of being “disgusted” by “spiritual impurities” (Ritter, Preston, Salomon, & Relihan-Johnson, Reference Ritter, Preston, Salomon and Relihan-Johnson2016) do not reliably demonstrate that the cognitive system of disgust is actually activated. The lay meaning of the term “disgust” is difficult to disentangle from “anger” or “contempt” (Herz & Hinds, Reference Herz and Hinds2013; Nabi, Reference Nabi2002; Piazza, Landy, Chakroff, Young, & Wasserman, Reference Piazza, Landy, Chakroff, Young, Wasserman, Strohminger and Kumar2018), and people likely use the term metaphorically to communicate their disapproval (Armstrong, Wilbanks, Leong, & Hsu, Reference Armstrong, Wilbanks, Leong and Hsu2020; Bloom, Reference Bloom2004; Nabi, Reference Nabi2002; Royzman & Kurzban, Reference Royzman and Kurzban2011; Royzman & Sabini, Reference Royzman and Sabini2001). In line with this idea, pathogen-free violations of “spiritual purity” are not associated with the facial expression of disgust (Franchin, Geipel, Hadjichristidis, & Surian, Reference Franchin, Geipel, Hadjichristidis and Surian2019; Ritter et al., Reference Ritter, Preston, Salomon and Relihan-Johnson2016); do not elicit a disgust-related phenomenology (nausea, gagging, loss of appetite), nor action tendency (desire to move away) (Royzman, Atanasov, Landy, Parks, & Gepty, Reference Royzman, Atanasov, Landy, Parks and Gepty2014); and are not or negligibly associated with reporting being “grossed-out” (Kollareth et al., Reference Kollareth, Brownell, Duran and Russell2022; Kollareth & Russell, Reference Kollareth and Russell2019) – the lay term more aptly capturing the cognitively strict sense of disgust (Herz & Hinds, Reference Herz and Hinds2013; Nabi, Reference Nabi2002).

Second, disgust-based accounts of puritanism rely on the premise that simply perceiving an action as disgusting is sufficient to judge it immoral. As researchers have noted, however, many behaviors are disgusting without being immoral (Kayyal, Pochedly, McCarthy, & Russell, Reference Kayyal, Pochedly, McCarthy and Russell2015; Piazza et al., Reference Piazza, Landy, Chakroff, Young, Wasserman, Strohminger and Kumar2018; Pizarro, Inbar, & Helion, Reference Pizarro, Inbar and Helion2011; Schein et al., Reference Schein, Ritter and Gray2016). It seems, moreover, evolutionarily unclear why disgust should have acquired such a secondary moralizing function (Fitouchi et al., Reference Fitouchi, André, Baumard, Al-Shawaf and Shackelfordin press), and the experimental evidence seems overall to cast doubt on this possibility (see Piazza et al. [Reference Piazza, Landy, Chakroff, Young, Wasserman, Strohminger and Kumar2018], for an extensive review). In particular, a meta-analysis (Landy & Goodwin, Reference Landy and Goodwin2015), highly powered replications (Ghelfi et al., Reference Ghelfi, Christopherson, Urry, Lenne, Legate, Fischer and Sullivan2020; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Wortman, Cheung, Hein, Lucas, Donnellan and Narr2016), and recent studies (Jylkkä, Härkönen, & Hyönä, Reference Jylkkä, Härkönen and Hyönä2021) strongly suggest that feelings of disgust do not increase moral condemnation, nor cause moralization of otherwise morally neutral actions. Relatedly, correlations between disgust-sensitivity and condemnations of sex- and purity-related behaviors (Crawford, Inbar, & Maloney, Reference Crawford, Inbar and Maloney2014; Horberg et al., Reference Horberg, Oveis, Keltner and Cohen2009; Inbar, Pizarro, Knobe, & Bloom, Reference Inbar, Pizarro, Knobe and Bloom2009) have been found to disappear when perceptions of harm are controlled for (Schein et al., Reference Schein, Ritter and Gray2016; see also Gray & Schein, Reference Gray and Schein2016; Gray, Schein, & Ward, Reference Gray, Schein and Ward2014), and to partly result from the more general effect of affective states (not only disgust) on a wide range of (not only moral) judgments (Cheng, Ottati, & Price, Reference Cheng, Ottati and Price2013; Landy & Piazza, Reference Landy and Piazza2017).

2.2. Self-serving norms and conflicts of sexual strategies

An important framework posits that moral cognition evolved not to promote cooperation, but to advance the condemner's self-interest, by recruiting allies to condemn enemies, coordinating side-taking in conflicts, and promoting moral norms advantageous to oneself (DeScioli, Reference DeScioli2016; DeScioli & Kurzban, Reference DeScioli and Kurzban2009, Reference DeScioli and Kurzban2013; DeScioli, Massenkoff, Shaw, Petersen, & Kurzban, Reference DeScioli, Massenkoff, Shaw, Petersen and Kurzban2014; Petersen, Reference Petersen2018; Sznycer et al., Reference Sznycer, Lopez Seal, Sell, Lim, Porat, Shalvi and Tooby2017; Tooby & Cosmides, Reference Tooby, Cosmides, Hogh-Olesen, Boesch and Cosmides2010). Within this framework, researchers argue that some puritanical norms emerge from self-serving attempts by some individuals to promote their reproductive interests at the expense of others' (Kurzban et al., Reference Kurzban, Dukes and Weeden2010; Weeden & Kurzban, Reference Weeden and Kurzban2016; Weeden, Cohen, & Kenrick, Reference Weeden, Cohen and Kenrick2008). The reproductive religiosity model argues that a high level of promiscuity in the environment threatens monogamous individuals' ability to reap the benefits of their committed, parentally investing reproductive strategy, by increasing the risk of cuckoldry or mate-poaching (Weeden & Kurzban, Reference Weeden and Kurzban2016). Monogamous individuals, thus, have an interest in normatively curbing sexual promiscuity. Researchers also argue that males' attempts to control female sexuality explain values of female chastity and modesty, and restrictions aimed at increasing paternity certainty, such as veiling, virginity tests, female claustration, and menstrual taboos (Becker, Reference Becker2019; Blake, Fourati, & Brooks, Reference Blake, Fourati and Brooks2018; Dickemann, Reference Dickemann, Alexander and Tinkle1981; Pazhoohi, Lang, Xygalatas, & Grammer, Reference Pazhoohi, Lang, Xygalatas and Grammer2017a; Strassmann, Reference Strassmann1992; Strassmann et al., Reference Strassmann, Kurapati, Hug, Burke, Gillespie, Karafet and Hammer2012).

These accounts are not incompatible with our proposal. Mate-guarding surely underlies many sexual restrictions (Strassmann et al., Reference Strassmann, Kurapati, Hug, Burke, Gillespie, Karafet and Hammer2012) and is consistent with the frequent double standard favoring men in the moralization of sexuality (Broude & Greene, Reference Broude and Greene1976; Dabhoiwala, Reference Dabhoiwala2012). Consistent with the reproductive religiosity model, monogamous individuals more harshly oppose sexual promiscuity (Weeden & Kurzban, Reference Weeden and Kurzban2016) and its facilitators (e.g., drugs; Kurzban et al., Reference Kurzban, Dukes and Weeden2010; Quintelier et al., Reference Quintelier, Ishii, Weeden, Kurzban and Braeckman2013), and seem to use religion to facilitate and encourage monogamous pair-bonding (Baumard & Chevallier, Reference Baumard and Chevallier2015; Jacquet et al., Reference Jacquet, Pazhoohi, Findling, Mell, Chevallier and Baumard2021; Moon, Reference Moon2021; Moon, Krems, Cohen, & Kenrick, Reference Moon, Krems, Cohen and Kenrick2019; Weeden et al., Reference Weeden, Cohen and Kenrick2008; Weeden & Kurzban, Reference Weeden and Kurzban2013).

However, these accounts fail to sufficiently explain the more general condemnation of hedonic excesses beyond sexuality, such as gluttony (Hill, Reference Hill2011; Steim & Nemeroff, Reference Steim and Nemeroff1995), drinking, harmless idleness (Tierney et al., Reference Tierney, Hardy, Ebersole, Viganola, Clemente, Gordon and Uhlmann2021), or general lack of self-discipline (Mooijman et al., Reference Mooijman, Meindl, Oyserman, Monterosso, Dehghani, Doris and Graham2018) – sexual lust is only one of the many pleasure-seeking tendencies that puritanical morality condemns. They also do not account for the condemnation of bodily pleasures as intrinsically sinful, even when they are truly harmless to males or monogamous strategists, such as in frequent sexuality between monogamous, married partners (e.g., Dabhoiwala, Reference Dabhoiwala2012, pp. 7–9; Seidman, Reference Seidman1990; Suiming, Reference Suiming1998, p. 16), or in solitary masturbation (Seidman, Reference Seidman1990). Besides, puritanical societies moralize not only female lust but male sexual desires as well – an observation inconsistent with the mate-guarding hypothesis (Muslim Zanzibar: Beckmann, Reference Beckmann2010; medieval and early modern Europe: Dabhoiwala, Reference Dabhoiwala2012, p. 8; McIntosh, Reference McIntosh2002, pp. 73–74; Victorian England: Seidman, Reference Seidman1990).

Although manipulative use of moral discourse is surely used to justify oppressive norms (Strassmann et al., Reference Strassmann, Kurapati, Hug, Burke, Gillespie, Karafet and Hammer2012), and advance condemners' self-interests (DeScioli et al., Reference DeScioli, Massenkoff, Shaw, Petersen and Kurzban2014; Sznycer et al., Reference Sznycer, Lopez Seal, Sell, Lim, Porat, Shalvi and Tooby2017), we propose, in the following, that people genuinely perceive puritanical norms as mutually beneficial in the social contexts in which they prevail.

3. The moral disciplining theory of puritanism

We propose that hedonistic behaviors, although inherently victimless, are condemned because they are perceived as indirectly favoring uncooperative behaviors (e.g., aggression, infidelity, free-riding), by altering people's self-control. This hypothesis assumes that people perceive cooperation as requiring self-control (sect. 3.1); that people perceive hedonistic behaviors, such as intoxicant use, bodily pleasures, and undisciplined lifestyles, as reducing people's self-control (sect. 3.2); and that moral cognition is triggered not only by intrinsic instances of cheating, but also by behaviors perceived to indirectly and probabilistically favor socially harmful outcomes (sect. 3.3).

3.1. People perceive that cooperation requires self-control

Before reviewing evidence that people perceive self-control as necessary for cooperative behavior (sect. 3.1.3), we argue that this intuition is somewhat justified: Central forms of human cooperation – reciprocal and reputation-based cooperation – objectively require delaying gratification (sects. 3.1.1 and 3.1.2). This objective relationship between cooperation and self-control allows explaining why people perceive that cooperation requires self-control in the first place.

3.1.1. Reciprocal and reputation-based cooperation require delaying gratification

Cooperation refers to any behavior that benefits another individual (the recipient), and the evolutionary function of which is, at least in part, to benefit the recipient (West, Griffin, & Gardner, Reference West, Griffin and Gardner2007, Reference West, El Mouden and Gardner2011). Some types of cooperation provide immediate inclusive fitness benefits to the actor. This is the case of kin altruism, whereby the actor automatically increases his indirect fitness (Hamiltron, Reference Hamiltron1964). This is also the case of by-product mutualisms (West, El Mouden, & Gardner, Reference West, El Mouden and Gardner2011), or cooperation for “shared interests” (West, Cooper, Ghoul, & Griffin, Reference West, Cooper, Ghoul and Griffin2021), whereby the actor benefits the recipient as a by-product of pursing his own immediate self-interest (e.g., cooperative hunting in social carnivores; Leimar & Connor, Reference Leimar, Connor and Hammerstein2003; Leimar & Hammerstein, Reference Leimar and Hammerstein2010).

When the actor's and the recipient's immediate interests are not fully aligned, however, cooperation requires the actor to invest an immediate cost (to benefit the recipient), that is rewarded only in the future, by greater benefit-provision (or reduced cost-infliction) from the recipient or third parties – such as through direct reciprocity (Axelrod, Reference Axelrod1984; Trivers, Reference Trivers1971), indirect reciprocity (Nowak & Sigmund, Reference Nowak and Sigmund2005; Panchanathan & Boyd, Reference Panchanathan and Boyd2004), or partner choice (Barclay, Reference Barclay2013; Roberts, Reference Roberts2020). In the iterated Prisoner's Dilemma, for example, reciprocal cooperation yields higher payoff than defection only in the long run, by securing partners' willingness to reciprocate in subsequent rounds – in the immediate present of each round, cheating pays more than cooperating (Axelrod, Reference Axelrod1984; Axelrod & Hamilton, Reference Axelrod and Hamilton1981). Using evolutionary simulations, Roberts (Reference Roberts2020) shows that the same holds for cooperation under reputation-based partner choice: Cooperation is adaptive when the cost of renouncing the immediate benefit of cheating is exceeded, in the long run, by the increased probability of being chosen as a partner in subsequent interactions. In other words, because people selectively associate with trustworthy partners, a good reputation can be understood as a capital that yields future benefits at each time step of the rest of an individual's life. Damaging this capital by exploiting others brings immediate benefits (e.g., more resources, sexual opportunities, less effort), yet deprives oneself of all the benefits that a good reputation could have brought at each later time-point, by attracting others' cooperative investments (Lie-Panis & André, Reference Lie-Panis and André2022).

Accordingly, scholars have widely noted that reciprocal and reputation-based cooperation require delaying gratification: Individuals must renounce the immediate, smaller reward of cheating to secure the future, larger benefits of cooperating (Axelrod, Reference Axelrod2006; Frank, Reference Frank1988; Manrique et al., Reference Manrique, Zeidler, Roberts, Barclay, Walker, Samu and Raihani2021; Roberts, Reference Roberts2020; Stevens et al., Reference Stevens, Cushman and Hauser2005). Lie-Panis and André (Reference Lie-Panis and André2022) develop a formal understanding of this idea. In their model, individuals are characterized by a discount rate, and engage in numerous trust games during their lifetime, with a certain probability of being observed by others, who transmit reputational information impacting future partner choice. At equilibrium in their model, individuals who cooperate are those who are sufficiently future-oriented, that is, who discount the future benefit of having a good reputation in the rest of their life little enough for this benefit to outweigh the immediate cost of cooperation.

3.1.2. Cooperation and self-control at the proximate level

In line with the fact that reciprocal and reputation-based cooperation ultimately require delaying gratification, psychologists have long noted that self-control – the ability to resist temptations of immediate rewards – is likely involved in cooperative decision making (Ainslie, Reference Ainslie2013; Hofmann et al., Reference Hofmann, Meindl, Mooijman and Graham2018; Manrique et al., Reference Manrique, Zeidler, Roberts, Barclay, Walker, Samu and Raihani2021; Stevens et al., Reference Stevens, Cushman and Hauser2005). For example, when faced with an attractive mating opportunity, avoiding cheating one's partner requires resisting temptations of immediate sexual pleasure (see Gailliot & Baumeister, Reference Gailliot and Baumeister2007). By renouncing this immediate reward, one secures the long-term benefit of preserving one's pair-bonding relationship – a particular type of cooperative interaction (Gurven, Winking, Kaplan, von Rueden, & McAllister, Reference Gurven, Winking, Kaplan, von Rueden and McAllister2009) – as well as one's reputation as a trustworthy partner. Meeting obligations to share resources with others, similarly, requires resisting the immediate reward of consuming these resources for oneself (Hofmann et al., Reference Hofmann, Meindl, Mooijman and Graham2018; Sebastián-Enesco & Warneken, Reference Sebastián-Enesco and Warneken2015). By resisting this temptation, one secures the larger, future benefits of ensuring reciprocal help, as well as a good reputation. Similarly, avoiding interpersonal conflicts sometimes requires overriding aggressive impulses (see Barton-Crosby & Hirtenlehner, Reference Barton-Crosby and Hirtenlehner2021); and doing one's part in collaborative work requires renouncing immediate leisure or procrastination. We call (1) “temptations to cheat” these impulses for immediate rewards (e.g., food, sex, rest) that conflict with prosocial obligations, and (2) “moral self-control” the resistance to these temptations to cheat (following Hofmann et al., Reference Hofmann, Meindl, Mooijman and Graham2018).

Converging lines of evidence demonstrate this importance of self-control for a wide range of cooperative behaviors. Performance on a delay-of-gratification task predicts children's propensity to share resources with others, after controlling for age (Sebastián-Enesco & Warneken, Reference Sebastián-Enesco and Warneken2015). Focusing on the future rather than immediate consequences of their behaviors makes participants more likely to share with others (Sjåstad, Reference Sjåstad2019), and less likely to behave unethically (Hershfield, Cohen, & Thompson, Reference Hershfield, Cohen and Thompson2012; van Gelder, Hershfield, & Nordgren, Reference van Gelder, Hershfield and Nordgren2013; Vonasch & Sjåstad, Reference Vonasch and Sjåstad2021). Consistent with a trade-off between the immediate benefit of cheating and its future reputational cost, these associations between cooperation and future-orientation are mediated by reputational concern (Sjåstad, Reference Sjåstad2019; Vonasch & Sjåstad, Reference Vonasch and Sjåstad2021). Disrupting participants' right lateral prefrontal cortex – implied in the self-control of impulses for instant rewards (Kober et al., Reference Kober, Mende-Siedlecki, Kross, Weber, Mischel, Hart and Ochsner2010) – makes participants more likely to cheat in cooperative interactions (Knoch & Fehr, Reference Knoch and Fehr2007; Knoch, Schneider, Schunk, Hohmann, & Fehr, Reference Knoch, Schneider, Schunk, Hohmann and Fehr2009; Ruff, Ugazio, & Fehr, Reference Ruff, Ugazio and Fehr2013; Soutschek, Sauter, & Schubert, Reference Soutschek, Sauter and Schubert2015; Strang et al., Reference Strang, Gross, Schuhmann, Riedl, Weber and Sack2015). Following 1,000 children from birth to age 32, Moffitt et al. (Reference Moffitt, Arseneault, Belsky, Dickson, Hancox, Harrington and Caspi2011) show that children with poor self-control are more likely to be convicted of a criminal offense as adults, after controlling for social class origins and IQ. Meta-analytic evidence confirms that low self-control is associated with criminal behaviors (Vazsonyi, Mikuška, & Kelley, Reference Vazsonyi, Mikuška and Kelley2017), lower propensity to forgive others and refrain from retaliation (Burnette et al., Reference Burnette, Davisson, Finkel, Van Tongeren, Hui and Hoyle2014; Liu & Li, Reference Liu and Li2020), and poorer interpersonal functioning (e.g., loyalty) (de Ridder, Lensvelt-Mulders, Finkenauer, Stok, & Baumeister, Reference de Ridder, Lensvelt-Mulders, Finkenauer, Stok and Baumeister2012). Low self-control predicts greater propensity to deceive others to obtain more benefits (Fan, Ren, Zhang, Xiao, & Zhong, Reference Fan, Ren, Zhang, Xiao and Zhong2020), lower likelihood to keep promises in relationships (Peetz & Kammrath, Reference Peetz and Kammrath2011), as well as uncooperative behaviors in the workplace (e.g., low accommodation of coworkers' needs) (Cohen, Panter, Turan, Morse, & Kim, Reference Cohen, Panter, Turan, Morse and Kim2014; Restubog, Garcia, Wang, & Cheng, Reference Restubog, Garcia, Wang and Cheng2010). Regarding sexual cheating, low conscientiousness – a construct related to self-control (Duckworth & Seligman, Reference Duckworth and Seligman2017) – predicts greater likelihood of infidelity in men and women across 52 countries of 10 world regions (Schmitt, Reference Schmitt2004; see also Pronk, Karremans, & Wigboldus, Reference Pronk, Karremans and Wigboldus2011). And studies suggest that intensity of sexual desire, as well as tendencies to notice attractive alternative partners, predict greater infidelity among people with low, but not high dispositional self-control (Brady, Baker, & Miller, Reference Brady, Baker and Miller2020; McIntyre, Barlow, & Hayward, Reference McIntyre, Barlow and Hayward2015).

Despite this wealth of evidence, the self-control requirement of cooperation has been questioned by results from economic games, where meta-analytic evidence finds no association between self-control and cooperation (Thielmann, Spadaro, & Balliet, Reference Thielmann, Spadaro and Balliet2020). Studies also found that American participants cooperate more when forced to decide quickly than when forced to delay their decision – suggesting that cooperation in economic games, rather than requiring self-control, may be spontaneous and effortless (Rand, Reference Rand2016, Reference Rand2017; Rand, Greene, & Nowak, Reference Rand, Greene and Nowak2012). However, this “intuitive cooperation” effect failed to replicate in several, highly powered replications (Bouwmeester et al., Reference Bouwmeester, Verkoeijen, Aczel, Barbosa, Bègue, Brañas-Garza and Wollbrant2017; Camerer et al., Reference Camerer, Dreber, Holzmeister, Ho, Huber, Johannesson and Wu2018; Fromell, Nosenzo, & Owens, Reference Fromell, Nosenzo and Owens2020; Isler, Yilmaz, & John Maule, Reference Isler, Yilmaz and John Maule2021). Recent evidence indicates that, although cooperating in economic games may be prosocial individuals' spontaneous impulse, the reverse is true for more selfish individuals, in which deliberation increases cooperation – consistent with a role of self-control (Alós-Ferrer & Garagnani, Reference Alós-Ferrer and Garagnani2020; Andrighetto, Capraro, Guido, & Szekely, Reference Andrighetto, Capraro, Guido and Szekely2020; Nockur & Pfattheicher, Reference Nockur and Pfattheicher2021; Yamagishi et al., Reference Yamagishi, Matsumoto, Kiyonari, Takagishi, Li, Kanai and Sakagami2017). As Thielmann et al. (Reference Thielmann, Spadaro and Balliet2020) note, this moderation by prosocial disposition may have obscured the relationship between self-control and cooperation in their meta-analysis of economic games (pp. 62–63). Field experiments also suggest that economic games underestimate the involvement of self-control in real-life cooperative decisions. Studying Brazilian fishermen living from their catch from a common lake, Fehr and Leibbrandt (Reference Fehr and Leibbrandt2011) found that, while impulsivity was not associated with lower cooperation in an economic game, it did predict likelihood to free-ride on the common-pool resource in real life.

3.1.3. People perceive that cooperation requires self-control

Our account of puritanism assumes that people intuitively perceive this self-control requirement of cooperation – a premise that is well supported. Lie-Panis and André (Reference Lie-Panis and André2022) show that, because ability to delay gratification enables higher levels of cooperation, it can evolve into a credible signal of trustworthiness. Psychological evidence confirms that, in interaction with strangers as well as in established relationships, people infer others' self-control from their behavior, and expect individuals they perceive as more self-controlled to behave more cooperatively (Buyukcan-Tetik & Pronk, Reference Buyukcan-Tetik and Pronk2021; Buyukcan-Tetik, Finkenauer, Siersema, Vander Heyden, & Krabbendam, Reference Buyukcan-Tetik, Finkenauer, Siersema, Vander Heyden and Krabbendam2015; Gai & Bhattacharjee, Reference Gai and Bhattacharjee2022; Gomillion, Lamarche, Murray, & Harris, Reference Gomillion, Lamarche, Murray and Harris2014; Koval, VanDellen, Fitzsimons, & Ranby, Reference Koval, VanDellen, Fitzsimons and Ranby2015; Peetz & Kammrath, Reference Peetz and Kammrath2013; Righetti & Finkenauer, Reference Righetti and Finkenauer2011). People's intuitions about a good moral character include traits arguably related to self-control, such as being principled or responsible (Goodwin, Reference Goodwin2015; Goodwin, Piazza, & Rozin, Reference Goodwin, Piazza and Rozin2013). And many societies consider self-control, self-discipline, or self-restraint, as key virtues inherent to a good moral character (e.g., Lybian Bedouins: Abu-Lughod, Reference Abu-Lughod2016, pp. 90–93; Buddhism: Clark, Reference Clark1932, pp. 86–88; Goodman, Reference Goodman and Zalta2017; Confucianism: Csikszentmihalyi, Reference Csikszentmihalyi and Zalta2020; Tiwald, Reference Tiwald and Zalta2020; Sunni Islam: el-Aswad, Reference el-Aswad and Leeming2014; Wolof: Irvine, Reference Irvine1974, pp. 126–127; Zanzibar: Beckmann, Reference Beckmann2010, p. 620; Christianity: Spiegel, Reference Spiegel2020).

3.2. People perceive that some behaviors alter self-control

People, thus, intuit that cooperation requires self-control. We argue that puritanical moral judgments emerge from the interaction of this intuition with folk-psychological beliefs that some behaviors alter self-control. These behaviors include consuming intoxicants (e.g., alcohol, drugs), exposing oneself to tempting environments (e.g., immodest clothes, unruly music and dances), overindulging in potentially addictive pleasures (e.g., food, sex, intoxicants), or pursuing undisciplined lifestyles (e.g., intemperance, idleness, lack of ritual observance). The moral disciplining theory posits that these behaviors are moralized when perceived as undermining people's ability to control their impulses, to the point of endangering compliance with their cooperative obligations. This section characterizes the folk-psychological beliefs which, we propose, underlie puritanical moral judgments.

3.2.1. Lay theories of modifiers of state-self-control

Some behaviors of the puritanical constellation, we argue, are perceived as altering self-control as a state – that is, the ability to resist temptation in a given moment. We call them “modifiers of state-self-control.”

Intoxicants. A first perceived modifier of state-self-control is intoxicant use. Psychological evidence shows that people widely believe alcohol to cause loss of self-control (Brett, Leavens, Miller, Lombardi, & Leffingwell, Reference Brett, Leavens, Miller, Lombardi and Leffingwell2016; Critchlow, Reference Critchlow1986; Leigh, Reference Leigh1987). Studies similarly suggest that people perceive drug use as enhancing short-term sexual impulses (Quintelier et al., Reference Quintelier, Ishii, Weeden, Kurzban and Braeckman2013). These lay theories likely stem from observation of intoxicants' objective psychological effects. Alcohol actually impairs the inhibition of impulses (Heatherton & Wagner, Reference Heatherton and Wagner2011), narrows attention to cues of immediate rewards – an effect known as “alcohol myopia” (Giancola, Josephs, Parrott, & Duke, Reference Giancola, Josephs, Parrott and Duke2010) – , and fuels a range of impulsive behaviors (e.g., reactive aggression: Duke, Smith, Oberleitner, Westphal, & McKee, Reference Duke, Smith, Oberleitner, Westphal and McKee2018; Gan, Sterzer, Marxen, Zimmermann, & Smolka, Reference Gan, Sterzer, Marxen, Zimmermann and Smolka2015; Parrott & Eckhardt, Reference Parrott and Eckhardt2018; sexual impulsivity: Rehm, Shield, Joharchi, & Shuper, Reference Rehm, Shield, Joharchi and Shuper2012; economic impulsivity: Schilbach, Reference Schilbach2019). Consumption of drugs is similarly associated with impulsivity (Duke et al., Reference Duke, Smith, Oberleitner, Westphal and McKee2018; Nemoto, Iwamoto, Morris, Yokota, & Wada, Reference Nemoto, Iwamoto, Morris, Yokota and Wada2007; Weafer, Mitchell, & de Wit, Reference Weafer, Mitchell and de Wit2014).

The moral disciplining theory thus posits that intoxicants are moralized because they are perceived as favoring uncooperative behaviors, such as aggression, infidelity, and general negligence of obligations, by leading people to lose control over immediate impulses, and fueling disregard of future consequences. This hypothesis contrasts with existing accounts, which ignore cooperative concerns in the moralization of intoxicants, by arguing that their moralization stems from disgust-based concerns for the “purity of the soul” (Clifford et al., Reference Clifford, Iyengar, Cabeza and Sinnott-Armstrong2015; Henderson & Dressler, Reference Henderson and Dressler2019; Horberg et al., Reference Horberg, Oveis, Keltner and Cohen2009; Silver, Reference Silver2020), or from exclusively selfish attempts of monogamous strategists to limit sexual promiscuity specifically (Kurzban et al., Reference Kurzban, Dukes and Weeden2010).

Immodesty as cue exposure. Beside intoxicants, people perceive that self-control is also threatened by exposure to stimuli triggering short-term-oriented impulses – an effect called cue exposure (Heatherton & Wagner, Reference Heatherton and Wagner2011). Animal brains evolved reward systems tracking stimuli contributing to reproductive success (e.g., food items, sexual opportunities). Environmental cues predicting such items' availability in the immediate environment (e.g., sexual cues, appetizing smell) are thus rapidly learned and imbued with “wanting” properties (Duckworth, Gendler, & Gross, Reference Duckworth, Gendler and Gross2016a; Hyman, Reference Hyman2007; Kringelbach & Berridge, Reference Kringelbach and Berridge2009). Exposure to these cues thus generates strong urges to consume the reward in the here and now, pushing individuals toward immediate gratification at the expense of long-term goals (Boswell & Kober, Reference Boswell and Kober2016; Demos, Heatherton, & Kelley, Reference Demos, Heatherton and Kelley2012; Fujita, Reference Fujita2011; Heatherton & Wagner, Reference Heatherton and Wagner2011).

People have a folk-understanding of cue exposure. Early in development, children understand that distracting their attention away from tempting cues (e.g., the marshmallow in front of them) allows them to delay gratification more easily (Carlson & Beck, Reference Carlson, Beck, Winsler, Fernyhough and Montero2001; Mischel & Mischel, Reference Mischel and Mischel1983; Peake, Hebl, & Mischel, Reference Peake, Hebl and Mischel2002). Also witnessing this folk-understanding, people develop “situational” strategies for self-control, rearranging their environment upstream (e.g., by not storing tempting snacks at home) to prevent short-term impulses to be triggered by cue exposure (Duckworth et al., Reference Duckworth, Gendler and Gross2016a; Duckworth, White, Matteucci, Shearer, & Gross, Reference Duckworth, White, Matteucci, Shearer and Gross2016b; Milyavskaya, Saunders, & Inzlicht, Reference Milyavskaya, Saunders and Inzlicht2021). This folk-understanding, we argue, has moral consequences when cue exposure is perceived as endangering, not personal self-control (e.g., resisting sugar to preserve health), but moral self-control (resisting impulses to refrain from cheating).

This allows explaining another part of the puritanical constellation – the condemnation of immodesty. Behaviors condemned as immodest by puritanical standards typically involve emission of stimuli likely perceived as triggering impulses, thus favoring harmful self-control failures. Immodest clothing reveals cues of female fertility or sexual interest, such as body curves, skin, hair, or eyes (Pazhoohi, Reference Pazhoohi2016; Pazhoohi & Hosseinchari, Reference Pazhoohi and Hosseinchari2014). Exposure to these cues is known to alter males' state-self-control, by triggering their reward systems and sexual appetite (Platek & Singh, Reference Platek and Singh2010; Spicer & Platek, Reference Spicer and Platek2010; Symons, Reference Symons, Ambramson and Pinkerton1995), and increasing their preference for immediate over delayed rewards (Kim & Zauberman, Reference Kim and Zauberman2013; Wilson & Daly, Reference Wilson and Daly2004). Studies also suggest that exposure to sexual cues increases males' propensity to engage in manipulative and coercive behaviors to obtain sexual gratification – and thus to facilitate, not only self-control failures in general, but also moral self-control failures (Ariely & Loewenstein, Reference Ariely and Loewenstein2006).

Prescriptions of modesty, we thus argue, are another strategy – besides prohibition of intoxicants – for preventing self-control failures with socially harmful effects. Immodest clothing and behaviors are moralized because, by increasing cue exposure, they are seen as increasing the probability that people – especially males – lose control over impulses, thereby favoring antisocial behaviors such as sexual aggression, conflicts, adultery, or premarital sex.Footnote 3 Just as people remove tempting snacks from their environment when feeling unable to resist them, societies can deem mutually beneficial, when fearing the fragility of their members' self-control (see sect. 5), to remove tempting stimuli from their environment to prevent uncooperative behaviors.

This hypothesis contrasts with existing accounts of modesty norms, which mainly regard them as selfish attempts of males to guard their mates (Dickemann, Reference Dickemann, Alexander and Tinkle1981; Pazhoohi et al., Reference Pazhoohi, Lang, Xygalatas and Grammer2017a). Although males' mate-guarding interests likely contribute to these norms' attractiveness, we propose that moralization of modesty also emerge from more widely shared concern for the general social harm (e.g., conflicts, aggressions, infidelity) that may result from failures to control sexual impulses.

3.2.2. Lay theories of modifiers of trait-self-control

Other behaviors of the puritanical constellation, we argue, are perceived as altering self-control as a trait – that is, as the stable psychological disposition to resist temptations across situations. We term them “modifiers of trait-self-control.”

Immoderate indulgence in bodily pleasures. Moralizations of victimless bodily pleasures, we argue, stem from perceptions that excessively or too frequently indulging in bodily pleasures would decrease trait-self-control. Such beliefs may be grounded in experience: The bodily pleasures typically condemned by puritanical standards generate common addictions, such as food addictions (Volkow, Wang, & Baler, Reference Volkow, Wang and Baler2011, Reference Volkow, Wise and Baler2017), sexual addictions (Farré et al., Reference Farré, Fernández-Aranda, Granero, Aragay, Mallorquí-Bague, Ferrer and Jiménez-Murcia2015; Karila et al., Reference Karila, Wery, Weinstein, Cottencin, Petit, Reynaud and Billieux2014), alcohol addictions (Vengeliene, Bilbao, Molander, & Spanagel, Reference Vengeliene, Bilbao, Molander and Spanagel2008), drug addictions (Baler & Volkow, Reference Baler and Volkow2007), or gambling disorders (Farré et al., Reference Farré, Fernández-Aranda, Granero, Aragay, Mallorquí-Bague, Ferrer and Jiménez-Murcia2015) – addiction being widely viewed as a disruption of self-control (Baler & Volkow, Reference Baler and Volkow2007; see also Vonasch, Clark, Lau, Vohs, & Baumeister, Reference Vonasch, Clark, Lau, Vohs and Baumeister2017). Researchers have long noted the potent reinforcement learning associated with consumption of bodily pleasures or intoxicants. Past experience with such a reward (e.g., energy-rich food) increases the motivational drive (“wanting”) elicited by future exposure to it, making harder the future self-control of the associated impulse (e.g., food craving; Baler & Volkow, Reference Baler and Volkow2007; Story, Vlaev, Seymour, Darzi, & Dolan, Reference Story, Vlaev, Seymour, Darzi and Dolan2014; Volkow, Wise, & Baler, Reference Volkow, Wise and Baler2017).

Accordingly, a widespread belief seems to be that the more one indulges in bodily pleasures, the more their temptations become hard to resist. A significant share of people believes that pornography (Grubbs, Grant, & Engelman, Reference Grubbs, Grant and Engelman2018a; Grubbs, Kraus, & Perry, Reference Grubbs, Kraus and Perry2019), fatty and sugary foods (Ruddock & Hardman, Reference Ruddock and Hardman2017), and intoxicants (Edelstein et al., Reference Edelstein, Wacht, Grinstein-Cohen, Reznik, Pruginin and Isralowitz2020; El Khoury, Noufi, Ahmad, Akl, & El Hayek, Reference El Khoury, Noufi, Ahmad, Akl and El Hayek2019) can be addictive – and people likely associate addiction with loss of self-control (see Vonasch et al., Reference Vonasch, Clark, Lau, Vohs and Baumeister2017). In vignette studies, we found that participants judged individuals increasing their indulgence in bodily pleasures over several months (e.g., pornography, alcohol, fatty and sugary foods) as altering their trait-self-control as a result of this lifestyle change (Fitouchi et al., Reference Fitouchi, André, Baumard and Nettle2022). Surveying religious attitudes toward pleasure, Glucklich (Reference Glucklich2020, pp. 13–27) concludes that the addictive character of food, sex, alcohol, or gambling, is a major concern across world religions. Reviewing attitudes toward sex in European history, Dabhoiwala (Reference Dabhoiwala2012) highlights that “It was a Christian commonplace that anyone who succumbed to this impure appetite [lust], even just once, risked developing a fatal addiction to it” (p. 33). In Hinduism, similarly, “Ancient Indian texts often call the four major addictions that kings were vulnerable to ‘the vices of lust,’ sometimes naming them after the activities themselves – gambling, drinking, fornicating, hunting” (Doniger, Reference Doniger2014, p. 365). Table 1 summarizes selected cases of such folk-psychological beliefs in various cultural contexts.

Table 1. Examples of folk-psychological beliefs about modifiers of trait-self-control

If people perceive that cooperation requires self-control (sect. 3.1.3), and that overindulgence in bodily pleasures reduces self-control, they may moralize bodily pleasures as indirectly facilitating uncooperative behaviors. For example, if indulgence in sexual pleasure in victimless situations (e.g., masturbation, frequent sex within marriage), is perceived as making people addict to sex, it becomes responsible for impeding the control of sexual urges in cooperative situations as well, where these impulses are socially harmful (e.g., when resisting them is necessary to avoid adultery). If victimless gluttony is perceived as making people addict to food, it becomes responsible for fueling uncontrollable urges which, in other situations, will prove socially harmful (e.g., when resisting food cravings is necessary to respect others' property). Repeated indulgence in bodily pleasures may be perceived, more generally, as decreasing self-control across domains, thus decreasing people's cooperativeness in general. This would be consistent with the lay theory we discuss next: That repeatedly practicing self-control would train self-control.

Self-control training, daily self-discipline, and ritual observance. Another recurrent lay theory seems to be that self-control can be trained by repeated practice – although the objective efficacy of such training is scientifically debated (Berkman, Reference Berkman, Vohs and Baumeister2016; Friese, Frankenbach, Job, & Loschelder, Reference Friese, Frankenbach, Job and Loschelder2017; Miles et al., Reference Miles, Sheeran, Baird, Macdonald, Webb and Harris2016). Field experiments on parents suggest a widespread belief that children's self-control can be improved, associated with self-control-training practices, such as giving children unhealthy snacks less often, or bringing them less frequently to fast-food restaurants (Mukhopadhyay & Yeung, Reference Mukhopadhyay and Yeung2010). In vignette studies, participants judged that sustained self-discipline over several months (e.g., exercising regularly, reducing indulgence in bodily pleasures) would likely improve a target's trait-self-control (Fitouchi et al., Reference Fitouchi, André, Baumard and Nettle2022). This is consistent with cross-culturally recurrent beliefs that investment in ascetic practices or effortful activities allow to “build character” and improve people's self-control (see Table 1).

If people perceive both that cooperation requires self-control (Righetti & Finkenauer, Reference Righetti and Finkenauer2011), and that regular self-discipline trains self-control, they may moralize effortful activities (e.g., waking up early, spiritual disciplines, needless hard work), as means to “build character” – that is, to improve the self-control required to honor prosocial obligations. This helps explaining another component of the puritanical constellation: The moralization of constant self-discipline, needless hard work, and unproductive effort, even when the latter are devoid of direct benefits to other people (Celniker et al., Reference Celniker, Gregory, Koo, Piff, Ditto and Shariff2023; Tierney et al., Reference Tierney, Hardy, Ebersole, Viganola, Clemente, Gordon and Uhlmann2021).

This also allows explaining the moralization of pious ritual observance. Indeed, psychologists have extensively argued that rituals of world religions, such as fasting, meditation, regular prayer, or effortful pilgrimages, appear specifically geared toward training self-control (Geyer & Baumeister, Reference Geyer, Baumeister, Paloutzian and Park2005; Koole, Meijer, & Remmers, Reference Koole, Meijer and Remmers2017; McCullough & Carter, Reference McCullough and Carter2013; McCullough & Willoughby, Reference McCullough and Willoughby2009; Tian et al., Reference Tian, Schroeder, Häubl, Risen, Norton and Gino2018; Wood, Reference Wood2017). These rituals require sustained restrictions of bodily desires (e.g., fasting), commitment to regular practice (e.g., praying five times a day, at fixed hours), cognitive effort (e.g., reading and memorizing the scriptures), and repeated inhibition of spontaneous tendencies (McCullough & Willoughby, Reference McCullough and Willoughby2009). We argue that these activities, as the rest of the puritanical constellation, are ascribed a moral disciplining function: Cultivating the self-control perceived necessary to honor prosocial obligations. This allows explaining why moralizations of diligent ritual observance cluster with other puritanical values (sect. 1.1) – so that “piety” is commonly listed, alongside temperance and restraint from bodily pleasures, among the core virtues of the “purity” morality (Graham et al., Reference Graham, Haidt, Koleva, Motyl, Iyer, Wojcik and Ditto2013; Haidt, Reference Haidt2012; Haidt & Joseph, Reference Haidt and Joseph2007).

3.3. Puritanism and the moral mind

Our last assumptions concern the cognitive mechanisms of moral judgment. First, our account rests on a unitary theory of moral cognition, according to which moral judgments – including puritanical ones – are produced by a single, functionally unified cognitive system sensitive to cooperation (André et al., Reference André, Fitouchi, Debove and Baumard2022). In line with other unitary theories of moral cognition (Gray et al., Reference Gray, Waytz and Young2012, Reference Gray, Schein and Ward2014; Schein & Gray, Reference Schein and Gray2015, Reference Schein and Gray2018), we insist that the plurality of moral values at the cultural level does not imply the existence of a plurality of moral systems at the cognitive level. The same moral system can produce, based on the very same computational procedures, a wide variety of outputs, and thus culturally variable values, depending on the varying inputs that it receives (Aarøe & Petersen, Reference Aarøe and Petersen2014; Nettle & Saxe, Reference Nettle and Saxe2020, Reference Nettle and Saxe2021). In the case of puritanical norms, a domain-general system sensitive to harm (Gray et al., Reference Gray, Schein and Ward2014), or fairness (Baumard et al., Reference Baumard, André and Sperber2013), can moralize victimless behaviors, as long as it is fed by causal representations depicting those behaviors as indirectly leading to socially harmful outcomes.

Second, we assume that this moral system is triggered not only by intrinsic instances of uncooperative behaviors (e.g., violence, adultery, unfair sharing), but also by behaviors perceived as indirectly and probabilistically leading to social harm. This is consistent with experimental evidence that the triggering of moral judgment depends on the computation of a – potentially indirect – causal chain between a perpetrator's action and an undeserved cost imposed on another individual (Cushman, Reference Cushman2008; Guglielmo & Malle, Reference Guglielmo and Malle2017; Sloman, Fernbach, & Ewing, Reference Sloman, Fernbach, Ewing and Ross2009). Victimless excesses should be preemptively moralized when perceived to causally contribute, through their deleterious effects on self-control, to an increased prevalence of uncooperative behaviors. Restrained behaviors should be praised when perceived to positively contribute, through their preserving effects on self-control, to the improvement of people's cooperativeness.

3.4. The cultural evolution of puritanism as a behavioral technology

So far, we have focused on the psychological level of moral judgment. Yet puritanism also manifests in socially transmitted traits, subject to cultural elaboration. Carnal sins are not only judged in everyday life; they have been systematized in explicit religious classifications (e.g., the seven deadly sins; Hill, Reference Hill2011; Tentler, Reference Tentler2015). Ascetic rituals of fasting, meditation, or regular prayer have been crafted and institutionalized by doctrinal religions (Brown, Reference Brown2012; Tentler, Reference Tentler2015). Legal regulations of alcohol have been gradually elaborated and negotiated in cultural groups (Martin, Reference Martin2009; Matthee, Reference Matthee2014). Thus, the emergence of puritanical norms is also fruitfully conceived in cultural evolutionary terms. These puritanical cultural traits, we argue, have evolved as people, based on their folk-psychological theories of self-control, have attempted to facilitate self-control to ensure cooperative behavior.

Prominent cultural evolutionary theories argue that normative cultural traits, such as monogamous marriage (Henrich, Boyd, & Richerson, Reference Henrich, Boyd and Richerson2012), moralizing religions (Norenzayan et al., Reference Norenzayan, Shariff, Gervais, Willard, McNamara, Slingerland and Henrich2016), or large-scale cooperative institutions (Richerson et al., Reference Richerson, Baldini, Bell, Demps, Frost, Hillis and Zefferman2016), spread in human populations because they procure objective adaptive benefits by increasing cooperation. Although human enforcement mechanisms (e.g., reputation, punishment) can stabilize any norm (Aumann & Shapley, Reference Aumann, Shapley and Megiddo1994; Boyd & Richerson, Reference Boyd and Richerson1992), intergroup competition would favor cooperation-facilitating norms at the expense of other evolutionarily stable equilibria (Henrich & Muthukrishna, Reference Henrich and Muthukrishna2021). Thus, one possibility is that puritanical norms emerge through random variation, as one of the many stable equilibria that enforcement mechanisms can maintain, and are then favored by cultural group selection. In this perspective, puritanical norms should be objectively effective in increasing cooperation by facilitating self-control (see McCullough & Carter, Reference McCullough and Carter2013), and would be favored by impersonal selective pressures that are independent of people's understanding of the mechanisms these norms involve or the function they serve (see Henrich, Reference Henrich2017, Reference Henrich2020).

Another possibility, however, is that the cultural evolution of norms is driven by people's subjective perceptions that some norms are efficient in satisfying their goals, leading them to selectively retain these norms at the expense of others (Fitouchi & Singh, Reference Fitouchi and Singh2023; Singh, Reference Singh2022; Singh, Wrangham, & Glowacki, Reference Singh, Wrangham and Glowacki2017). This “subjective selection” approach stresses that providing objective benefits to individuals or groups is not necessary for many cultural traits to evolve (Singh, Reference Singh2022). People use their intuitions and folk-theories to craft cultural traits. Sometimes, these intuitions accurately perceive objective benefits, leading people to retain adaptive technologies, such as efficient tools or weapons (Osiurak & Reynaud, Reference Osiurak and Reynaud2019). Other times, people's psychological biases and folk-theories are simply erroneous, leading them to retain ineffective practices – such as divination (Hong & Henrich, Reference Hong and Henrich2021), dark magic (Singh, Reference Singh2021), bloodletting (Miton, Claidière, & Mercier, Reference Miton, Claidière and Mercier2015), shamanism (Singh, Reference Singh2018), or rain-making rituals (Hong, Slingerland, & Henrich, Reference Hong, Slingerland and Henrichforthcoming). Such technologies recurrently evolve in human societies despite providing no adaptive benefits, simply because people wrongly perceive them as beneficial.

Normative culture, we argue, is no different. Just as people use their technical reasoning to craft technical artifacts, they use their folk-psychology to design behavioral technologies (e.g., norms, religions, institutions) aimed at influencing other individuals' behaviors. For example, Ostrom (Reference Ostrom1990) famously reviewed how people in many small-scale communities deliberately developed, often through years of trial-and-error, institutional rules they perceived as efficient in limiting free-riding. Researchers similarly argue that beliefs in punitive gods develop because people's folk-psychology perceives these beliefs – potentially wrongly – as likely to motivate others to cooperate (Fitouchi & Singh, Reference Fitouchi and Singh2022).

We argue that puritanical norms, from disciplinary rituals to Victorian anti-masturbation campaigns (Seidman, Reference Seidman1990), emerge from similar processes. They culturally evolve as people, based on their folk-theories of self-control, attempt to manipulate the psychological mechanics of temptation and self-control to promote prosocial behavior (Fitouchi et al., Reference Fitouchi, André and Baumard2021). This drives the cultural evolution of norms aimed at training self-control (e.g., techniques of self-discipline, prohibitions of alcohol) or nudging resistance to temptations (e.g., modest clothing), to favor social harmony and mutually beneficial interactions. Just as for other technologies, these norms may or may not be objectively effective in promoting self-control and thus cooperation – what matters is that people perceive that they are. In fact, there is only mixed evidence that self-control-training (Friese et al., Reference Friese, Frankenbach, Job and Loschelder2017; Miles et al., Reference Miles, Sheeran, Baird, Macdonald, Webb and Harris2016) and religious observance (Marcus & McCullough, Reference Marcus and McCullough2021) can actually make people more self-controlled. Of course, this doesn't imply that puritanical norms never work. Econometric analyses suggest that some moral crusades against alcohol (nineteenth and twentieth centuries) have successfully reduced drunkenness-driven violent crimes (Lowe, Reference Lowe2020). Field experiments in Iran suggest that conservative religious clothing actually decreases chances of male–female encounter (Pazhoohi & Burriss, Reference Pazhoohi and Burriss2016). In our model, however, these objective benefits will affect cultural evolutionary dynamics only if they are reflected in people's subjective perceptions of efficacy, thus impacting people's adoption and promotion strategies.

4. Explaining the core features of puritanism

This section derives predictions from the moral disciplining theory (MDT), contrasts them with those of alternative accounts, reviews current evidence supporting them, and outlines avenues for further testing. MDT generates predictions for the moralizations of each behavior of the “puritanical constellation,” which can be tested independently – on bodily pleasures and lack of self-discipline (sect. 4.1), intoxicants (sect. 4.2), piety and ritual observance (sect. 4.3), and immodest clothing, music and dances (sect. 4.4).

Several predictions apply to each behavior of the constellation. For each behavior, MDT predicts that (a) its moralization should be most robustly associated with the perception that it affects cooperation (e.g., cause social harm); (b) the more people perceive the behavior as affecting self-control, the more they should moralize it; and (c) the perception that the behavior affects self-control should mediate the perception that it affects cooperation. Reliably disconfirming these predictions for a given behavior would result in falsification of MDT's hypothesis for the moralization of this behavior. Predictions that are more specific to one particular behavior (e.g., immodesty) are further specified in the dedicated subsection (e.g., sect. 4.4).

4.1. The praise of temperance and the condemnation of bodily pleasures

4.1.1. Moralizations of bodily pleasures should be most robustly associated with perceptions that they facilitate social harm

As a cooperation-based theory of puritanism, MDT predicts that moralizations of victimless bodily pleasures, and lack of self-discipline, should be most robustly associated with the perception that they facilitate uncooperative behaviors and cause social harm. In line with this idea, Schein et al. (Reference Schein, Ritter and Gray2016) show that moralizations of sexual indulgences (e.g., oral sex) are most strongly associated with perceptions that these behaviors are “dangerous” or “harmful.” Associations between disgust (or disgust-sensitivity) and moralizations of sexual indulgences disappear when perceptions of harm are controlled for (Schein et al., Reference Schein, Ritter and Gray2016; see also Gray et al., Reference Gray, Schein and Ward2014; Gray & Schein, Reference Gray and Schein2016). In the General Social Survey, support for legal restrictions of pornography is associated with the belief that “sexual material lead people to commit rape” (Sherkat & Ellison, Reference Sherkat and Ellison1997). In two vignette studies (N > 1,100), we presented participants with a target led to increase their indulgence in bodily pleasures, such as gluttony and masturbation (Fitouchi et al., Reference Fitouchi, André, Baumard and Nettle2022). Participants judged that, as a result of this lifestyle change, the target had likely become more prone to uncooperative behaviors, such as refusing to help a friend, free-riding on colleagues' work, and cheating his partner if he had the chance (Fitouchi et al., Reference Fitouchi, André, Baumard and Nettle2022). The more participants perceived that bodily pleasures would decrease the target's cooperativeness, the more they morally condemned victimless indulgence in those pleasures (Fitouchi et al., Reference Fitouchi, André, Baumard and Nettle2022).

At a more general level, MDT predicts that moralizations of victimless excesses should relate to cooperation-based moral concerns. By contrast, moral foundations theory (MFT) argues that moralizations of self-discipline are part of disgust-based concerns unrelated to cooperation. Thus, as per MFT, condemnations of victimless excesses should relate more strongly to “purity” concerns – putatively independent of cooperation – than to other moral concerns unambiguously related to cooperation (e.g., loyalty). Across four studies using the Moral Foundations Questionnaire (N > 3,000), participants' tendency to moralize victimless excesses (e.g., snacking on junk food, getting high on drugs, failing to exercise) is predicted not only by concerns for “purity” – which MFT assumes to be independent of cooperation – , but also, and to a similar extent, by moral concerns uncontroversially related to cooperation, such as loyalty/betrayal, and authority/respect (Mooijman et al., Reference Mooijman, Meindl, Oyserman, Monterosso, Dehghani, Doris and Graham2018). This suggests that self-discipline is moralized because it is seen as necessary to ensure within-group cooperation and social order.

These findings are consistent with historical evidence. Historians and social scientists have argued that puritanical moral campaigns of early modern Europe – variously labeled “disciplinary revolutions” (Gorski, Reference Gorski1993, Reference Gorski2003), “social disciplining” (Oestreich et al., Reference Oestreich, Oestreich and Koenigsberger1982), “civilizing offensives” (Eisner, Reference Eisner2014; Powell, Reference Powell2013), or “reform of popular culture” (Burke, Reference Burke1978) – condemned undisciplined indulgence in bodily pleasures to “foster forms of socialization that would promote cooperation and harmony and result in a well-disciplined and well-ordered society” (Martin, Reference Martin2009, p. 9; see also Burke, Reference Burke1978; Eisner, Reference Eisner2014; McIntosh, Reference McIntosh2002). Similarly, ancient ascetic spiritualities (e.g., Late Stoicism, Platonism, early Christianity, Pythagoreanism), prescribed an “overall habituation to temperance” with the explicit aim to prevent bodily appetites to fuel antisocial behaviors (Gaca, Reference Gaca2003). As Platonism contended, for example,

sexual eros and the other two core appetites [eating and drinking], unless held in check by reason, are the origin of human-motivated social ills because they stimulate all vices from avarice to zealotry. If only we minded the necessary limits of sexual activity and ate and drank moderately, the society of peace and justice would be ours for the taking. (Gaca, Reference Gaca2003, pp. 35–36)

4.1.2. The more people perceive bodily pleasures as altering self-control, the more they should moralize bodily pleasures

MDT posits that people perceive bodily pleasures as degrading cooperativeness because they perceive bodily pleasures as degrading self-control, for example because of their addictive character. Thus, the more people perceive victimless bodily pleasures as addictive, or as altering self-control more generally, the more they should moralize bodily pleasures. In line with this idea, studies consistently find that the moralization of pornography is associated with the perception that it is addictive for the self (Grubbs et al., Reference Grubbs, Exline, Pargament, Hook and Carlisle2015, Reference Grubbs, Grant and Engelman2018a; Grubbs, Wilt, Exline, Pargament, & Kraus, Reference Grubbs, Wilt, Exline, Pargament and Kraus2018b). The perception that pornography is addictive is also more prevalent among religious people (Droubay & Butters, Reference Droubay and Butters2020), who are known to moralize pornography more strongly (Droubay, Butters, & Shafer, Reference Droubay, Butters and Shafer2021; Grubbs et al., Reference Grubbs, Exline, Pargament, Hook and Carlisle2015). In vignette studies, the more participants perceive indulgence in gluttony, masturbation, and harmless laziness, as reducing an individual's self-control, the more they morally condemn victimless indulgence in those pleasures (Fitouchi et al., Reference Fitouchi, André, Baumard and Nettle2022).

4.1.3. The perception that bodily pleasures alter self-control should mediate the perception that they affect cooperation

Experimental evidence suggests that the perceived effect of bodily pleasures on self-control mediates their perceived effect on cooperativeness. In the above-mentioned studies, the effect of indulgence (vs. restraint) on a target's perceived change in cooperativeness was 100% mediated by the perceived deleterious effect of indulgence (vs. restraint) on the target's self-control (Fitouchi et al., Reference Fitouchi, André, Baumard and Nettle2022). In other words, participants perceived indulgence in bodily pleasures, as opposed to restraint, as increasing an individual's propensity to uncooperative behaviors almost exactly to the extent that they perceived indulgence (vs. restraint) as altering this individual's self-control.

This converges with experimental evidence on the moralization of gluttony. People regularly indulging in fatty and sugary foods (e.g., hamburgers, donuts), compared to people with healthy diets, are seen not only as less moral and less trustworthy (Mankar, Joshi, Belsare, Jog, & Watve, Reference Mankar, Joshi, Belsare, Jog and Watve2008; Merritt, Reference Merritt2013; Oakes & Slotterback, Reference Oakes and Slotterback2004; Steim & Nemeroff, Reference Steim and Nemeroff1995), but also as less self-controlled (Gerrits, de Ridder, de Wit, & Kuijer, Reference Gerrits, de Ridder, de Wit and Kuijer2009; Merritt, Reference Merritt2013; Puhl & Heuer, Reference Puhl and Heuer2010; Steim & Nemeroff, Reference Steim and Nemeroff1995). Experimental evidence indicates that these perceptions of lower self-control mediate the relationship between indulgent (vs. restrained) diet and perceived lower morality and trustworthiness (Fitouchi et al., Reference Fitouchi, André, Baumard and Nettle2022; Merritt, Reference Merritt2013; Steim & Nemeroff, Reference Steim and Nemeroff1995) – more so than other mediators such as a health-related concern or a general halo effect (Steim & Nemeroff, Reference Steim and Nemeroff1995).

These results converge with historical and ethnographic evidence that, in various cultural contexts, overindulgence in food or sex is condemned as causing uncooperative behaviors through the erosion of self-control. Scholars have argued that moral panics over masturbation in Victorian England emerged from the fear that excessive sexual activity “could, and probably would, lead to habits of indulgence in sensual pleasure and thus cause the erosion of self-control” (Hunt, Reference Hunt1998, p. 589; Seidman, Reference Seidman1990). Masturbation, or too frequent marital sexuality, were denounced as

a vice which excites…the strongest and most uncontrollable propensities of animal nature [i.e., sexual impulses]: these are rendered more active by indulgence, while the power of restraint is lessened by it in a tenfold degree…Controlled by sensual urges the individual loses self-control and social purpose. This inevitably leads to self-destruction and to social chaos and decline. (Seidman, Reference Seidman1990, pp. 50–52, emphases added)

In Muslim Zanzibar, “once tried out, sex is said to dominate a person's thoughts with ever-increasing desire and to make it difficult to refrain from behavior that is classified as immoral, including disrespect of the elders or drug and alcohol abuse” (Beckmann, Reference Beckmann2010, p. 622). In North India, excessive indulgence in sweets is perceived to develop an impulsive character trait, leading to commit antisocial behaviors, “like cheating, stealing and selling daughter” (Vatuk & Vatuk, Reference Vatuk and Vatuk1967, p. 111). As ethnographers report:

The problem of the chatora [the sweet addict] in Indian society seems to be perceived by our informants as a problem of loss of control over the senses by the sweet addict. … Excessive indulgence in sweets has made the chatora incapable of resisting the temptations which continually beset a man from all directions. … The weakness of the chatora is inexcusable…because it is evidence of his potential weakness against all the temptations of anti-social behavior. (Vatuk & Vatuk, Reference Vatuk and Vatuk1967, pp. 111–112, emphases added)

In the Amhara peasants (Ethiopia), “there is also the fear that if one indulges in eating and drinking he may become uncontrollably hostile” (Levine, Reference Levine1965, p. 223), because “eating and drinking to full satiation, and maintaining this state over time, makes one thankless, arrogant, unmindful of law and custom, and dangerously impulsive” (Reminick, Reference Reminick1975, p. 29). Surveys of moral attitudes toward food since European antiquity conclude that “[t]he heart of the problem it seems is that food pleasure challenges self-control” (Coveney, Reference Coveney2006, p. xii; Hill, Reference Hill2011). In medieval Christianity, excessive food pleasure – alongside other deadly sins – “represent devilish temptations that challenge the Christian to develop and practice discretion and self-control” (Hill, Reference Hill2011, p. 135). By weakening the will, gluttony “turns humans into dishonest animals, destined for hell” (Hill, Reference Hill2007, p. 68). By seeking “delicious viands,” a man can “do good to fewer others and cannot withhold himself so that he may help a poor man, or two, or more” (Hill, Reference Hill2007, p. 68). As Doniger (Reference Doniger2014) highlights, similar rationales for abstaining from meat recur in Hindu texts: “flesh heats the passions and is, therefore, dangerous for the ideal Hindu person, who is always in control of his emotions” (p. 415).

4.2. The praise of sobriety and the moralization of intoxicants

According to existing theories, intoxicants are moralized because they elicit disgust (Horberg et al., Reference Horberg, Oveis, Keltner and Cohen2009), or because they favor sexual promiscuity, thereby infringing on the self-interest on monogamous individuals (Kurzban et al., Reference Kurzban, Dukes and Weeden2010). By contrast, MDT predicts that:

  1. (1) the moralization of intoxicants should be most robustly related to the perception that they cause uncooperative behaviors in general – including in the sexual domain (e.g., infidelity), but not exclusively;

  2. (2) because they threaten not just monogamous individuals' self-interest, but mutually beneficial cooperation more generally (e.g., social harmony), intoxicants should be moralized not only by monogamous strategists, but also by other individuals;

  3. (3) the moralizations of intoxicants should be associated with the perception that they decrease self-control, either as a state, because of their immediate psychoactive effects (sect. 3.2.1), or as a trait, because of their addictive nature (sect. 3.2.2);

  4. (4) the perception that intoxicants decrease self-control should mediate the perceived relationship between intoxicant use and uncooperative behaviors.

Future studies could test these predictions. Several lines of evidence, in the meantime, suggest their plausibility.

First, survey data show that people across countries believe that intoxicants and aggression are causally linked (Critchlow, Reference Critchlow1986; Leigh, Reference Leigh1987; Lindman & Lang, Reference Lindman and Lang1994; Paglia & Room, Reference Paglia and Room1998). In the 1996 General Social Survey, 70.9% of respondents view people addicted to alcohol as likely to do something violent to others, whereas this proportion reaches 87.3% for people addicted to drugs (Pescosolido, Monahan, Link, Stueve, & Kikuzawa, Reference Pescosolido, Monahan, Link, Stueve and Kikuzawa1999) – a pattern found in various studies and countries (Yang, Wong, Grivel, & Hasin, Reference Yang, Wong, Grivel and Hasin2017). In a representative sample of the United Kingdom population, “fear of violence,” and perceptions that they have a “bad character” – a notion related to trustworthiness (Goodwin, Reference Goodwin2015) – are the best predictors of stigmatizing attitudes toward heroin-addicts, explaining respectively 23.5 and 12.4% of the variance (Mushtaq, Mendes, Nikolaou, & Luty, Reference Mushtaq, Mendes, Nikolaou and Luty2015). By contrast, perceived risk of contagion – relevant to the disgust-hypothesis – explained only 0.9% of the variance (Mushtaq et al., Reference Mushtaq, Mendes, Nikolaou and Luty2015). In a sample of 1,512 Uruguayan adults, the strongest predictor of opposition to the government's recent decision to legalize marijuana is the belief that this law will worsen public security (Cruz, Boidi, & Queirolo, Reference Cruz, Boidi and Queirolo2018).

This converges with historical and ethnographic evidence that moral concerns over drinking revolve around the uncooperative behaviors it facilitates, such as adultery, conflicts, economic free-riding, or poor performance of family roles (Eisner, Reference Eisner2014; Martin, Reference Martin2002, Reference Martin2009; McIntosh, Reference McIntosh2002; Room, Reference Room1984, Reference Room1996). In a systematic study of more than 200 English and French primary sources (e.g., sermons) from 1300 to 1700, Warner (Reference Warner1997) shows that drinking was condemned as causing six main types of social harm, including domestic violence, public violence, disrespect of authorities, and resources loss (placing economic burdens on the community). Legal regulations of drinking in traditional Europe (thirteenth to eighteenth centuries) are declared necessary because alcohol is “the root and foundation of many other enormous sins, such as bloodshed, stabbing, murder, swearing, fornification [sic], adultery, and such like” (Martin, Reference Martin2009, p. 2), and results in “idleness, blasphemy, homicides and other damage and harm” (Martin, Reference Martin2009, p. 30). Analyses of articles on marijuana published in American popular magazines between 1935 and 1940 – a period of moral concern over drug use – similarly find that 85% mentioned violence as one of its effects (Himmelstein, Reference Himmelstein1983).

Second, intoxicants appear perceived as causing uncooperative behaviors because of their effects on self-control. In survey responses, beliefs that alcohol causes “loss of self-control” and “disinhibition” is associated with the belief that alcohol favors “nasty” behaviors (e.g., fights, aggression) (r = 0.54) (Leigh, Reference Leigh1987). Presenting participants with 15 vignettes of a dating situation, Shively (Reference Shively2001) asked participants to rate a man's level of self-control and likelihood of sexual aggression. As the man was described as more inebriated, participants perceived him as less self-controlled and less able to stop himself from sexual aggression. In other studies, participants judged that a target led to increase his consumption of alcohol would become less self-controlled, more likely to commit uncooperative behaviors, and would worsen his moral character as a result of this lifestyle change (Fitouchi et al., Reference Fitouchi, André, Baumard and Nettle2022). Perceived change in the targets' self-control fully mediated the effect of alcohol indulgence (vs. restraint) on perceived change in cooperativeness; and mediated 71% of its effect on perceived change in moral character (Fitouchi et al., Reference Fitouchi, André, Baumard and Nettle2022).

This converges with historical and ethnographic evidence. Studies of especially acute moral crusades against drinking (“temperance movements,” nineteenth century) conclude that “in any place where temperance movements developed – alcohol was defined as dangerous, as a problem, in terms of its perceived ability to destroy individual self-control” (Levine, Reference Levine1993; see also Eisner, Reference Eisner2014; Yeomans, Reference Yeomans2011). As Gusfield (Reference Gusfield, Brandt and Rozin1997) summarizes,

Dominating the temperance ethic through much of the nineteenth and early twentieth century was a belief in the sinfulness and degradation of drinking per se. Use of spirits, beer, and wine was inherently threatening to the self-control that marked the moral and potentially successful person. Use endangered reputation, social standing, and income, and inevitably brought chronic inebriety and all its attendant harms. (Gusfield, Reference Gusfield, Brandt and Rozin1997, p. 213, emphases added)

Earlier in history, medieval and early modern moralists condemned alcohol for “extinguishing reason and dulling the mind,” making it “the gateway to other sins” (Martin, Reference Martin2009, p. 21; see also Adamson, Reference Adamson2004, p. 93). Similar attributions occur in medieval Chinese Buddhism (Sterckx, Reference Sterckx2005, pp. 224, 228). The Muslim Hadith similarly describes alcohol as “that which befogs the mind” (Michalak & Trocki, Reference Michalak and Trocki2006, p. 529), making it the “source (literally ‘mother’) of all evils” (Powell, Reference Powell and Robertson2004, p. 97). Further suggesting that moralizations of drinking aim at preventing loss of self-control, they often apply with varying intensity to different groups of people, as a function of their perceived likelihood to lose self-control. For example, the Konso agriculturalists (Ethiopia), who “know only too well what disruptive effects drunkenness can have on social relations,” reserved the right to consume alcohol to old men only, “supposed to be milder in their passions than the young, and more self-controlled” (Hallpike, Reference Hallpike2008, p. 219; see also traditional Middle East: Matthee, Reference Matthee2014, p. 104).

4.3. The praise of piety and ritual observance

MDT maintains that pious ritual observance is morally praised because it is perceived to cultivate self-control and thus cooperativeness (sect. 3.2.2). Future research could test whether ritual observance is more strongly moralized by people perceiving regular practice of a religious discipline, such as fasting, meditation, or regular prayer, as an efficient way to increase one's ability to resist temptations – including uncooperative ones. Several lines of evidence suggest the plausibility of this idea.

First, across countries with Hindu, Buddhist, Christian, and secular majorities (N > 3,200), religious people are perceived as less likely to commit uncooperative behaviors than non-religious people (Gervais, Reference Gervais2013, Reference Gervais2014; Gervais et al., Reference Gervais, Xygalatas, McKay, van Elk, Buchtel, Aveyard and Bulbulia2017), and part of this effect may stem from the perception that religious people regularly exercise self-control. Consistent with this possibility, experimental evidence indicates that religious people are perceived not only as more trustworthy, but also as more self-controlled (Moon, Krems, & Cohen, Reference Moon, Krems and Cohen2018), and that the perception that they are more self-controlled mediates the relationship between religiosity and greater perceived trustworthiness (Moon et al., Reference Moon, Krems and Cohen2018). Among religious people, those described as respecting rituals that require exercising self-control (e.g., abstaining from meat during Lent for Christians) are perceived as more trustworthy than religious people who do not, across religious affiliations (Hall, Cohen, Meyer, Varley, & Brewer, Reference Hall, Cohen, Meyer, Varley and Brewer2015; see also Singh & Henrich, Reference Singh and Henrich2020). Surveys of both Javanese Muslim and Christian American participants also find that a substantial share of both samples report increasing and expressing self-control as a motivation for fasting (Tamney, Reference Tamney1980, Reference Tamney1986).

These results converge with the fact that puritanical traditions explicitly ascribe self-control-training functions to ritual performance, with the specific aim of facilitating prosocial behavior. Specialists of Confucianism have long noted that “the function of rituals has been seen in China as a kind of block against or prevention of the influence of desires or selfish behavior” (Csikszentmihalyi, Reference Csikszentmihalyi, Lagerwey and Kalinowski2009, pp. 523–524). By cultivating self-discipline (Slingerland, Reference Slingerland2014, pp. 70–80), and “blocking the overflow of desires” (Csikszentmihalyi, Reference Csikszentmihalyi and Zalta2020, p. 7), ritual propriety was explicitly meant to facilitate prosocial behavior (Csikszentmihalyi, Reference Csikszentmihalyi2004, Reference Csikszentmihalyi, Lagerwey and Kalinowski2009, Reference Csikszentmihalyi and Zalta2020; Graziani, Reference Graziani2009; Slingerland, Reference Slingerland2014). Similarly in the Christian tradition: “as a Christian virtue, self-control is a product of spiritual discipline, a trait for which the Christian much engage in ‘strict training’” (Spiegel, Reference Spiegel2020, p. 1; see also Gaca, Reference Gaca2003; Gorski, Reference Gorski2003). Several ritual practices (e.g., fasting, mediation, self-denial) are “aimed at cultivating self-mastery or strength of will,” allowing to “get the victory over wickedness” or “train the soul to decline genuinely appealing immoral choices” (Spiegel, Reference Spiegel2020, pp. 1–12).

In ethnographic studies in Bhubaneswar (India), Odia Hindu report that daily performance of prayers and ablutions “teaches one to exercise self-control and enable one to cultivate self-discipline” (Menon, Reference Menon2013, p. 204) – qualities they also see as required for doing one's duties toward others (Menon, Reference Menon2013, pp. 201, 204). In Muslim Zanzibar, it is similarly acknowledged that the moral character trait of self-control “need[s] constant work and become[s] easier with growing age and piety” (Beckmann, Reference Beckmann2010, p. 120; Islam more generally: Garden, Reference Garden2014; Rehman, Reference Rehman2019). Among the Amhara (Ethiopia), “[t]he rigorous schedule of fasting is believed to help contain one's passions which could lead to an uncontrollable situation and eventual violence” (Reminick, Reference Reminick1975, p. 29). In Japanese Buddhist culture, practicing an “austere mental and physical discipline that one pursues for decades,” such as rituals or martial arts, is seen as allowing to “become a person of great discipline, character, and compassion,” who is “of far greater service to her neighbors” (Mann, Reference Mann2011, pp. 74–77).

The moral disciplining theory could further be tested by quantitatively investigating the cross-cultural association between the moral praise of ritual observance and such ascriptions of moral disciplining functions to ritual performance.

4.4. The condemnation of immodest clothing, music, and dances

MDT proposes that, to prevent social harm, puritanical values do not only prescribe behaviors viewed as improving people's “inner” self-control (e.g., disciplinary rituals, restraint from bodily pleasures), but also use what psychologists call “preventive” or “situational” strategies for self-control (Duckworth et al., Reference Duckworth, Gendler and Gross2016a, Reference Duckworth, White, Matteucci, Shearer and Gross2016b; Hofmann & Kotabe, Reference Hofmann and Kotabe2012). Such strategies aim at lowering the demand for self-control by preventing temptations to arise in the first place (Duckworth et al., Reference Duckworth, Gendler and Gross2016a, Reference Duckworth, White, Matteucci, Shearer and Gross2016b). This, we have argued, is what modesty norms do by limiting exposure to sexual cues (e.g., female body curves), perceived as triggering hard-to-control sex drives in impulsive males, potentially resulting in uncooperative behaviors (e.g., adultery, sexual aggression) (sect. 3.2.1). This hypothesis generates the following predictions.

4.4.1. The more people perceive male self-control as vulnerable to cue exposure, the more they should moralize immodesty

If immodesty is moralized as a risk factor for harmful self-control failures, it should be all the more condemned that people perceive surrounding males as unable to resist sexual urges in the face of cue exposure. Indeed, if males are believed able to remain peaceful and self-controlled even when exposed to tempting cues, immodesty should lose its (perceived) potential to generate social harm, and should therefore not be condemned. In line with this prediction, Moon et al. (Reference Moon, Wongsomboon and Sevi2021) show that the more people stereotype men as unable to control their sexual urges, the more they moralize female immodest clothing and other cue-exposing behaviors (e.g., public breastfeeding). This effect does not generalize to phenomena unrelated to cue exposure, and holds after controlling for participants' reproductive strategy and other potential confounds (e.g., religiosity, conservatism, benevolent or hostile sexism). This suggests that, although inherently harmless, immodesty is moralized because of its perceived potential implications on male moral self-control (Moon et al., Reference Moon, Wongsomboon and Sevi2021).

4.4.2. Modesty norms should be well designed to prevent cue exposure specifically

Second, modesty norms should specifically target behaviors increasing cue exposure. This seems to be the case. Eye-tracking experiments show that Islamic clothing decreases visual access to female body curves (Pazhoohi, Macedo, & Arantes, Reference Pazhoohi, Macedo and Arantes2017b), and accordingly decrease males' rating of female attractiveness (Mahmud & Swami, Reference Mahmud and Swami2009; Pazhoohi & Hosseinchari, Reference Pazhoohi and Hosseinchari2014; Sheen, Yekani, & Jordan, Reference Sheen, Yekani and Jordan2018). Modest clothing across puritanical traditions specifically hide such sexually arousing stimuli (e.g., Puritans’ austere clothing: Bremer, Reference Bremer2009; Islamic veiling: Mernissi, Reference Mernissi2011; Jewish Tznihut dress: Andrews, Reference Andrews2010; Hindu India: Stephens, Reference Stephens1972, p. 4; ancient Christian veiling: Tariq, Reference Tariq2014). Psychological evidence indicates that immodesty is more negatively viewed in public – that is, when its cue-exposing effects are amplified (Acker, Reference Acker2009). The Quran also specifies that elderly women, deemed less attractive, can go unveiled (Mernissi, Reference Mernissi2011), and studies of rural Muslim villages report that demands of modesty apply less strongly to elderly women (Antoun, Reference Antoun1968, p. 683; Belghiti, Reference Belghiti1969).

Music and dance, similarly, are often condemned as exposing people to auditory and visual stimuli impeding self-control, thus making antisocial behaviors more likely. Islamic warning against the dangers of music insist on its self-control-impeding effects: “music as an activity is about exciting pleasures that make humans slaves to unruly passions” (Kiyimba, Reference Kiyimba, Nannyonga-Tamusuza and Solomon2012, pp. 93–94); and compare music's psychological effects to those of alcohol: “avoid singing for it decreases shame, increases desire…and verily it takes the place of wine and does what drunkenness does” (Otterbeck & Ackfeldt, Reference Otterbeck and Ackfeldt2012, p. 232). Christian oppositions to dancing often specifically targeted the “mixed” dancing of men and women, exposing people to sexual cues and “lascivious” bodily movements (Wagner, Reference Wagner1997). In early China, Confucius condemned the “immoral, seductive popular music of Zheng,” which “was sung by mixed groups of men and women, and gave rise to sexual improprieties” (Slingerland, Reference Slingerland2014, p. 76). In early modern Europe, “plays, songs and, above all, dances were condemned for awakening dangerous emotions and as incitement to fornication” (Burke, Reference Burke1978, p. 212), whereas festivals were denounced as “occasions of violence” (Burke, Reference Burke1978, p. 212). Tellingly, the very same entertainment – music – can become morally praised when it takes the form of a disciplined, effortful, and patient activity perceived as cultivating people's self-control rather than impeding it, thus facilitating prosociality. For example, although he condemned arousing popular music, Confucius prescribed classical musical performance, precisely because the latter “place[s] limits on appetitive desires” and thus “promote[s] unselfishness…and prepare[s] people to behave morally in different domains of their life” (Csikszentmihalyi, Reference Csikszentmihalyi and Zalta2020, pp. 7, 5).

5. Explaining the fall of puritanism

Although widespread, puritanical values are not ubiquitous. Their most systematically documented pattern of variation is their decline in particularly rich, WEIRD societies. In line with cultural psychological studies (Atari et al., Reference Atari, Haidt, Graham, Koleva, Stevens and Dehghani2022; Haidt et al., Reference Haidt, Koller and Dias1993; Shweder et al., Reference Shweder, Mahapatra, Miller, Kagan and Lamb1987), the World Value Surveys demonstrate that virtually all societies, when they are made richer by modern economic development, progressively abandon puritanical values (Inglehart, Reference Inglehart2018; Inglehart & Baker, Reference Inglehart and Baker2000). The richest regions of the world (e.g., Western Europe, North America, Australia/New Zealand) show the lowest levels of puritanical values, whereas the poorest regions (e.g., Africa, Middle East, Communist Asia) exhibit the highest levels (Weeden & Kurzban, Reference Weeden and Kurzban2013). Why does puritanism decline in particularly rich, WEIRD societies?

MDT maintains that people promote puritanical norms to ensure the self-control necessary for cooperative behavior. Puritanical norms, however, have a cost: They restrict people's enjoyment of highly gratifying pleasures, and impose effortful disciplinary activities. Thus, puritanical norms should be perceived as morally warranted only when they are worth this cost. This should especially be the case in environments where people's spontaneous self-control is perceived as insufficient to ensure social order and acceptable levels of cooperation – making puritanical norms, precisely aimed at supplementing this fragile self-control, appear necessary. In fact, historians and social scientists have stressed that Puritans' zealous emphasis on maintaining constant self-control was tied to an “extraordinary fear of disorder and anarchy” (Walzer, Reference Walzer1963, p. 84), alimented by a pessimistic view of human nature as naturally weak-willed, driven by powerful impulses, and perpetually tempted by selfishness (Luttmer, Reference Luttmer2000; Seidman, Reference Seidman1990; Sherkat & Ellison, Reference Sherkat and Ellison1997; Walzer, Reference Walzer1963, Reference Walzer1982).

This reasoning closely parallels the logic of variations in authoritarian values (Nettle & Saxe, Reference Nettle and Saxe2021), which are associated with puritanical values (Atari et al., Reference Atari, Haidt, Graham, Koleva, Stevens and Dehghani2022; Harper & Rhodes, Reference Harper and Rhodes2021). Nettle and Saxe (Reference Nettle and Saxe2021) present experimental and cross-national evidence that, in poorer environments, people are more supportive of authoritarian leaders because they expect other people to spontaneously behave less cooperatively. This lower trust in others leads people to view strong leaders, who monitor and punish cheating intransigently, as necessary to ensure acceptable levels of cooperation (Nettle & Saxe, Reference Nettle and Saxe2021). Just as monitoring and punishment by authoritarian leaders appears less necessary in particularly rich environments, where people view others as spontaneously cooperative (Nettle & Saxe, Reference Nettle and Saxe2021), we argue that puritanical norms, aimed at disciplining others for cooperation, become unnecessary when people see others as spontaneously self-controlled and trustworthy.

In line with this idea, the particularly rich environments in which puritanism declines lead to the development of more inherently self-controlled psychologies. People living in materially safer environments, compared to people living in poverty, are spontaneously more self-controlled (Dohmen, Enke, Falk, Huffman, & Sunde, Reference Dohmen, Enke, Falk, Huffman and Sunde2018; Pepper & Nettle, Reference Pepper and Nettle2017; Sheehy-Skeffington, Reference Sheehy-Skeffington2020), invest more in extended prosociality (Holland, Silva, & Mace, Reference Holland, Silva and Mace2012; Lettinga, Jacquet, André, Baumand, & Chevallier, Reference Lettinga, Jacquet, André, Baumand and Chevallier2020; Nettle, Reference Nettle2015; Silva & Mace, Reference Silva and Mace2014; Zwirner & Raihani, Reference Zwirner and Raihani2020), are less susceptible to impulsive defection or retaliation (McCullough, Pedersen, Schroder, Tabak, & Carver, Reference McCullough, Pedersen, Schroder, Tabak and Carver2012), and have higher trust in others (Alesina & La Ferrara, Reference Alesina and La Ferrara2002; Guillou, Grandin, & Chevallier, Reference Guillou, Grandin and Chevallier2021; Ortiz-Ospina & Roser, Reference Ortiz-Ospina and Roser2016; Petersen & Aarøe, Reference Petersen and Aarøe2015).

Material security and higher levels of self-control are also associated with lower spontaneous engagement in the very self-control-impeding behaviors that puritanical norms preemptively restrict. Richer individuals are less susceptible to heavy drinking (Huckle, You, & Casswell, Reference Huckle, You and Casswell2010; Lewer, Meier, Beard, Boniface, & Kaner, Reference Lewer, Meier, Beard, Boniface and Kaner2016), which in turn predicts less alcohol-related “undesirable” outcomes (e.g., physical fights, being away from work) (Huckle et al., Reference Huckle, You and Casswell2010). Higher socioeconomic status (SES) predicts lower reward-sensitivity and greater inhibition (Yaple & Yu, Reference Yaple and Yu2020), which are associated with lower vulnerability to addiction problems and cue exposure (Auger, Lo, Cantinotti, & O'Loughlin, Reference Auger, Lo, Cantinotti and O'Loughlin2010; Gullo, Loxton, & Dawe, Reference Gullo, Loxton and Dawe2014; Osadchiy et al., Reference Osadchiy, Mayer, Bhatt, Labus, Gao, Kilpatrick and Gupta2019; Story et al., Reference Story, Vlaev, Seymour, Darzi and Dolan2014; Thomsen et al., Reference Thomsen, Callesen, Hesse, Kvamme, Pedersen, Pedersen and Voon2018; Volkow et al., Reference Volkow, Wise and Baler2017). Urges for temperance likely become useless when people are more moderate anyway, and less likely to develop hard-to-control addictions to bodily pleasures. Prohibitions of alcohol likely become superfluous when people are less susceptible to heavy drinking, and when drinking generates less social problems anyway than in poorer societies (e.g., medieval Europe: Eisner, Reference Eisner2001, Reference Eisner2003, Reference Eisner2014; Martin, Reference Martin2009). Prescriptions of religious techniques of self-discipline likely seem unnecessary when people perceive others as disciplined enough to honor their duties.

This hypothesis generates testable predictions: Puritanical values should decline when surrounding individuals are perceived as particularly self-controlled and trustworthy. In line with this prediction, recent analyses of the World Value Survey (N > 200,000) show that, across more than 100 countries, individuals with puritanical values have lower trust in others – that is, more belief that surrounding individuals are not spontaneously cooperative. As mentioned above, the more people perceive males as spontaneously sexually self-controlled, the less they support puritanical restrictions of immodesty (Moon et al., Reference Moon, Wongsomboon and Sevi2021). In the United States, van Leeuwen, Koenig, Graham, and Park (Reference van Leeuwen, Koenig, Graham and Park2014) find that living in a state where many people have short-term-oriented “life-history strategies” (which relate to lower self-control: Pepper & Nettle, Reference Pepper and Nettle2017) predicts individual endorsement of purity values more strongly and robustly than any other predictor – including pathogen prevalence, relevant to disgust-based accounts of purity, but also urbanization, education, social class, and cognitive ability. Similarly, more religious and conservative American states, which are more puritanical, have been found to exhibit the greatest levels of pornography use (Edelman, Reference Edelman2009; MacInnis & Hodson, Reference MacInnis and Hodson2015; Whitehead & Perry, Reference Whitehead and Perry2018), and prevalence of pornography use in 20 Arab-Muslim countries (N > 15,000) has been found to be higher than in some less puritanical countries (e.g., Australia, Italy) (Eljawad et al., Reference Eljawad, Se'eda, Ghozy, El-Qushayri, Elsherif, Elkassar and Islam2021). Recent studies find that people holding “binding moral foundations,” which include purity values, tend to be less self-controlled than people holding liberal, less puritanical values (Silver & Silver, Reference Silver and Silver2019). The moral disciplining model allows making sense of these apparently paradoxical findings, previously seen as left unexplained (see Silver & Silver, Reference Silver and Silver2019; van Leeuwen et al., Reference van Leeuwen, Koenig, Graham and Park2014): The need to moralize bodily pleasures and self-discipline is felt more strongly when people perceive, by observing others' behaviors or extrapolating from their own psychology, that surrounding individuals' ability to resist temptations is not guaranteed, and thus needs to be compensated by puritanical restrictions.

6. Extending and discussing the disciplining account

6.1. Self-control and other moralizations

In this section, we further illustrate the fecundity of the moral disciplining approach, by showing how it can explain other purity-related moral judgments beyond those considered so far in this article.

6.1.1. Hygiene norms

Moral psychologists often define purity as condemning not only bodily pleasures and undisciplined lifestyles, but also dirty and unclean behaviors (Graham et al., Reference Graham, Haidt, Koleva, Motyl, Iyer, Wojcik and Ditto2013; Gray et al., Reference Gray, DiMaggio, Schein and Kachanoff2022; Kollareth et al., Reference Kollareth, Brownell, Duran and Russell2022). Moral prescriptions of hygiene may seem, in line with moral foundations theory (Graham et al., Reference Graham, Haidt, Koleva, Motyl, Iyer, Wojcik and Ditto2013), straightforwardly related to disgust and pathogen-avoidance. Yet recent evidence suggests the fecundity of the moral disciplining framework to explain even this part of purity concerns. In almost 20,000 participants across 56 countries, the restrictiveness of hygiene norms (e.g., against spitting, for handwashing) is more strongly and robustly predicted by the valorization of self-control than by perceived pathogen threat and historical pathogen prevalence – whose effect on the main factor of hygiene norms disappears when self-control values are controlled for (Eriksson, Dickins, & Strimling, Reference Eriksson, Dickins and Strimling2021). This suggests that not only ascetic restraint, but also the conscientious observance of hygienic practices, may be moralized as an exercise of self-discipline.

6.1.2. “Impure thoughts” and the moralization of mental intimacy

Puritanical traditions can go as far as moralizing, not only victimless behaviors and private lifestyles, but also the very mental states individuals experience, such as the “impure” thoughts and desires they may entertain. In psychological experiments, Christians and Protestant participants judge some mental states (e.g., adulterous desire) to be as immoral as the behaviors that could follow them (Cohen, Reference Cohen2003; Cohen & Rozin, Reference Cohen and Rozin2001; Siev & Cohen, Reference Siev and Cohen2007). Early Church fathers (e.g., Paul, Matthew) insisted that a man looking with desire at a woman already commits adultery “in his heart” (Gaca, Reference Gaca2003, pp. 152–153). From the thirteenth century onward, Christians were obligated to confess not only their immoral actions but also their sinful thoughts (Tentler, Reference Tentler2015). In the Muslim tradition, the condemnation of zina (unlawful, non-marital sex) also extends to mental states such as looking at the body curves of a woman (“zina of the eye”) or hearing an unrelated woman's voice (“zina of the ear”) (Bouhdiba, Reference Bouhdiba2012, pp. 38–39; see also Rabbinic Judaism: Hezser, Reference Hezser2018, pp. 15–16).

The moral disciplining account helps explain this phenomenon as a special case of prevention of harmful self-control failures. Picturing oneself enjoying a reward, for example in adulterous or violent thoughts, may be perceived as triggering urges to consume this reward (e.g., sexual pleasure), making people more likely to succumb harmful temptations (e.g., adultery, assault). As written in the most popular Christian devotional book (The Imitation of Christ, fifteenth century), “first there comes into the mind an evil thought: next, a [vivid] picture: then delight, and urge to evil” (Tentler, Reference Tentler2015, p. 156). According to our account, although inherently harmless, mental states and desires are moralized when perceived as critically increasing the probability of harmful self-control failures.

In line with this idea, the puritanical moralization of mental states is often associated with prescriptions of “intra-psychic” self-control techniques (see Duckworth et al., Reference Duckworth, Gendler and Gross2016a, Reference Duckworth, White, Matteucci, Shearer and Gross2016b), aimed at detecting the birth of impulses in one's consciousness to facilitate their self-control. For example, historians have noted that “[i]t was characteristic of puritans to subject themselves to intense self-examination” (Bremer, Reference Bremer2009, p. 38). By meditating frequently, and recording his impulses in moral diaries (Bremer, Reference Bremer2009; Gorski, Reference Gorski1993, Reference Gorski2003), the individual “was to reflect on the sins he was especially prone to commit so that he might guard against those urges” (Bremer, Reference Bremer2009, p. 55). In the Christian tradition more generally, meditation has been construed as a self-control technique that “kills temptation at the root, by producing a mental ‘soil’ that will not sustain the development of illicit desires that lead to vice” (Spiegel, Reference Spiegel2020, p. 12). Neo-Confucian traditions under China's Song, Yuan, and Ming dynasties (tenth to seventeenth centuries) similarly adopted and adapted techniques of meditation used to “detect and undercut selfish inclinations and desires” (Tiwald, Reference Tiwald and Zalta2020). In line with these cases, psychological evidence suggests that the tendency to attribute to others the ability to internally control their mental states and impulses predicts the propensity to moralize mental states (Weiss, Forstmann, & Burgmer, Reference Weiss, Forstmann and Burgmer2021).

6.2. Outstanding questions

Purity was originally introduced to moral psychology to raise awareness on the cross-cultural variation of morality: Beyond the harm- and fairness-centered values of WEIRD societies, morality involves, in more traditional societies, temperance, chastity, and piety (Haidt, Reference Haidt2012; Haidt et al., Reference Haidt, Koller and Dias1993; Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, Reference Henrich, Heine and Norenzayan2010; Shweder et al., Reference Shweder, Mahapatra, Miller, Kagan and Lamb1987). This cross-cultural revolution in moral psychology was essential, and led to systematic documentation of the fall of puritanical values in modern societies (see sect. 5; Atari et al., Reference Atari, Haidt, Graham, Koleva, Stevens and Dehghani2022; Haidt et al., Reference Haidt, Koller and Dias1993). This movement, we suggest, should go one step further.

Indeed, not all traditional societies are puritanical. Available cross-cultural codes suggest that small-scale societies, somewhat similarly to WEIRD societies, exhibit less moral emphasis on sexual restraint and modesty than larger-scale, traditional societies with world religions (Jackson, Gelfand, & Ember, Reference Jackson, Gelfand and Ember2020; Murdock, Reference Murdock1949; Stephens, Reference Stephens1972). Ethnographers have reported lax attitudes toward bodily pleasure in various small-scale societies (e.g., !Kung: Lee, Reference Lee2013; Azande, Central Africa: Evans-Pritchard, Reference Evans-Pritchard1973; Nivkh: Shternberg, Reference Shternberg1933; Chuckchee, northeast Asia: Broude, Reference Broude and Ember1996; Trobriander, Papua New Guinea: Malinowski, Reference Malinowski1929; Weiner, Reference Weiner1988; Intuit: Hoebel, Reference Hoebel2009). Even in larger-scale traditional societies, puritanical values seem not to always have been so prevalent than in the societies mentioned by initial emphases on purity (e.g., contemporary India, rural Brazil; Haidt, Reference Haidt2012; Haidt et al., Reference Haidt, Koller and Dias1993; Shweder et al., Reference Shweder, Mahapatra, Miller, Kagan and Lamb1987). Puritanical values appear to have increased in ancient Rome between the late Republic and the early Empire (Duby, Pantel, Thébaud, & Perrot, Reference Duby, Pantel, Thébaud and Perrot1994; Norena, Reference Norena2007; Rousselle, Reference Rousselle2013; Veyne, Reference Veyne1978; see also Gaca, Reference Gaca2003). In China, although bodily pleasures appear less restricted in antiquity (Goldin, Reference Goldin2017; Hinsch, Reference Hinsch1994; Wells & Yao, Reference Wells, Yao and Benjamin2018), starting with the Tang and continuing through the Song, the Yuan, the Ming, and the Qing, self-discipline, bodily pleasures, and asceticism are increasingly moralized (Benn, Reference Benn and Sterckx2005; Sommer, Reference Sommer2000; Suiming, Reference Suiming1998; Wells & Yao, Reference Wells, Yao and Benjamin2018; Yü, Reference Yü2021). In medieval Europe, historical work similarly documents an increasingly strict policing of lack of self-control, sexual misconducts, alcohol consumption, and lax religious observance, culminating in the moralistic religious movements of the early modern period (Burke, Reference Burke1978; Ingram, Reference Ingram1990, Reference Ingram, Griffiths, Fox and Hindle1996; Martin, Reference Martin2009; McIntosh, Reference McIntosh2002).

In other words, the focus on the WEIRD/non-WEIRD dichotomy (Henrich et al., Reference Henrich, Heine and Norenzayan2010) may have obscured substantial variation in puritanical values among non-WEIRD societies themselves, potentially limiting our understanding of the cross-cultural variation that psychological theories must account for. Furthering our understanding of the psychological origins of puritanical values, and testing predictions of various theories, requires the field to move forward a systematic, quantitative documentation of the full spectrum of puritanical values' variations across human societies.

7. Conclusion

Many societies develop apparently unnecessarily austere norms, depriving people from the harmless pleasures of life. In face of the apparent disconnect of puritanical values from cooperation, the latter have either been ignored by cooperation-centered theories of morality, or been explained by mechanisms orthogonal to cooperative challenges, such as concerns for the purity of the soul, rooted in disgust intuitions. We have argued for a theoretical reintegration of puritanical morality in the otherwise theoretically grounded and empirically supported perspective of morality as cooperation. For deep evolutionary reasons, cooperation as a long-term strategy requires resisting impulses for immediate pleasures. To protect cooperative interactions from the threat of temptation, many societies develop preemptive moralizations aimed at facilitating moral self-control. This may explain why, aside from values of fairness, reciprocity, solidarity, or loyalty, many societies develop hedonically restrictive standards of sobriety, asceticism, temperance, modesty, piety, and self-discipline.

Acknowledgments

We thank Daniel Nettle, Hugo Mercier, Manvir Singh, Amine Sijilmassi, and four anonymous reviewers for their very thoughtful feedback on previous versions of this manuscript.

Financial support

This work was supported by the EUR FrontCog grant ANR-17-EURE-0017.

Competing interest

None.

Footnotes

*

These authors contributed equally.

1. Adultery is an instance of cheating in the context of pair-bonding and marriage – a cooperative interaction on reproduction, resource production, and parental investment (Gurven et al., Reference Gurven, Winking, Kaplan, von Rueden and McAllister2009). Committed pair-bonds enjoy mutual benefits of parental certainty and greater efficiency in child care (Chapais, Reference Chapais2009; Gurven et al., Reference Gurven, Winking, Kaplan, von Rueden and McAllister2009). Yet, just as in other cooperation dilemmas (Rand & Nowak, Reference Rand and Nowak2013), they also face short-term incentives to cheat by taking advantage of their partner's exclusive provision of benefits (fidelity), while not reciprocating it to reap the benefits of extra-pair mating (see Buss & Schmitt, Reference Buss and Schmitt1993; Gangestad & Simpson, Reference Gangestad and Simpson2000). Adultery, thus, amounts to take the benefits of one's partner cooperation (fidelity) while not oneself paying the costs of cooperation (by oneself remaining faithful) – and is accordingly nearly universally condemned (Broude & Greene, Reference Broude and Greene1976; Murdock, Reference Murdock1949; Poushter, Reference Poushter2014). This is not to deny the often-patriarchal nature of adultery proscriptions, which often sanction female's infidelity more strongly than male adultery (Broude & Greene, Reference Broude and Greene1976), and often take the form of prohibitions for men to steal each other's wife, treating women as the property of their husband, father, or brothers (Dabhoiwala, Reference Dabhoiwala2012, p. 5). Such patriarchal norms are likely grounded in males' interests to police female sexuality (see sect. 2.2).

2. Not all cooperation-based theories of morality are unitary, and not all unitary theories are cooperation-based. Some theories, such as the morality-as-cooperation framework (Curry, Reference Curry, Shackelford and Hansen2016), view morality as functioning entirely for cooperation, yet slice morality into multiple domains corresponding to distinct domains of cooperation (see also Cosmides, Guzmán, & Tooby, Reference Cosmides, Guzmán and Tooby2018). Conversely, some theories maintain that morality did not evolve for cooperation, but rather to advance condemners' self-interest, yet regard morality as a functionally unitary cognitive mechanism (DeScioli & Kurzban, Reference DeScioli and Kurzban2009, Reference DeScioli and Kurzban2013).

3. Premarital sex also amounts to cheating other people in some social contexts. In many societies, marriage is not only a cooperative interaction between spouses, but also a way to forge social exchange relationships between families (Coontz, Reference Coontz2006; Schlegel, Reference Schlegel1991). In this context, young people's premarital sex inflicts important costs to families, for example, by decreasing daughters' “value” on the matrimonial market (Beckmann, Reference Beckmann2010, p. 623; Dickemann, Reference Dickemann, Alexander and Tinkle1981; LeVine, Reference LeVine1959, pp. 978–979), or leading to costly pregnancies out of wedlock or to unwanted marriages (Broude, Reference Broude and Ember1996; Dabhoiwala, Reference Dabhoiwala2012, pp. 29–30; Goody, Reference Goody1976, p. 17). As a result, everyone may benefit if everyone limits premarital promiscuity, which, as a threat to the public good, is collectively condemned and brings a bad moral reputation to “fornicators” and their relatives (Beckmann, Reference Beckmann2010, pp. 622–623; Dabhoiwala, Reference Dabhoiwala2012, p. 618).

References

Aarøe, L., & Petersen, M. B. (2014). Crowding out culture: Scandinavians and Americans agree on social welfare in the face of deservingness cues. The Journal of Politics, 76(3), 684697. https://doi.org/10.1017/S002238161400019XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abrams, S., Jackson, J. C., Vonasch, A., & Gray, K. (2020). Moralization of religiosity explains worldwide trends in religious belief [preprint]. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/5a2dbCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abu-Lughod, L. (2016). Veiled sentiments honor and poetry in a Bedouin society. University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Acker, M. (2009). Breast is best…but not everywhere: Ambivalent sexism and attitudes toward private and public breastfeeding. Sex Roles, 61(7), 476490. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-009-9655-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adamson, M. W. (2004). Food in medieval times. Greenwood Press.Google Scholar
Ainslie, G. (2013). Intertemporal bargaining predicts moral behavior, even in anonymous, one-shot economic games. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(1), 7879.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alesina, A., & La Ferrara, E. (2002). Who trusts others? Journal of Public Economics, 85(2), 207234. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0047-2727(01)00084-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alexander, R. D. (1987). The biology of moral systems. de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Alós-Ferrer, C., & Garagnani, M. (2020). The cognitive foundations of cooperation. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 175, 7185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2020.04.019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
André, J.-B., Fitouchi, L., Debove, S., & Baumard, N. (2022). An evolutionary contractualist theory of morality [preprint]. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/2hxguCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrews, C. (2010). Defining and exploring modesty in Jewish American women. Journal of Religion and Health, 50, 818834. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10943-010-9435-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrighetto, G., Capraro, V., Guido, A., & Szekely, A. (2020). Cooperation, response time, and social value orientation: A meta-analysis [preprint]. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/cbakzCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antoun, R. T. (1968). On the modesty of women in Arab Muslim villages: A study in the accommodation of traditions. American Anthropologist, 70(4), 671697. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1968.70.4.02a00010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ariely, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2006). The heat of the moment: The effect of sexual arousal on sexual decision making. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19(2), 8798. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.501CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, T., Wilbanks, D., Leong, D., & Hsu, K. J. (2020). Is there a measurement crisis in disgust research? [preprint]. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/a8u5mCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atari, M., Haidt, J., Graham, J., Koleva, S., Stevens, S. T., & Dehghani, M. (2022). Morality beyond the WEIRD: How the nomological network of morality varies across cultures. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/q6c9rCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auger, N., Lo, E., Cantinotti, M., & O'Loughlin, J. (2010). Impulsivity and socio-economic status interact to increase the risk of gambling onset among youth. Addiction, 105(12), 21762183. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03100.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aumann, R. J., & Shapley, L. S. (1994). Long-term competition – A game-theoretic analysis. In Megiddo, N. (Ed.), Essays in game theory (pp. 115). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-2648-2_1Google Scholar
Axelrod, R. (1984). The evolution of cooperation. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Axelrod, R., & Hamilton, W. D. (1981). The evolution of cooperation.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Axelrod, R. M. (2006). The evolution of cooperation (Rev. ed). Basic Books.Google Scholar
Baler, R., & Volkow, N. (2007). Drug addiction: The neurobiology of disrupted self-control. Trends in Molecular Medicine, 12, 559566. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.molmed.2006.10.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barclay, P. (2013). Strategies for cooperation in biological markets, especially for humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(3), 164175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.02.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barton-Crosby, J., & Hirtenlehner, H. (2021). The role of morality and self-control in conditioning the criminogenic effect of provocation. A partial test of situational action theory. Deviant Behavior, 42(10), 12731294. https://doi.org/10.1080/01639625.2020.1738645CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumard, N. (2016). The origins of fairness: How evolution explains our moral nature. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baumard, N., André, J.-B., & Sperber, D. (2013). A mutualistic approach to morality: The evolution of fairness by partner choice. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(1), 5978. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11002202CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumard, N., & Chevallier, C. (2015). The nature and dynamics of world religions: A life-history approach. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282(1818), 20151593. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.1593CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beal, B. (2020). What are the irreducible basic elements of morality? A critique of the debate over monism and pluralism in moral psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(2), 273290. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619867106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Becker, A. (2019). On the economic origins of restrictions on women's sexuality.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beckmann, N. (2010). Pleasure and danger: Muslim views on sex and gender in Zanzibar. Culture, Health & Sexuality, 12(6), 619632. https://doi.org/10.1080/13691051003663713CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belghiti, M. (1969). Les relations féminines et le statut de la femme dans la famille rurale dans trois villages de la Tessaout. Etudes sociologiques sur le Maroc. In Bulletin économique et social du Maroc (pp. 289361). Societé d'études économiques, sociales et statistiques.Google Scholar
Benn, J. A. (2005). Buddhism, alcohol, and tea in medieval China. In Sterckx, R. (Ed.), Of tripod and palate (pp. 213236). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berkman, E. T. (2016). Self-regulation training. In Vohs, K. D. & Baumeister, R. F. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 440457). Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
Blake, K. R., Fourati, M., & Brooks, R. C. (2018). Who suppresses female sexuality? An examination of support for Islamic veiling in a secular Muslim democracy as a function of sex and offspring sex. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(6), 632638. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.06.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, P. (2004). Descartes’ baby: How the science of child development explains what makes us human (pp. xv, 271). Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2013). Just babies: The origins of good and evil. Crown/Random House.Google Scholar
Boehm, C. (2012). Moral origins: The evolution of virtue, altruism, and shame. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Boswell, R. G., & Kober, H. (2016). Food cue reactivity and craving predict eating and weight gain: A meta-analytic review. Obesity Reviews, 17(2), 159177. https://doi.org/10.1111/obr.12354CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouhdiba, A. (2012). Sexuality in Islam. Saqi Books.Google Scholar
Bouwmeester, S., Verkoeijen, P. P. J. L., Aczel, B., Barbosa, F., Bègue, L., Brañas-Garza, P., … Wollbrant, C. E. (2017). Registered replication report: Rand, Greene, and Nowak (2012). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(3), 527542. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617693624CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1992). Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizable groups. Ethology and Sociobiology, 13(3), 171195. https://doi.org/10.1016/0162-3095(92)90032-YCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2001). Norms and bounded rationality. In Gigerenzer, G. & Selten, R. (Eds.), Bounded rationality: The adaptive toolbox (pp. 281296). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Brady, A., Baker, L. R., & Miller, R. S. (2020). Look but don't touch?: Self-regulation determines whether noticing attractive alternatives increases infidelity. Journal of Family Psychology, 34(2), 135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bremer, F. J. (2009). Puritanism: A very short introduction. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brett, E. I., Leavens, E. L., Miller, M. B., Lombardi, N., & Leffingwell, T. R. (2016). Normative perceptions of alcohol-related consequences among college students. Addictive Behaviors, 58, 1620. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.02.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brokaw, C. J. (2014). The ledgers of merit and demerit: Social change and moral order in late imperial China. University Press.Google Scholar
Broude, G. J. (1996). Variations in sexual attitudes, norms and practices. In Ember, C. R. (Ed.), Cross-cultural research for social sciences (pp. 123151). Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Broude, G. J., & Greene, S. J. (1976). Cross-cultural codes on twenty sexual attitudes and practices. Ethnology, 15(4), 409429. https://doi.org/10.2307/3773308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, P. (2012). The rise of Western Christendom: Triumph and diversity, A.D. 200–1000. Wiley.Google Scholar
Burke, P. (1978). Popular culture in early modern Europe. Harper Torchbooks.Google Scholar
Burnette, J. L., Davisson, E. K., Finkel, E. J., Van Tongeren, D. R., Hui, C. M., & Hoyle, R. H. (2014). Self-control and forgiveness: A meta-analytic review. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5(4), 443450. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550613502991CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100(2), 204232. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.100.2.204CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buyukcan-Tetik, A., Finkenauer, C., Siersema, M., Vander Heyden, K., & Krabbendam, L. (2015). Social relations model analyses of perceived self-control and trust in families. Journal of Marriage and Family, 77(1), 209223. https://doi.org/10.1111/jomf.12154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buyukcan-Tetik, A., & Pronk, T. (2021). Partner self-control and intrusive behaviors: A gender-specific examination of the mediating role of trust. Current Psychology, 3, 111. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-02462-4Google Scholar
Bynum, C. W. (2000). Holy feast and holy fast: The religious significance of food to medieval women (paperback print, [reprinted]). University of California Press.Google Scholar
Camerer, C. F., Dreber, A., Holzmeister, F., Ho, T.-H., Huber, J., Johannesson, M., … Wu, H. (2018). Evaluating the replicability of social science experiments in nature and science between 2010 and 2015. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(9), 637644. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0399-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, S. M., & Beck, D. M. (2001). Symbols as tools in the development of executive function. In Winsler, A., Fernyhough, C., & Montero, I. (Eds.), Private speech, executive functioning, and the development of verbal self-regulation (pp. 163175). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511581533.014Google Scholar
Celniker, J. B., Gregory, A., Koo, H. J., Piff, P. K., Ditto, P. H., & Shariff, A. F. (2023). The moralization of effort. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 152(1), 6079.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chakroff, A., Russell, P. S., Piazza, J., & Young, L. (2017). From impure to harmful: Asymmetric expectations about immoral agents. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 69, 201209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2016.08.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapais, B. (2009). Primeval kinship: How pair-bonding gave birth to human society. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheng, J. S., Ottati, V. C., & Price, E. D. (2013). The arousal model of moral condemnation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(6), 10121018. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.06.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, C. A. (1932). Religions of old Korea. Fleming H. Revell. https://ehrafworldcultures-yale-edu.proxy.library.upenn.edu/document?id=aa01-005Google Scholar
Clifford, S., Iyengar, V., Cabeza, R., & Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (2015). Moral foundations vignettes: A standardized stimulus database of scenarios based on moral foundations theory. Behavior Research Methods, 47(4), 11781198. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-014-0551-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, A. B. (2003). Religion, likelihood of action, and the morality of mentality. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 13(4), 273285. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327582IJPR1304_4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, A. B., & Rozin, P. (2001). Religion and the morality of mentality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(4), 697710. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.4.697CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, T. R., Panter, A. T., Turan, N., Morse, L., & Kim, Y. (2014). Moral character in the workplace. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(5), 943.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coontz, S. (2006). Marriage, a history: How love conquered marriage. Penguin.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L., Guzmán, R. A., & Tooby, J. (2018). The evolution of moral cognition. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coveney, J. (2006). Food, morals and meaning: The pleasure and anxieties of eating. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crawford, J. T., Inbar, Y., & Maloney, V. (2014). Disgust sensitivity selectively predicts attitudes toward groups that threaten (or uphold) traditional sexual morality. Personality and Individual Differences, 70, 218223. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.07.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Critchlow, B. (1986). The powers of John Barleycorn: Beliefs about the effects of alcohol on social behavior. American Psychologist, 41(7), 751764.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crone, D. (2022). Conceptual issues with the moral foundation of purity: The case of religion. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/3e8bvCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cruz, J. M., Boidi, M. F., & Queirolo, R. (2018). Saying no to weed: Public opinion towards cannabis legalisation in Uruguay. Drugs: Education, Prevention and Policy, 25(1), 6776. https://doi.org/10.1080/09687637.2016.1237475Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2004). Material virtue: Ethics and the body in early China. Brill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2009). Ethics and self-cultivation practice in early China. In Lagerwey, J. & Kalinowski, M. (Eds.), Early Chinese religion, part one: Shang through Han (1250 BC–220 AD) (2 vols.) (pp. 519542). Brill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2020). Confucius. In Zalta, E. N. (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (summer 2020). Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2020/entries/confucius/Google Scholar
Curry, O. S. (2016). Morality as cooperation: A problem-centred approach. In Shackelford, T. K. & Hansen, R. D. (Eds.), The evolution of morality (pp. 2751). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-19671-8_2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curry, O. S., Jones Chesters, M., & Van Lissa, C. J. (2019a). Mapping morality with a compass: Testing the theory of “morality-as-cooperation” with a new questionnaire. Journal of Research in Personality, 78, 106124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2018.10.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curry, O. S., Mullins, D. A., & Whitehouse, H. (2019b). Is it good to cooperate? Testing the theory of morality-as-cooperation in 60 societies. Current Anthropology, 60(1), 4769. https://doi.org/10.1086/701478CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cushman, F. (2008). Crime and punishment: Distinguishing the roles of causal and intentional analyses in moral judgment. Cognition, 108(2), 353380. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.03.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dabhoiwala, F. (2012). The origins of sex: A history of the first sexual revolution. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Demos, K. E., Heatherton, T. F., & Kelley, W. M. (2012). Individual differences in nucleus accumbens activity to food and sexual images predict weight gain and sexual behavior. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(16), 55495552. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5958-11.2012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Ridder, D. T. D., Lensvelt-Mulders, G., Finkenauer, C., Stok, F. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2012). Taking stock of self-control: A meta-analysis of how trait self-control relates to a wide range of behaviors. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16(1), 7699. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868311418749CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeScioli, P. (2016). The side-taking hypothesis for moral judgment. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 2327. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.07.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeScioli, P., & Kurzban, R. (2009). Mysteries of morality. Cognition, 112(2), 281299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeScioli, P., & Kurzban, R. (2013). A solution to the mysteries of morality. Psychological Bulletin, 139(2), 477496. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029065CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeScioli, P., Massenkoff, M., Shaw, A., Petersen, M. B., & Kurzban, R. (2014). Equity or equality? Moral judgments follow the money. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1797), 20142112. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.2112CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickemann, M. (1981). Paternal confidence and dowry competition: A biocultural analysis of purdah. In Alexander, R. D. & Tinkle, D. W. (Eds.), Natural selection and social behavior (pp. 417438). Chiron Press.Google Scholar
Dohmen, T., Enke, B., Falk, A., Huffman, D., & Sunde, U. (2018). Patience and comparative development. University of Bonn, Mimeo.Google Scholar
Doniger, W. (2014). On Hinduism. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Droubay, B., Butters, R., & Shafer, K. (2021). The pornography debate: Religiosity and support for censorship. Journal of Religion and Health, 60, 16521652. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10943-018-0732-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Droubay, B. A., & Butters, R. P. (2020). Pornography, religiosity, and social work. Journal of Social Work, 20(5), 557575. https://doi.org/10.1177/1468017319852599CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duby, G., Pantel, P. S., Thébaud, F., & Perrot, M. (1994). A history of women in the west: Toward a cultural identity in the 20th century. Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Duckworth, A. L., Gendler, T. S., & Gross, J. J. (2016a). Situational strategies for self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(1), 3555. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615623247CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duckworth, A. L., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2017). The science and practice of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(5), 715718. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617690880CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duckworth, A. L., White, R. E., Matteucci, A. J., Shearer, A., & Gross, J. J. (2016b). A stitch in time: Strategic self-control in high school and college students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108(3), 329341. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000062CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duke, A. A., Smith, K. M. Z., Oberleitner, L. M. S., Westphal, A., & McKee, S. A. (2018). Alcohol, drugs, and violence: A meta-meta-analysis. Psychology of Violence, 8(2), 238249. https://doi.org/10.1037/vio0000106CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dungan, J., Chakroff, A., & Young, L. (2017). The relevance of moral norms in distinct relational contexts: Purity versus harm norms regulate self-directed actions. PLoS ONE, 12, e0173405. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0173405CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edelman, B. (2009). Markets: Red light states: Who buys online adult entertainment? Journal of Economic Perspectives, 23(1), 209220. https://doi.org/10.1257/jep.23.1.209CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edelstein, O. E., Wacht, O., Grinstein-Cohen, O., Reznik, A., Pruginin, I., & Isralowitz, R. (2020). Does religiosity matter? University student attitudes and beliefs toward medical cannabis. Complementary Therapies in Medicine, 51, 102407. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ctim.2020.102407CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisner, M. (2001). Modernization, self-control and lethal violence. The long-term dynamics of European homicide rates in theoretical perspective. British Journal of Criminology, 41(4), 618638. https://doi.org/10.1093/bjc/41.4.618CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisner, M. (2003). Long-term historical trends in violent crime. Crime and Justice, 30, 83142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisner, M. (2014). From swords to words: Does macro-level change in self-control predict long-term variation in levels of homicide? Crime and Justice, 43(1), 65134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
el-Aswad, E.-S. (2014). Patience in Sunni Muslim worldviews. In Leeming, D. A. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of psychology and religion (pp. 13181321). Springer US. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-6086-2_9317CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eljawad, M. A., Se'eda, H., Ghozy, S., El-Qushayri, A. E., Elsherif, A., Elkassar, A. H., … Islam, S. M. S. (2021). Pornography use prevalence and associated factors in Arab countries: A multinational cross-sectional study of 15,027 individuals. The Journal of Sexual Medicine, 18(3), 539548. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsxm.2020.12.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
El Khoury, J., Noufi, P., Ahmad, A., Akl, E., & El Hayek, S. (2019). Attitudes, beliefs, and knowledge of substance use amongst youth in the Eastern Mediterranean region: A systematic review. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 196, 7178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.12.019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, L. (1988). The victimful–victimless crime distinction, and seven universal demographic correlates of victimful criminal behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 9(3), 525548. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(88)90151-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eriksson, K., Dickins, T. E., & Strimling, P. (2021). Hygiene norms across 56 nations are predicted by self-control values and disease threat. Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology, 2, 100013. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cresp.2021.100013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1973). Some notes on Zande sex habits. American Anthropologist, 75(1), 171175. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1973.75.1.02a00100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fan, W., Ren, M., Zhang, W., Xiao, P., & Zhong, Y. (2020). Higher self-control, less deception: The effect of self-control on deception behaviors. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 16(3), 228241. https://doi.org/10.5709/acp-0299-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farré, J. M., Fernández-Aranda, F., Granero, R., Aragay, N., Mallorquí-Bague, N., Ferrer, V., … Jiménez-Murcia, S. (2015). Sex addiction and gambling disorder: Similarities and differences. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 56, 5968. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.10.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fehr, E., & Leibbrandt, A. (2011). A field study on cooperativeness and impatience in the tragedy of the commons. Journal of Public Economics, 95(9–10), 11441155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpubeco.2011.05.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitouchi, L., André, J.-B., & Baumard, N. (2021). The intertwined cultural evolution of ascetic spiritualities and puritanical religions as technologies of self-discipline. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 11(2), 197206. https://doi.org/10.1080/2153599X.2021.1881607CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitouchi, L., André, J.-B., & Baumard, N. (in press). Are there really so many moral emotions? Carving morality at its functional joints. In Al-Shawaf, L. & Shackelford, T. K. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of evolution and the emotions. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fitouchi, L., André, J.-B., Baumard, N., & Nettle, D. (2022). Harmless bodily pleasures are moralized because they are perceived as reducing self-control and cooperativeness. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/fzv43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitouchi, L., & Singh, M. (2022). Supernatural punishment beliefs as cognitively compelling tools of social control. Current Opinion in Psychology, 44, 252257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitouchi, L., & Singh, M. (2023). Punitive justice serves to restore reciprocal cooperation in three small-scale societies. Evolution and Human Behavior. Advance online publication.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franchin, L., Geipel, J., Hadjichristidis, C., & Surian, L. (2019). Many moral buttons or just one? Evidence from emotional facial expressions. Cognition and Emotion, 33(5), 943958. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2018.1520078CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, R. H. (1988). Passions within reason: The strategic role of the emotions. Norton.Google Scholar
Friese, M., Frankenbach, J., Job, V., & Loschelder, D. D. (2017). Does self-control training improve self-control? A meta-analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(6), 10771099. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617697076CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fromell, H., Nosenzo, D., & Owens, T. (2020). Altruism, fast and slow? Evidence from a meta-analysis and a new experiment. Experimental Economics, 23(4), 9791001. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-020-09645-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fujita, K. (2011). On conceptualizing self-control as more than the effortful inhibition of impulses. Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(4), 352366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaca, K. L. (2003). The making of fornication: Eros, ethics, and political reform in Greek philosophy and early Christianity. University of California Press.Google Scholar
Gai, P. J., & Bhattacharjee, A. (2022). Willpower as moral ability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 159(8), 19992006. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001169CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gailliot, M. T., & Baumeister, R. F. (2007). Self-regulation and sexual restraint: Dispositionally and temporarily poor self-regulatory abilities contribute to failures at restraining sexual behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(2), 173186. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167206293472CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gan, G., Sterzer, P., Marxen, M., Zimmermann, U. S., & Smolka, M. N. (2015). Neural and behavioral correlates of alcohol-induced aggression under provocation. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40(13), 28862896. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2015.141CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evolution of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(4), 573587. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0000337XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garden, K. (2014). The first Islamic reviver: Abū Ḥāmid al-Ghazālī and his revival of the religious sciences. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gerrits, J. H., de Ridder, D. T. D., de Wit, J. B. F., & Kuijer, R. G. (2009). Cool and independent or foolish and undisciplined? Adolescents’ prototypes of (un)healthy eaters and their association with eating behaviour. Appetite, 53(3), 407413. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2009.08.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gervais, W. M. (2013). In godlessness we distrust: Using social psychology to solve the puzzle of anti-atheist prejudice: In godlessness we distrust. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7(6), 366377. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12035CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gervais, W. M. (2014). Everything is permitted? People intuitively judge immorality as representative of atheists. PLoS ONE, 9(4), e92302. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0092302CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gervais, W. M., Xygalatas, D., McKay, R. T., van Elk, M., Buchtel, E. E., Aveyard, M., … Bulbulia, J. (2017). Global evidence of extreme intuitive moral prejudice against atheists. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(8), 0151. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0151CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geyer, A. L., & Baumeister, R. F. (2005). Religion, morality, and self-control: Values, virtues, and vices. In Paloutzian, R. F. & Park, C. L. (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of religion and spirituality (pp. 412432). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Ghelfi, E., Christopherson, C. D., Urry, H. L., Lenne, R. L., Legate, N., Fischer, M. A., … Sullivan, D. (2020). Reexamining the effect of gustatory disgust on moral judgment: A multilab direct replication of Eskine, Kacinik, and Prinz (2011). Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 3(1), 323. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245919881152CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giancola, P. R., Josephs, R. A., Parrott, D. J., & Duke, A. A. (2010). Alcohol myopia revisited: Clarifying aggression and other acts of disinhibition through a distorted lens. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(3), 265278. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610369467CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glucklich, A. (2020). The joy of religion: Exploring the nature of pleasure in spiritual life. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldin, P. R. (2017). The culture of sex in ancient China. University of Hawaii Press. https://doi.org/10.1515/9780824864651CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gomillion, S., Lamarche, V. M., Murray, S. L., & Harris, B. (2014). Protected by your self-control: The influence of partners’ self-control on actors’ responses to interpersonal risk. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5(8), 873882. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550614538462CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, C. (2017). Ethics in Indian and Tibetan Buddhism. In Zalta, E. N. (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Spring 2017). Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2017/entries/ethics-indian-buddhism/Google Scholar
Goodwin, G., Piazza, J., & Rozin, P. (2013). Moral character predominates in person perception and evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(1), 148168. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034726CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodwin, G. P. (2015). Moral character in person perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(1), 3844. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414550709CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goody, J. (1976). Production and reproduction: A comparative study of the domestic domain. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gorski, P. S. (1993). The protestant ethic revisited: Disciplinary revolution and state formation in Holland and Prussia. American Journal of Sociology, 99(2), 265316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gorski, P. S. (2003). The disciplinary revolution: Calvinism and the rise of the state in early modern Europe. University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, J., Haidt, J., Koleva, S., Motyl, M., Iyer, R., Wojcik, S. P., & Ditto, P. H. (2013). Moral foundations theory: The pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. In P. Devine & A. Plant (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 47, pp. 55130). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-407236-7.00002-4Google Scholar
Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 10291046. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015141CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, K., DiMaggio, N., Schein, C., & Kachanoff, F. (2022). The problem of purity in moral psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Review. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868322112Google ScholarPubMed
Gray, K., & Keeney, J. E. (2015). Impure or just weird? Scenario sampling bias raises questions about the foundation of morality. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6(8), 859868. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550615592241CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, K., & Schein, C. (2016). No absolutism here: Harm predicts moral judgment 30× better than disgust – Commentary on Scott, Inbar, & Rozin (2016). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(3), 325329. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616635598CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, K., Schein, C., & Ward, A. F. (2014). The myth of harmless wrongs in moral cognition: Automatic dyadic completion from sin to suffering. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(4), 16001615. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036149CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, K., Waytz, A., & Young, L. (2012). The moral dyad: A fundamental template unifying moral judgment. Psychological Inquiry, 23(2), 206215. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2012.686247CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graziani, R. (2009). Optimal states and self-defeating plans: The problem of intentionality in early Chinese self-cultivation. Philosophy East and West, 59(4), 440466. https://doi.org/10.1353/pew.0.0067CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grubbs, J. B., Exline, J. J., Pargament, K. I., Hook, J. N., & Carlisle, R. D. (2015). Transgression as addiction: Religiosity and moral disapproval as predictors of perceived addiction to pornography. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 44(1), 125136. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-013-0257-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grubbs, J. B., Grant, J. T., & Engelman, J. (2018a). Self-identification as a pornography addict: Examining the roles of pornography use, religiousness, and moral incongruence. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 25(4), 269292. https://doi.org/10.1080/10720162.2019.1565848CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grubbs, J. B., Kraus, S. W., & Perry, S. L. (2019). Self-reported addiction to pornography in a nationally representative sample: The roles of use habits, religiousness, and moral incongruence. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 8(1), 8893. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.7.2018.134CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grubbs, J. B., Wilt, J. A., Exline, J. J., Pargament, K. I., & Kraus, S. W. (2018b). Moral disapproval and perceived addiction to internet pornography: A longitudinal examination. Addiction, 113(3), 496506. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.14007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guerrand, R.-H. (1984). Haro sur la masturbation. In Duby, G. (Ed.), Amour et sexualité en occident (pp. 99102). Seuil.Google Scholar
Guglielmo, S., & Malle, B. F. (2017). Information-acquisition processes in moral judgments of blame. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(7), 957971. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167217702375CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guillou, L., Grandin, A., & Chevallier, C. (2021). Temporal discounting mediates the relationship between socio-economic status and social trust. Royal Society Open Science, 8(6), 202104. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.202104CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gullo, M. J., Loxton, N. J., & Dawe, S. (2014). Impulsivity: Four ways five factors are not basic to addiction. Addictive Behaviors, 39(11), 15471556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gurven, M., Winking, J., Kaplan, H., von Rueden, C., & McAllister, L. (2009). A bioeconomic approach to marriage and the sexual division of labor. Human Nature, 20(2), 151183. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-009-9062-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gusfield, J. R. (1997). The culture of public problems: Drinking-driving and the symbolic order. In Brandt, A. M. & Rozin, P. (Eds.), Morality and health (pp. 201229). Routledge.Google Scholar
Gusinde, M. (1931). The fireland Indians: Vol. 1. The Selk'Nam, on the life and thought of a hunting people of the Great Island of Tierra Del Fuego. Verlag der Internationalen Zeitschrift. https://ehrafworldcultures-yale-edu.proxy.library.upenn.edu/document?id=sh04-001Google Scholar
Hackett, C., & McClendon, D. (2017). Christians remain world's largest religious group, but they are declining in Europe. Pew Research Center.Google Scholar
Haidt, J. (2007). The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science, 316(5827), 9981002. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1137651CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. Knopf Doubleday.Google Scholar
Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2007). When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Social Justice Research, 20(1), 98116. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-007-0034-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J., & Hersh, M. A. (2001). Sexual morality: The cultures and emotions of conservatives and liberals. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 31(1), 191221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2004). Intuitive ethics: How innately prepared intuitions generate culturally variable virtues. Daedalus, 133(4), 5566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2007). The moral mind: How five sets of innate intuitions guide the development of many culture-specific virtues, and perhaps even modules. The Innate Mind, 3, 367391.Google Scholar
Haidt, J., Koller, S. H., & Dias, M. G. (1993). Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(4), 613.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, D. L., Cohen, A. B., Meyer, K. K., Varley, A. H., & Brewer, G. A. (2015). Costly signaling increases trust, even across religious affiliations. Psychological Science, 26(9), 13681376. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615576473CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hallowell, A. I. (1976). Ojibwa world view and disease. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Hallpike, C. (2008). The Konso of Ethiopia: A study of the values of an east Cushitic people (Revised ed.). AuthorHouse.Google Scholar
Hamiltron, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behavior: I. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harper, C. A., & Rhodes, D. (2021). Reanalysing the factor structure of the moral foundations questionnaire. British Journal of Social Psychology, 60(4), 13031329. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12452CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, P. (2000). An introduction to Buddhist ethics: Foundations, values and issues. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatcher, B. A. (2017). India's many puritans: Connectivity and friction in the study of modern Hinduism. History Compass, 15(1), e12369. https://doi.org/10.1111/hic3.12369CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heatherton, T. F., & Wagner, D. D. (2011). Cognitive neuroscience of self-regulation failure. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(3), 132139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.12.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helzer, E. G., & Pizarro, D. A. (2011). Dirty liberals! Reminders of physical cleanliness influence moral and political attitudes. Psychological Science, 22(4), 517522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henderson, N. L., & Dressler, W. W. (2019). Cultural models of substance misuse risk and moral foundations: Cognitive resources underlying stigma attribution. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 19(1–2), 7896. https://doi.org/10.1163/15685373-12340049CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J. (2017). The secret of our success: How culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2020). The weirdest people in the world: How the west became psychologically peculiar and particularly prosperous. Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2012). The puzzle of monogamous marriage. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1589), 657669. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0290CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 6183. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., & Muthukrishna, M. (2021). The origins and psychology of human cooperation. Annual Review of Psychology, 72(1), 207240. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-081920-042106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hershfield, H. E., Cohen, T. R., & Thompson, L. (2012). Short horizons and tempting situations: Lack of continuity to our future selves leads to unethical decision making and behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 117(2), 298310. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2011.11.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herz, R., & Hinds, A. (2013). Stealing is not gross: Language distinguishes visceral disgust from moral violations. The American Journal of Psychology, 126, 275286. https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.126.3.0275CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hezser, C. (2018). Self-control in a world controlled by others: Palestinian rabbinic “asceticism” in late antiquity. Religion in the Roman Empire, 4(1), 927.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, S. E. (2007). “The ooze of gluttony”: Attitudes towards food, eating, and excess in the middle ages. The Seven Deadly Sins, 57–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, S. E. (2011). Eating to excess: The meaning of gluttony and the fat body in the ancient world. Praeger.Google Scholar
Himmelstein, J. L. (1983). The strange career of marihuana: Politics and ideology of drug control in America (Vol. 94). Greenwood.Google Scholar
Hinsch, B. H. (1994). Women in early imperial China. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hoebel, E. (2009). The law of primitive man: A study in comparative legal dynamics. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, W., & Kotabe, H. (2012). A general model of preventive and interventive self-control: PI-model of self-control. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 6(10), 707722. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2012.00461.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, W., Meindl, P., Mooijman, M., & Graham, J. (2018). Morality and self-control: How they are intertwined and where they differ. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(4), 286291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, W., Wisneski, D. C., Brandt, M. J., & Skitka, L. J. (2014). Morality in everyday life. Science, 345(6202), 13401343. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1251560CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holland, J., Silva, A. S., & Mace, R. (2012). Lost letter measure of variation in altruistic behaviour in 20 neighbourhoods. PLoS ONE, 7(8), e43294. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0043294CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hong, Z., & Henrich, J. (2021). The cultural evolution of epistemic practices: The case of divination. Human Nature, 32(3), 622651. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09408-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hong, Z., Slingerland, E., & Henrich, J. (forthcoming). Magic and empiricism in early Chinese rainmaking. Current Anthropology.Google Scholar
Horberg, E. J., Oveis, C., Keltner, D., & Cohen, A. B. (2009). Disgust and the moralization of purity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(6), 963976. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017423CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huckle, T., You, R. Q., & Casswell, S. (2010). Socio-economic status predicts drinking patterns but not alcohol-related consequences independently. Addiction, 105(7), 11921202. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.02931.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hugh-Jones, C. (1979). From the milk river: Spatial and temporal processes in Northwest Amazonia. Cambridge University Press. https://ehrafworldcultures-yale-edu.proxy.library.upenn.edu/document?id=sq19-011Google Scholar
Hunt, A. (1998). The great masturbation panic and the discourses of moral regulation in nineteenth- and early twentieth-century Britain. Journal of the History of Sexuality, 8(4), 575615.Google Scholar
Hyman, S. E. (2007). The neurobiology of addiction: Implications for voluntary control of behavior. The American Journal of Bioethics, 7(1), 811. https://doi.org/10.1080/15265160601063969CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., Knobe, J., & Bloom, P. (2009). Disgust sensitivity predicts intuitive disapproval of gays. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 9(3), 435439. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015960CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inglehart, R. (2018). Cultural evolution: People's motivations are changing, and reshaping the world. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inglehart, R., & Baker, W. E. (2000). Modernization, cultural change, and the persistence of traditional values. American Sociological Review, 65(1), 19. https://doi.org/10.2307/2657288CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ingram, M. (1990). Church courts, sex and marriage in England, 15701640. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ingram, M. (1996). Reformation of manners in early modern England. In Griffiths, P., Fox, A., & Hindle, S. (Eds.), The experience of authority in early modern England (pp. 4788). Macmillan. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-349-24834-6_3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irvine, J. (1974). Caste and communication in a Wolof village. University Microfilms. https://ehrafworldcultures-yale-edu.proxy.library.upenn.edu/document?id=ms30-043Google Scholar
Irvine, W. B. (2009). A guide to the good life: The ancient art of Stoic joy. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Isler, O., Yilmaz, O., & John Maule, A. (2021). Religion, parochialism and intuitive cooperation. Nature Human Behaviour, 5(4), 512521. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-01014-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, J. C., Gelfand, M., & Ember, C. R. (2020). A global analysis of cultural tightness in non-industrial societies. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 287(1930), 20201036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacquet, P. O., Pazhoohi, F., Findling, C., Mell, H., Chevallier, C., & Baumard, N. (2021). Predictive modeling of religiosity, prosociality, and moralizing in 295,000 individuals from European and non-European populations. Humanities and Social Sciences Communications, 8(1), 9. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-020-00691-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, D. J., Wortman, J., Cheung, F., Hein, M., Lucas, R. E., Donnellan, M. B., … Narr, R. K. (2016). The effects of disgust on moral judgments: Testing moderators. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(7), 640647. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550616654211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jylkkä, J., Härkönen, J., & Hyönä, J. (2021). Incidental disgust does not cause moral condemnation of neutral actions. Cognition and Emotion, 35(1), 96109. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2020.1810639CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kan, S. (1989). Symbolic immortality: The Tlingit potlatch of the nineteenth century. Smithsonian Institution Press. https://ehrafworldcultures-yale-edu.proxy.library.upenn.edu/document?id=na12-032Google Scholar
Karila, L., Wery, A., Weinstein, A., Cottencin, O., Petit, A., Reynaud, M., & Billieux, J. (2014). Sexual addiction or hypersexual disorder: Different terms for the same problem? A review of the literature. Current Pharmaceutical Design, 20(25), 40124020. https://doi.org/10.2174/13816128113199990619CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kayyal, M. H., Pochedly, J., McCarthy, A., & Russell, J. A. (2015). On the limits of the relation of disgust to judgments of immorality. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 19. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00951CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keown, D. (2003). A dictionary of Buddhism. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kim, B. K., & Zauberman, G. (2013). Can Victoria's secret change the future? A subjective time perception account of sexual-cue effects on impatience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(2), 328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiyimba, A. (2012). Music and Islam in Uganda: Diverse opinions and practices. In Nannyonga-Tamusuza, S. & Solomon, T. (Eds.), Ethnomusicology in East Africa: Perspectives from Uganda and beyond (pp. 93109). Fountain.Google Scholar
Knoch, D., & Fehr, E. (2007). Resisting the power of temptations: The right prefrontal cortex and self-control. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1104(1), 123134. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1390.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knoch, D., Schneider, F., Schunk, D., Hohmann, M., & Fehr, E. (2009). Disrupting the prefrontal cortex diminishes the human ability to build a good reputation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(49), 2089520899. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0911619106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kober, H., Mende-Siedlecki, P., Kross, E. F., Weber, J., Mischel, W., Hart, C. L., & Ochsner, K. N. (2010). Prefrontal–striatal pathway underlies cognitive regulation of craving. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(33), 1481114816. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1007779107CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koleva, S. P., Graham, J., Iyer, R., Ditto, P. H., & Haidt, J. (2012). Tracing the threads: How five moral concerns (especially purity) help explain culture war attitudes. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(2), 184194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2012.01.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kollareth, D., Brownell, H., Duran, J. I., & Russell, J. A. (2022). Is purity a distinct and homogeneous domain in moral psychology? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 152(1), 211235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kollareth, D., & Russell, J. A. (2019). Disgust and the sacred: Do people react to violations of the sacred with the same emotion they react to something putrid? Emotion, 19(1), 3752. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000412CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koole, S. L., Meijer, M., & Remmers, C. (2017). Religious rituals as tools for adaptive self-regulation. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 7(3), 250253. https://doi.org/10.1080/2153599X.2016.1156562CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koval, C. Z., VanDellen, M. R., Fitzsimons, G. M., & Ranby, K. W. (2015). The burden of responsibility: Interpersonal costs of high self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(5), 750.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kringelbach, M. L., & Berridge, K. C. (2009). Towards a functional neuroanatomy of pleasure and happiness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(11), 479487. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.08.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kupfer, T. R., Inbar, Y., & Tybur, J. M. (2020). Reexamining the role of intent in moral judgements of purity violations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 91, 104043. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2020.104043CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurzban, R., Dukes, A., & Weeden, J. (2010). Sex, drugs and moral goals: Reproductive strategies and views about recreational drugs. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277(1699), 35013508. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.0608CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landy, J., & Piazza, J. (2017). Reevaluating moral disgust: Sensitivity to many affective states predicts extremity in many evaluative judgments. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 10, 194855061773611. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617736110Google Scholar
Landy, J. F., & Goodwin, G. P. (2015). Does incidental disgust amplify moral judgment? A meta-analytic review of experimental evidence. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(4), 518536. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615583128CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langlands, R. (2006). Sexual morality in ancient Rome. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511482823CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, R. B. (2013). The Dobe Ju/’hoansi (4th ed., student ed.). Wadsworth Cengage.Google Scholar
Le Goff, J. (1984). Le refus du plaisir in L'amour et la sexualité: Vol. Amour et Sexualité en Occident (pp. 5259). Points Histoire.Google Scholar
Leigh, B. C. (1987). Beliefs about the effects of alcohol on self and others. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 48(5), 467475. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsa.1987.48.467CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leimar, O., & Connor, R. C. (2003). By-product benefits, reciprocity, and pseudoreciprocity in mutualism. In Hammerstein, P. (Ed.), Genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation (pp. 203222). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Leimar, O., & Hammerstein, P. (2010). Cooperation for direct fitness benefits. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365(1553), 26192626. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0116CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lettinga, N., Jacquet, P. O., André, J.-B., Baumand, N., & Chevallier, C. (2020). Environmental adversity is associated with lower investment in collective actions. PLoS ONE, 15(7), e0236715. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0236715CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, D. N. (1965). Wax & gold: Tradition and innovation in Ethiopian culture. University of Chicago Press. https://ehrafworldcultures-yale-edu.proxy.library.upenn.edu/document?id=mp05-011Google Scholar
Levine, H. (1993). Temperance cultures: Concern about alcohol as a problem in Nordic and English-speaking cultures. In The nature of alcohol and drug-related problems (pp. 1636). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
LeVine, R. A. (1959). Gusii sex offenses: A study in social control. American Anthropologist, 61(6), 965990. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1959.61.6.02a00050CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewer, D., Meier, P., Beard, E., Boniface, S., & Kaner, E. (2016). Unravelling the alcohol harm paradox: A population-based study of social gradients across very heavy drinking thresholds. BMC Public Health, 16, 111. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-016-3265-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lie-Panis, J., & André, J.-B. (2022). Cooperation as a signal of time preferences. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 289(1973), 20212266. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2021.2266CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindman, R. E., & Lang, A. R. (1994). The alcohol-aggression stereotype: A cross-cultural comparison of beliefs. International Journal of the Addictions, 29(1), 113. https://doi.org/10.3109/10826089409047365CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, H., & Li, H. (2020). Self-control modulates the behavioral response of interpersonal forgiveness. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 18. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00472Google ScholarPubMed
Lowe, M. (2020). Religious revival and social order.Google Scholar
Lugo, L., Cooperman, A., Bell, J., O'Connell, E., & Stencel, S. (2013). The world's Muslims: Religion, politics and society. Politics and Society, 226.Google Scholar
Luttmer, F. (2000). Persecutors, tempters and vassals of the devil: The unregenerate in puritan practical divinity. The Journal of Ecclesiastical History, 51(1), 3768. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022046999002882CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynxwiler, J., & Gay, D. (2000). Moral boundaries and deviant music: Public attitudes toward heavy metal and rap. Deviant Behavior, 21(1), 6385. https://doi.org/10.1080/016396200266388Google Scholar
MacInnis, C. C., & Hodson, G. (2015). Do American states with more religious or conservative populations search more for sexual content on Google? Archives of Sexual Behavior, 44(1), 137147. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0361-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahmud, Y., & Swami, V. (2009). The influence of the hijab (Islamic head-cover) on perceptions of women's attractiveness and intelligence. Body Image, 7, 9093. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.09.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malinowski, B. (1929). The sexual life of savages in northwestern Melanesia. [La vie sexuelle des sauvages du Nord-Ouest de la Mélanesie (Trans. by S. Jankelevitch)]. Liveright.Google Scholar
Mankar, M., Joshi, R. S., Belsare, P. V., Jog, M. M., & Watve, M. G. (2008). Obesity as a perceived social signal. PLoS ONE, 3(9), 7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mann, J. K. (2011). Lutherans in need of self-discipline: Japanese Shugyō and the art of sanctification. Dialog, 50(3), 271279. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6385.2011.00620.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manrique, H. M., Zeidler, H., Roberts, G., Barclay, P., Walker, M., Samu, F., … Raihani, N. (2021). The psychological foundations of reputation-based cooperation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 376(1838), 20200287. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0287CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcus, Z. J., & McCullough, M. E. (2021). Does religion make people more self-controlled? A review of research from the lab and life. Current Opinion in Psychology, 40, 167170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.12.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, A. L. (2002). Alcohol, sex and gender in late medieval and early modern Europe. Palgrave.Google Scholar
Martin, A. L. (2009). Alcohol, violence, and disorder in traditional Europe. Truman State University Press.Google Scholar
Matthee, R. (2014). Alcohol in the Islamic Middle East: Ambivalence and ambiguity. Past & Present, 222(Suppl. 9), 100125. https://doi.org/10.1093/pastj/gtt031CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mcadams, D. P., Albaugh, M., Farber, E., Daniels, J., Logan, R. L., & Olson, B. (2008). Family metaphors and moral intuitions: How conservatives and liberals narrate their lives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(4), 978990.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCullough, M. E., & Carter, E. C. (2013). Religion, self-control, and self-regulation: How and why are they related? In APA handbook of psychology, religion, and spirituality (Vol. 1): Context, theory, and research (pp. 123138). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/14045-006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCullough, M. E., Pedersen, E. J., Schroder, J. M., Tabak, B. A., & Carver, C. S. (2012). Harsh childhood environmental characteristics predict exploitation and retaliation in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 280(1750), 20122104. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2012.2104CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCullough, M. E., & Willoughby, B. L. B. (2009). Religion, self-regulation, and self-control: Associations, explanations, and implications. Psychological Bulletin, 135(1), 6993. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014213CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McIntosh, M. K. (2002). Controlling misbehavior in England, 13701600. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McIntyre, J. C., Barlow, F. K., & Hayward, L. E. (2015). Stronger sexual desires only predict bold romantic intentions and reported infidelity when self-control is low. Australian Journal of Psychology, 67(3), 178186. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajpy.12073CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menon, U. (2013). The Hindu concept of self-refinement: Implicit yet meaningful. Psychology and Developing Societies, 25(1), 195222. https://doi.org/10.1177/0971333613477320CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mernissi, F. (2011). Beyond the veil: Male–female dynamics in modern Muslim society. Saqi Books.Google Scholar
Merritt, A. C. (2013). The interpersonal costs of indulgence. Stanford University.Google Scholar
Michalak, L., & Trocki, K. (2006). Alcohol and Islam: An overview. Contemporary Drug Problems, 33(4), 523562. https://doi.org/10.1177/009145090603300401CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miles, E., Sheeran, P., Baird, H., Macdonald, I., Webb, T. L., & Harris, P. R. (2016). Does self-control improve with practice? Evidence from a six-week training program. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(8), 10751091. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000185CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milyavskaya, M., Saunders, B., & Inzlicht, M. (2021). Self-control in daily life: Prevalence and effectiveness of diverse self-control strategies. Journal of Personality, 89(4), 634651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mischel, H. N., & Mischel, W. (1983). The development of children's knowledge of self-control strategies (pp. 603619).Google Scholar
Miton, H., Claidière, N., & Mercier, H. (2015). Universal cognitive mechanisms explain the cultural success of bloodletting. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(4), 303312. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.01.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., … Caspi, A. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 26932698. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1010076108CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mooijman, M., Meindl, P., Oyserman, D., Monterosso, J., Dehghani, M., Doris, J. M., & Graham, J. (2018). Resisting temptation for the good of the group: Binding moral values and the moralization of self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 115(3), 585599. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000149CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moon, J. W. (2021). Why are world religions so concerned with sexual behavior? Current Opinion in Psychology, 40, 1519. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.07.030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moon, J. W., Krems, J. A., & Cohen, A. B. (2018). Religious people are trusted because they are viewed as slow life-history strategists. Psychological Science, 29(6), 947960. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617753606CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moon, J. W., Krems, J. A., Cohen, A. B., & Kenrick, D. T. (2019). Is nothing sacred? Religion, sex, and reproductive strategies. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(4), 361365. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419838242CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moon, J. W., Wongsomboon, V., & Sevi, B. (2021). Beliefs about men's sexual self-control predict attitudes toward women's immodest clothing and public breastfeeding [preprint]. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/67vh9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mukhopadhyay, A., & Yeung, C. (2010). Building character: Effects of lay theories of self-control on the selection of products for children. Journal of Marketing Research, 47(2), 240250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murdock, G. P. (1949). Social structure. MacMillan.Google Scholar
Mushtaq, S., Mendes, V., Nikolaou, V., & Luty, J. (2015). Analysis of the possible components of stigmatised attitudes towards depression and heroin dependence. Journal of Substance Use, 20(6), 399406. https://doi.org/10.3109/14659891.2014.934306CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nabi, R. L. (2002). The theoretical versus the lay meaning of disgust: Implications for emotion research. Cognition & Emotion, 16(5), 695703. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000437CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nag, M. (1972). Sex, culture, and human fertility: India and the United States. Current Anthropology, 13(2), 231237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Najjar, L. Z., Young, C. M., Leasure, L., Henderson, C. E., & Neighbors, C. (2016). Religious perceptions of alcohol consumption and drinking behaviours among religious and non-religious groups. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 19(9), 10281041.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nemoto, T., Iwamoto, M., Morris, A., Yokota, F., & Wada, K. (2007). Substance use and sexual behaviors among Japanese tourists, students, and temporary workers in Honolulu, Hawaii. AIDS Education and Prevention, 19(1), 6881. https://doi.org/10.1521/aeap.2007.19.1.68CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nettle, D. (2015). Tyneside neighbourhoods: Deprivation, social life and social behaviour in one British city. Open Book.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nettle, D., & Saxe, R. (2020). Preferences for redistribution are sensitive to perceived luck, social homogeneity, war and scarcity. Cognition, 198, 104234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104234CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nettle, D., & Saxe, R. (2021). “If men were angels, no government would be necessary”: The intuitive theory of social motivation and preference for authoritarian leaders. Collabra: Psychology, 7(1), 28105. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.28105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nockur, L., & Pfattheicher, S. (2021). The beautiful complexity of human prosociality: On the interplay of honesty-humility, intuition, and a reward system. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12(6), 877886. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550620961262CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norena, C. F. (2007). Hadrian's chastity. Phoenix, 61(4), 296317.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A., Shariff, A., Gervais, W., Willard, A., McNamara, R., Slingerland, E., & Henrich, J. (2016). The cultural evolution of prosocial religions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, E1. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X14001356CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nowak, M. A., & Sigmund, K. (2005). Evolution of indirect reciprocity. Nature, 437(7063), 12911298. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature04131CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nylan, M. (2001). On the politics of pleasure. Asia Major, 14(1), 73124.Google Scholar
Oakes, M. E., & Slotterback, C. S. (2004). Prejudgments of those who eat a “healthy” versus and “unhealthy” food for breakfast. Current Psychology, 23(4), 267278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oestreich, G., Oestreich, B., & Koenigsberger, H. G. (1982). Neostoicism and the early modern state. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ortiz-Ospina, E., & Roser, M. (2016). Trust. Our world in data. https://ourworldindata.org/trustGoogle Scholar
Osadchiy, V., Mayer, E. A., Bhatt, R., Labus, J. S., Gao, L., Kilpatrick, L. A., … Gupta, A. (2019). History of early life adversity is associated with increased food addiction and sex-specific alterations in reward network connectivity in obesity. Obesity Science & Practice, 5(5), 416436. https://doi.org/10.1002/osp4.362CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osiurak, F., & Reynaud, E. (2019). The elephant in the room: What matters cognitively in cumulative technological culture. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 43, 157. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X19003236Google ScholarPubMed
Ostrom, E. (1990). Governing the commons: The evolution of institutions for collective action. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Otterbeck, J., & Ackfeldt, A. (2012). Music and Islam. Contemporary Islam, 6(3), 227233. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11562-012-0220-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paglia, A., & Room, R. (1998). Alcohol and aggression: General population views about causation and responsibility. Journal of Substance Abuse, 10(2), 199216. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0899-3289(99)80134-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panchanathan, K., & Boyd, R. (2004). Indirect reciprocity can stabilize cooperation without the second-order free rider problem. Nature, 432(7016), 499502. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature02978CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parrott, D. J., & Eckhardt, C. I. (2018). Effects of alcohol on human aggression. Current Opinion in Psychology, 19, 15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Partridge, C. H., & Moberg, M. (Eds.). (2017). The Bloomsbury handbook of religion and popular music. Bloomsbury Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pazhoohi, F. (2016). On the practice of cultural clothing practices that conceal the eyes: An evolutionary perspective. Evolution, Mind and Behaviour, 14(1), 5564. https://doi.org/10.1556/2050.2016.0002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pazhoohi, F., & Burriss, R. P. (2016). Hijab and “hitchhiking”: A field study. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 2(1), 3237. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-015-0033-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pazhoohi, F., & Hosseinchari, M. (2014). Effects of religious veiling on Muslim men's attractiveness ratings of Muslim women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43(6), 10831086. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0259-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pazhoohi, F., Lang, M., Xygalatas, D., & Grammer, K. (2017a). Religious veiling as a mate-guarding strategy: Effects of environmental pressures on cultural practices. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 3(2), 118124. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-016-0079-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pazhoohi, F., Macedo, A. F., & Arantes, J. (2017b). The effect of religious clothing on gaze behavior: An eye-tracking experiment. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 39(3), 176182. https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2017.1307748CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peake, P. K., Hebl, M., & Mischel, W. (2002). Strategic attention deployment for delay of gratification in working and waiting situations. Developmental Psychology, 38(2), 313326. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.2.313CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peetz, J., & Kammrath, L. (2011). Only because I love you: Why people make and why they break promises in romantic relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(5), 887904. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021857CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peetz, J., & Kammrath, L. (2013). Folk understandings of self regulation in relationships: Recognizing the importance of self-regulatory ability for others, but not the self. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(4), 712718. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.02.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepper, G. V., & Nettle, D. (2017). The behavioural constellation of deprivation: Causes and consequences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, E314. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X1600234XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pescosolido, B. A., Monahan, J., Link, B. G., Stueve, A., & Kikuzawa, S. (1999). The public's view of the competence, dangerousness, and need for legal coercion of persons with mental health problems. American Journal of Public Health, 89(9), 13391345. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.89.9.1339CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, M. B. (2018). Reproductive interests and dimensions of political ideology. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(2), 203211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.12.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petersen, M. B., & Aarøe, L. (2015). Birth weight and social trust in adulthood: Evidence for early calibration of social cognition. Psychological Science, 26(11), 16811692. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615595622CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piazza, J., Landy, J. F., Chakroff, A., Young, L., & Wasserman, E. (2018). What disgust does and does not do for moral cognition. In Strohminger, N. & Kumar, V. (Eds.), The moral psychology of disgust (pp. 5381). Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
Pizarro, D., Inbar, Y., & Helion, C. (2011). On disgust and moral judgment. Emotion Review, 3(3), 267268. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073911402394CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Platek, S., & Singh, D. (2010). Optimal waist-to-hip ratios in women activate neural reward centers in men. PLoS ONE, 5, e9042. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0009042CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poushter, J. (2014). What's morally acceptable? It depends on where in the world you live. Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2014/04/15/whats-morally-acceptable-it-depends-on-where-in-the-world-you-live/Google Scholar
Powell, A. (2004). Only in paradise: Alcohol and Islam. In Robertson, C. K. (Ed.), Religion and alcohol: Sobering thoughts (pp. 95110). Lang.Google Scholar
Powell, R. (2013). The theoretical concept of the “civilising offensive” (beschavingsoffensief): Notes on its origins and uses. Human Figurations, 2(2). http://hdl.handle.net/2027/spo.11217607.0002.203Google Scholar
Pronk, T. M., Karremans, J. C., & Wigboldus, D. H. J. (2011). How can you resist? Executive control helps romantically involved individuals to stay faithful. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(5), 827837. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021993CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Puhl, R. M., & Heuer, C. A. (2010). Obesity stigma: Important considerations for public health. American Journal of Public Health, 100(6), 10191028. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2009.159491CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purzycki, B. G., Pisor, A. C., Apicella, C., Atkinson, Q., Cohen, E., Henrich, J., … Xygalatas, D. (2018). The cognitive and cultural foundations of moral behavior. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(5), 490501. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.04.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quintelier, K. J. P., Ishii, K., Weeden, J., Kurzban, R., & Braeckman, J. (2013). Individual differences in reproductive strategy are related to views about recreational drug use in Belgium, The Netherlands, and Japan. Human Nature, 24(2), 196217. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-013-9165-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rand, D. G. (2016). Cooperation, fast and slow: Meta-analytic evidence for a theory of social heuristics and self-interested deliberation. Psychological Science, 27(9), 11921206. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616654455CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rand, D. G. (2017). Social dilemma cooperation (unlike dictator game giving) is intuitive for men as well as women. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 73, 164168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.06.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rand, D. G., Greene, J. D., & Nowak, M. A. (2012). Spontaneous giving and calculated greed. Nature, 489(7416), 427430. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature11467CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rand, D. G., & Nowak, M. A. (2013). Human cooperation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(8), 413425. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rehm, J., Shield, K. D., Joharchi, N., & Shuper, P. A. (2012). Alcohol consumption and the intention to engage in unprotected sex: Systematic review and meta-analysis of experimental studies. Addiction, 107(1), 5159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rehman, F. N. (2019). Self-control in Islam and its psychological aspect. Journal of Religious Studies, 2(1), 1636.Google Scholar
Reminick, R. A. (1975). The structure and functions of religious belief among the Amhara of Ethiopia. In Proceedings of the first United States conference on Ethiopian studies, Michigan State University, 2–5 May, 1973 (issue 3, pp. 25–42). African Studies Center, Michigan State University. https://ehrafworldcultures-yale-edu.proxy.library.upenn.edu/document?id=mp05-022Google Scholar
Restubog, S. L. D., Garcia, P. R. J. M., Wang, L., & Cheng, D. (2010). It's all about control: The role of self-control in buffering the effects of negative reciprocity beliefs and trait anger on workplace deviance. Journal of Research in Personality, 44(5), 655660. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2010.06.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richerson, P., Baldini, R., Bell, A. V., Demps, K., Frost, K., Hillis, V., … Zefferman, M. (2016). Cultural group selection plays an essential role in explaining human cooperation: A sketch of the evidence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, E30. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X1400106XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Righetti, F., & Finkenauer, C. (2011). If you are able to control yourself, I will trust you: The role of perceived self-control in interpersonal trust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(5), 874886. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021827CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ringel, M. M., & Ditto, P. H. (2019). The moralization of obesity. Social Science & Medicine, 237, 112399. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2019.112399CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ritter, R. S., Preston, J. L., Salomon, E., & Relihan-Johnson, D. (2016). Imagine no religion: Heretical disgust, anger and the symbolic purity of mind. Cognition and Emotion, 30(4), 778796. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1030334CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, G. (2020). Honest signaling of cooperative intentions. Behavioral Ecology, 31(4), 922932. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/araa035CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Room, R. (1984). Alcohol and ethnography: A case of problem deflation? Current Anthropology, 25(2), 169191. https://doi.org/10.1086/203107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Room, R. (1996). Alcohol consumption and social harm – Conceptual issues and historical perspectives. Contemporary Drug Problems, 23(3), 373388. https://doi.org/10.1177/009145099602300304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rousselle, A. (2013). Porneia: On desire and the body in antiquity. Wipf and Stock.Google Scholar
Royzman, E., Atanasov, P., Landy, J. F., Parks, A., & Gepty, A. (2014). CAD or MAD? Anger (not disgust) as the predominant response to pathogen-free violations of the divinity code. Emotion, 14(5), 892907. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036829CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Royzman, E., & Kurzban, R. (2011). Minding the metaphor: The elusive character of moral disgust. Emotion Review, 3(3), 269271. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073911402371CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Royzman, E. B., & Sabini, J. (2001). Something it takes to be an emotion: The interesting case of disgust. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 31(1), 2959. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-5914.00145CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P., & Haidt, J. (2013). The domains of disgust and their origins: Contrasting biological and cultural evolutionary accounts. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(8), 367368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (2008). Disgust. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M., & Barrett, L. F. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 757776). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Rozin, P., Lowery, L., Imada, S., & Haidt, J. (1999). The CAD triad hypothesis: A mapping between three moral emotions (contempt, anger, disgust) and three moral codes (community, autonomy, divinity). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(4), 574586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruddock, H. K., & Hardman, C. A. (2017). Food addiction beliefs amongst the lay public: What are the consequences for eating behaviour? Current Addiction Reports, 4(2), 110115. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-017-0136-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruff, C. C., Ugazio, G., & Fehr, E. (2013). Changing social norm compliance with noninvasive brain stimulation. Science, 342(6157), 482484. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1241399CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saroglou, V., & Craninx, M. (2021). Religious moral righteousness over care: A review and a meta-analysis. Current Opinion in Psychology, 40, 7985. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.09.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schein, C., & Gray, K. (2015). The unifying moral dyad: Liberals and conservatives share the same harm-based moral template. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(8), 11471163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schein, C., & Gray, K. (2018). The theory of dyadic morality: Reinventing moral judgment by redefining harm. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 22(1), 3270. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868317698288CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schein, C., Ritter, R. S., & Gray, K. (2016). Harm mediates the disgust–immorality link. Emotion, 16(6), 862876. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000167CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schielke, S. (2009). Being good in Ramadan: Ambivalence, fragmentation, and the moral self in the lives of young Egyptians. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 15, S24S40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schilbach, F. (2019). Alcohol and self-control: A field experiment in India. American Economic Review, 109(4), 12901322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schlegel, A. (1991). Status, property, and the value on virginity. American Ethnologist, 18(4), 719734. https://doi.org/10.1525/ae.1991.18.4.02a00050CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmitt, D. P. (2004). The Big Five related to risky sexual behaviour across 10 world regions: Differential personality associations of sexual promiscuity and relationship infidelity. European Journal of Personality, 18(4), 301319. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.520CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sebastián-Enesco, C., & Warneken, F. (2015). The shadow of the future: 5-year-olds, but not 3-year-olds, adjust their sharing in anticipation of reciprocation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 129, 4054. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.08.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidman, S. (1990). The power of desire and the danger of pleasure: Victorian sexuality reconsidered. Journal of Social History, 24(1), 4767. https://doi.org/10.1353/jsh/24.1.47CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheehy-Skeffington, J. (2020). The effects of low socioeconomic status on decision-making processes. Current Opinion in Psychology, 33, 183188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.07.043CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheen, M., Yekani, H. A. K., & Jordan, T. R. (2018). Investigating the effect of wearing the hijab: Perception of facial attractiveness by Emirati Muslim women living in their native Muslim country. PLoS ONE, 13(10), e0199537. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199537CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sherkat, D. E., & Ellison, C. G. (1997). The cognitive structure of a moral crusade: Conservative Protestantism and opposition to pornography. Social Forces, 75(3), 957980. https://doi.org/10.1093/sf/75.3.957CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shively, M. (2001). Male self-control and sexual aggression. Deviant Behavior, 22(4), 295321. https://doi.org/10.1080/016396201750267843CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shternberg, L. I. (1933). The Gilyak, Orochi, Goldi, Negidal, Ainu: Articles and materials. Human Relations Area Files.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A., Mahapatra, M., & Miller, J. G. (1987). Culture and moral development. In Kagan, J. & Lamb, S. (Eds.), The emergence of morality in young children (pp. 183). University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A., Much, N. C., Mahapatra, M., & Park, L. (1997). The “Big Three” of morality (autonomy, community, divinity) and the “Big Three” explanations of suffering. In Morality and health (pp. 119169). Taylor & Francis/Routledge.Google Scholar
Siev, J., & Cohen, A. B. (2007). Is thought–action fusion related to religiosity? Differences between Christians and Jews. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(4), 829837. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2006.05.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silva, A. S., & Mace, R. (2014). Cooperation and conflict: Field experiments in Northern Ireland. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1792), 20141435. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.1435CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silver, E. (2020). Students’ attitudes toward college drinking: A moral intuitionist approach. Deviant Behavior, 41(8), 10331051. https://doi.org/10.1080/01639625.2019.1596538CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silver, E., & Silver, J. R. (2019). Morality and self-control: The role of binding and individualizing moral motives. Deviant Behavior, 42(3), 366385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, M. (2018). The cultural evolution of shamanism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 41, E66. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X17001893CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singh, M. (2021). Magic, explanations, and evil: The origins and design of witches and sorcerers. Current Anthropology, 62(1), 229. https://doi.org/10.1086/713111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, M. (2022). Subjective selection and the evolution of complex culture. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 31, 266280. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/4t2udCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singh, M., & Henrich, J. (2020). Why do religious leaders observe costly prohibitions? Examining taboos on Mentawai shamans. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 2, e32. https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2020.32CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singh, M., Wrangham, R., & Glowacki, L. (2017). Self-interest and the design of rules. Human Nature, 28(4), 457480. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-017-9298-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sjåstad, H. (2019). Short-sighted greed? Focusing on the future promotes reputation-based generosity. Judgment and Decision Making, 14(2), 199213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slingerland, E. G. (2014). Trying not to try: The art and science of spontaneity (1st ed.). Crown.Google Scholar
Sloman, S. A., Fernbach, P. M., & Ewing, S. (2009). Chapter 1: Causal models: The representational infrastructure for moral judgment. In Ross, B. H. (Ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 50, pp. 126). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)00401-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sommer, M. H. (2000). Sex, law, and society in late imperial China. Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Soutschek, A., Sauter, M., & Schubert, T. (2015). The importance of the lateral prefrontal cortex for strategic decision making in the prisoner's dilemma. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 15, 854860. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-015-0372-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spicer, K., & Platek, S. (2010). Curvaceous female bodies activate neural reward centers in men. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 3, 282283. https://doi.org/10.4161/cib.3.3.11560CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spiegel, J. S. (2020). Cultivating self-control: Foundations and methods in the Christian theological tradition. Journal of Spiritual Formation and Soul Care, 13(2), 193210. https://doi.org/10.1177/1939790920918881CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanford, P. K. (2018). The difference between ice cream and Nazis: Moral externalization and the evolution of human cooperation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 41, e95. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X17001911CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steim, R. I., & Nemeroff, C. J. (1995). Moral overtones of food: Judgments of others based on what they eat. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(5), 480490. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167295215006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephens, W. N. (1972). A cross-cultural study of modesty. Behavior Science Notes, 7(1), 128. https://doi.org/10.1177/106939717200700101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sterckx, R. (Ed.). (2005). Of tripod and palate. Palgrave Macmillan US. https://doi.org/10.1057/9781403979278CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, J. R., Cushman, F. A., & Hauser, M. D. (2005). Evolving the psychological mechanisms for cooperation. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, 36(1), 499518. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.36.113004.083814CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Story, G. W., Vlaev, I., Seymour, B., Darzi, A., & Dolan, R. J. (2014). Does temporal discounting explain unhealthy behavior? A systematic review and reinforcement learning perspective. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 120. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00076CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strang, S., Gross, J., Schuhmann, T., Riedl, A., Weber, B., & Sack, A. T. (2015). Be nice if you have to – The neurobiological roots of strategic fairness. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(6), 790796. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strassmann, B. I. (1992). The function of menstrual taboos among the Dogon: Defense against cuckoldry? Human Nature, 3(2), 89131. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02692249CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strassmann, B. I., Kurapati, N. T., Hug, B. F., Burke, E. E., Gillespie, B. W., Karafet, T. M., & Hammer, M. F. (2012). Religion as a means to assure paternity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(25), 97819785. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1110442109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stunkard, A., LaFleur, W., & Wadden, T. (1998). Stigmatization of obesity in medieval times: Asia and Europe. International Journal of Obesity, 22(12), 11411144. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0800753CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stylianou, S. (2004). The role of religiosity in the opposition to drug use. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 48(4), 429448. https://doi.org/10.1177/0306624X03261253CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stylianou, S. (2010). Victimless deviance: Toward a classification of opposition justifications. Western Criminology Review, 11(2), 4356.Google Scholar
Suiming, P. (1998). The move toward spiritual asceticism in Chinese sexual culture. Chinese Sociology & Anthropology, 31(1), 1424. https://doi.org/10.2753/CSA0009-4625310114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sweeney, E. (2012). Aquinas on the seven deadly sins: Tradition and innovation. In Newhauser, R. & Ridyard, S. (Eds.), Sin in medieval and early modern culture: The tradition of the seven deadly sins (pp. 85106). Boydell & Brewer. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781782047414.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Symons, D.. (1995). Beauty is in the adaptations of the beholder: The evolutionary psychology of human female sexual attractiveness. In Ambramson, P. R. & Pinkerton, S. D. (Eds.), Sexual nature, sexual culture (pp. 80118). University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Sznycer, D., Lopez Seal, M. F., Sell, A., Lim, J., Porat, R., Shalvi, S., … Tooby, J. (2017). Support for redistribution is shaped by compassion, envy, and self-interest, but not a taste for fairness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(31), 84208425. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1703801114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamir, C., Connaughton, A., & Salazar, A. M. (2020). People's thoughts on whether belief in God is necessary to be moral vary by economic development, education and age. 39.Google Scholar
Tamney, J. B. (1980). Fasting and modernization. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 19(2), 129. https://doi.org/10.2307/1386247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamney, J. B. (1986). Fasting and dieting: A research note. Review of Religious Research, 27(3), 255262. https://doi.org/10.2307/3511420CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tariq, T. (2014). Let modesty be her raiment: The classical context of ancient-Christian veiling. Implicit Religion, 16(4), 493506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tentler, T. N. (2015). Sin and confession on the eve of the reformation. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thielmann, I., Spadaro, G., & Balliet, D. (2020). Personality and prosocial behavior: A theoretical framework and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 146(1), 3090. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000217CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomsen, K. R., Callesen, M. B., Hesse, M., Kvamme, T. L., Pedersen, M. M., Pedersen, M. U., & Voon, V. (2018). Impulsivity traits and addiction-related behaviors in youth. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7(2), 317330. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.7.2018.22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tian, A. D., Schroeder, J., Häubl, G., Risen, J. L., Norton, M. I., & Gino, F. (2018). Enacting rituals to improve self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 114(6), 851876. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tierney, W., Hardy, J., Ebersole, C. R., Viganola, D., Clemente, E. G., Gordon, M., … Uhlmann, E. L. (2021). A creative destruction approach to replication: Implicit work and sex morality across cultures. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 93, 104060. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2020.104060CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tiwald, J. (2020). Song-Ming Confucianism. In Zalta, E. N. (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (summer 2020). Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2020/entries/song-ming-confucianism/Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2020). The moral psychology of obligation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 43, E56. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X19001742CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2010). Groups in mind: The coalitional roots of war and morality. In Hogh-Olesen, H., Boesch, C., & Cosmides, L. (Eds.), Human morality and sociality: Evolutionary and comparative perspectives (pp. 91234). Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 46(1), 3557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., Kurzban, R., & DeScioli, P. (2013). Disgust: Evolved function and structure. Psychological Review, 120(1), 6584. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030778CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uhlmann, E. L., Poehlman, T. A., Tannenbaum, D., & Bargh, J. A. (2011). Implicit puritanism in American moral cognition. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(2), 312320. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.10.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Gelder, J.-L., Hershfield, H. E., & Nordgren, L. F. (2013). Vividness of the future self predicts delinquency. Psychological Science, 24(6), 974980. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612465197CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Leeuwen, F., Koenig, B. L., Graham, J., & Park, J. H. (2014). Moral concerns across the United States: Associations with life-history variables, pathogen prevalence, urbanization, cognitive ability, and social class. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(6), 464471. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.06.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vatuk, V. P., & Vatuk, S. J. (1967). Chatorpan: A culturally defined form of addiction in North India. International Journal of the Addictions, 2(1), 103113. https://doi.org/10.3109/10826086709074414CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vazsonyi, A. T., Mikuška, J., & Kelley, E. L. (2017). It's time: A meta-analysis on the self-control–deviance link. Journal of Criminal Justice, 48, 4863. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2016.10.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vengeliene, V., Bilbao, A., Molander, A., & Spanagel, R. (2008). Neuropharmacology of alcohol addiction. British Journal of Pharmacology, 154(2), 299315. https://doi.org/10.1038/bjp.2008.30CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veyne, P. (1978). La famille et l'amour sous le Haut-Empire Romain. Annales. Histoire, Sciences Sociales, 33(1), 3563.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volkow, N. D., Wang, G.-J., & Baler, R. D. (2011). Reward, dopamine and the control of food intake: Implications for obesity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(1), 3746. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volkow, N. D., Wise, R. A., & Baler, R. (2017). The dopamine motive system: Implications for drug and food addiction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(12), 741752. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2017.130CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vonasch, A. J., Clark, C. J., Lau, S., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2017). Ordinary people associate addiction with loss of free will. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 5, 5666. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.abrep.2017.01.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vonasch, A. J., & Sjåstad, H. (2021). Future-orientation (as trait and state) promotes reputation-protective choice in moral dilemmas. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12(3), 383391. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550619899257CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, A. L. (1997). Adversaries of dance: From the puritans to the present. University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Walzer, M. (1963). Puritanism as a revolutionary ideology. History and Theory, 3(1), 5990. https://doi.org/10.2307/2504304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walzer, M. (1982). The revolution of the saints: A study in the origins of radical politics. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Warner, J. (1997). Shifting categories of the social harms associated with alcohol: Examples from late medieval and early modern England. American Journal of Public Health, 87(11), 17881797.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weafer, J., Mitchell, S. H., & de Wit, H. (2014). Recent translational findings on impulsivity in relation to drug abuse. Current Addiction Reports, 1(4), 289300. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-014-0035-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weber, M. (1968). The religion of China: Confucianism and Taoism (New ed.). Free Press.Google Scholar
Weeden, J., Cohen, A. B., & Kenrick, D. T. (2008). Religious attendance as reproductive support. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(5), 327334. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2008.03.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weeden, J., & Kurzban, R. (2013). What predicts religiosity? A multinational analysis of reproductive and cooperative morals. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(6), 440445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.08.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weeden, J., & Kurzban, R. (2016). The hidden agenda of the political mind: How self-interest shapes our opinions and why we won't admit it. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Weiner, A. B. (1988). The Trobrianders of Papua New Guinea. Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Weiss, A., Forstmann, M., & Burgmer, P. (2021). Moralizing mental states: The role of trait self-control and control perceptions. Cognition, 214, 104662. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104662CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, S., & Yao, P. (2018). Discourses on gender and sexuality. In Benjamin, C. (Ed.), The Cambridge world history: Vol. 4, A world with states, empires and networks 1200 BCE–900 CE (pp. 154178). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
West, S. A., Cooper, G. A., Ghoul, M. B., & Griffin, A. S. (2021). Ten recent insights for our understanding of cooperation. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 5(4), 419430. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41559-020-01384-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
West, S. A., El Mouden, C., & Gardner, A. (2011). Sixteen common misconceptions about the evolution of cooperation in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(4), 231262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.08.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
West, S. A., Griffin, A. S., & Gardner, A. (2007). Social semantics: Altruism, cooperation, mutualism, strong reciprocity and group selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 20(2), 415432. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01258.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitehead, A. L., & Perry, S. L. (2018). Unbuckling the Bible belt: A state-level analysis of religious factors and Google searches for porn. The Journal of Sex Research, 55(3), 273283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiessner, P., & Tumu, A. (1998). Historical vines: Enga networks of exchange, ritual and warfare in Papua New Guinea. Smithsonian Institution Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (2004). Do pretty women inspire men to discount the future? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 271(Suppl 4), S177S179. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2003.0134CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, C. (2017). Ritual well-being: Toward a social signaling model of religion and mental health. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 7(3), 223243. https://doi.org/10.1080/2153599X.2016.1156556CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yamagishi, T., Matsumoto, Y., Kiyonari, T., Takagishi, H., Li, Y., Kanai, R., & Sakagami, M. (2017). Response time in economic games reflects different types of decision conflict for prosocial and proself individuals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114, 201608877. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1608877114CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, L. H., Wong, L. Y., Grivel, M. M., & Hasin, D. S. (2017). Stigma and substance use disorders: An international phenomenon. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 30(5), 378388. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000351CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yaple, Z. A., & Yu, R. (2020). Functional and structural brain correlates of socioeconomic status. Cerebral Cortex, 30(1), 181196. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhz080CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeomans, H. (2011). What did the British temperance movement accomplish? Attitudes to alcohol, the law and moral regulation. Sociology, 45(1), 3853. https://doi.org/10.1177/0038038510387189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Y. (2021). The religious ethic and mercantile spirit in early modern China. Columbia University Press. https://doi.org/10.7312/yu-20042CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zwirner, E., & Raihani, N. (2020). Neighbourhood wealth, not urbanicity, predicts prosociality towards strangers. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 287(1936), 20201359. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2020.1359CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Examples of folk-psychological beliefs about modifiers of trait-self-control