Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T15:41:23.821Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Conceptualization, context, and comparison are key to understanding the evolution of fear

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 May 2023

Jacob C. Dunn
Affiliation:
Behavioural Ecology Research Group, Anglia Ruskin University, Cambridge CB1 1PT, UK [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected] https://www.thepeergroup.org.uk; https://aru.ac.uk/people/caglar-akcay; https://aru.ac.uk/people/krishna-n-balasubramaniam; https://aru.ac.uk/people/rachael-miller-harrison; https://aru.ac.uk/people/claudia-wascher Department of Cognitive Biology, University of Vienna, 1090 Vienna, Austria Biological Anthropology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 1QH, UK
Rachael Miller
Affiliation:
Behavioural Ecology Research Group, Anglia Ruskin University, Cambridge CB1 1PT, UK [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected] https://www.thepeergroup.org.uk; https://aru.ac.uk/people/caglar-akcay; https://aru.ac.uk/people/krishna-n-balasubramaniam; https://aru.ac.uk/people/rachael-miller-harrison; https://aru.ac.uk/people/claudia-wascher Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, UK
Krishna Balasubramaniam
Affiliation:
Behavioural Ecology Research Group, Anglia Ruskin University, Cambridge CB1 1PT, UK [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected] https://www.thepeergroup.org.uk; https://aru.ac.uk/people/caglar-akcay; https://aru.ac.uk/people/krishna-n-balasubramaniam; https://aru.ac.uk/people/rachael-miller-harrison; https://aru.ac.uk/people/claudia-wascher
Çağlar Akçay
Affiliation:
Behavioural Ecology Research Group, Anglia Ruskin University, Cambridge CB1 1PT, UK [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected] https://www.thepeergroup.org.uk; https://aru.ac.uk/people/caglar-akcay; https://aru.ac.uk/people/krishna-n-balasubramaniam; https://aru.ac.uk/people/rachael-miller-harrison; https://aru.ac.uk/people/claudia-wascher
Claudia A. F. Wascher
Affiliation:
Behavioural Ecology Research Group, Anglia Ruskin University, Cambridge CB1 1PT, UK [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected] https://www.thepeergroup.org.uk; https://aru.ac.uk/people/caglar-akcay; https://aru.ac.uk/people/krishna-n-balasubramaniam; https://aru.ac.uk/people/rachael-miller-harrison; https://aru.ac.uk/people/claudia-wascher

Abstract

The fearful ape hypothesis proposes that heightened fearfulness in humans is adaptive. However, despite its attractive anthropocentric narrative, the evidence presented for greater fearfulness in humans versus other apes is not sufficient to support this claim. Conceptualization, context, and comparison are strongly lacking in Grossmann's proposal, but are key to understanding variation in the fear response among individuals and species.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Grossmann presents the fearful ape hypothesis, claiming that from infancy humans are more fearful than other apes, and that “in the context of cooperative caregiving and provisioning unique to human great ape group life, heightened fearfulness was adaptive” (target article, abstract). We consider Grossmann's hypothesis to represent a false premise. We question the evidence provided to demonstrate heightened fearfulness in humans, which is critical to the author's argument. In the absence of such evidence, the proposed hypothesis does not take us beyond adaptationist storytelling (Gould & Lewontin, Reference Gould and Lewontin1979) and we believe it should currently be treated as such. We argue that conceptualization, context, and comparison are strongly lacking in Grossmann's proposal, but are key to understanding variation in the fear response among individuals and species.

Grossmann discusses heightened fearfulness in humans without any explicit conceptualization or definition of how we should measure this multifaceted and complex emotional state (Boissy, Reference Boissy1995). While subjective conscious experience of emotion may be asked verbally of humans, this is not possible in pre-verbal infants, humans without language, or other animal species. Therefore, proxy measurements of emotional processes (including fear) have been used, including behavioural, neurobiological, physiological, and cognitive responses of individuals to a variety of different stimuli (Paul, Harding, & Mendl, Reference Paul, Harding and Mendl2005). Such methods offer important insights into the evolution of emotion. However, the results can be very difficult to interpret and judgements concerning affective states are stronger if based on multiple techniques and/or correlated measures, with appropriate controls (Miller, Garner, & Mench, Reference Miller, Garner and Mench2006; Safryghin, Hebesberger, & Wascher, Reference Safryghin, Hebesberger and Wascher2019). Grossmann cites a single study (Herrmann, Hare, Cissewski, & Tomasello, Reference Herrmann, Hare, Cissewski and Tomasello2011) as evidence for heightened fearfulness in humans. However, that study (Herrmann et al., Reference Herrmann, Hare, Cissewski and Tomasello2011) uniquely examines the behavioural responses of infant humans and apes to specific novel objects as a measure of temperament (shyness–boldness, or neophobia). Interpretation of this methodological paradigm has recently been criticized (Takola, Krause, Müller, & Schielzeth, Reference Takola, Krause, Müller and Schielzeth2021), and the inference that affective states such as fear may be judged by the behavioural response to novel objects alone is not supported by the current literature (Crane, Brown, Chivers, & Ferrari, Reference Crane, Brown, Chivers and Ferrari2020; Greggor, Thornton, & Clayton, Reference Greggor, Thornton and Clayton2015). A single study, based on a small number of “WEIRD” (Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, Reference Henrich, Heine and Norenzayan2010) and “STRANGE” (Webster & Rutz, Reference Webster and Rutz2020) subjects (24 bonobos, 24 chimpanzees, 24 orangutans, and 14 German children), and which uses a single behavioural measure, is simply not sufficient to establish the key evidence for Grossmann's claim.

Grossmann also characterizes fearfulness (measured as neophobia) as a species-level trait, which does not vary across individuals or contexts. However, responses to novelty are known to be highly context dependent, varying with a range of social, cultural, environmental, and developmental factors (Coleman & Mellgren, Reference Coleman and Mellgren1994; Forss, Koski, & van Schaik, Reference Forss, Koski and van Schaik2017; Greggor, Clayton, Fulford, & Thornton, Reference Greggor, Clayton, Fulford and Thornton2016; Miller, Bugnyar, Pölzl, & Schwab, Reference Miller, Bugnyar, Pölzl and Schwab2015; Moretti, Hentrup, Kotrschal, & Range, Reference Moretti, Hentrup, Kotrschal and Range2015; Webster & Lefebvre, Reference Webster and Lefebvre2000). For example, food neophobia in children varies considerably across differing cultural environments (Birch, Reference Birch1995). Furthermore, some species show high individual repeatability in their responses over time, and to different types of novelty, whereas other species are seemingly more flexible (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Lambert, Frohnwieser, Brecht, Bugnyar, Crampton and Clayton2022; Vernouillet & Kelly, Reference Vernouillet and Kelly2020). More broadly, ecological factors such as resource availability (Greggor et al., Reference Greggor, Clayton, Fulford and Thornton2016) and, in particular, predation pressure (Isbell, Reference Isbell2006; Kawai & Koda, Reference Kawai and Koda2016; Öhman, Reference Öhman2009) are known to play a significant role in both intra- and inter-specific variation in fear responses (Mobbs, Hagan, Dalgleish, Silston, & Prévost, Reference Mobbs, Hagan, Dalgleish, Silston and Prévost2015; Pellman & Kim, Reference Pellman and Kim2016); often conceptualized as “the ecology of fear” (e.g., Laundre, Hernandez, & Ripple, Reference Laundre, Hernandez and Ripple2010). Context can define whether neophobia is adaptive for an individual in the evolutionary sense. For instance, fear responses stemming from animals' encounters with predators can be beneficial to an individual in terms of increasing its survival (Mobbs et al., Reference Mobbs, Hagan, Dalgleish, Silston and Prévost2015; Pellman & Kim, Reference Pellman and Kim2016), or may influence adaptive evolutionary phenomena such as the development of enhanced visual systems (Isbell, Reference Isbell2006). On the other hand, fear responses can also be costly in terms of leading to poor innovation, competitive ability, or increased stress-related mortality (Crane et al., Reference Crane, Brown, Chivers and Ferrari2020). We therefore consider that the links between socioecological context and fear response behaviours, and their (mal)adaptive effects on free-living animals, are key considerations before any conclusions about the adaptiveness of fearfulness can be drawn.

Finally, although Grossmann places a strong emphasis on “phylogenetic origins” as evidence to support the fearful ape hypothesis, the evidence presented is highly hominoid-centric and no formal comparative analyses are conducted. There is no reference to non-ape species, despite the large literature on neophobia and fear responses in many other clades (Crane et al., Reference Crane, Brown, Chivers and Ferrari2020; Laundre et al., Reference Laundre, Hernandez and Ripple2010). As behavioural ecologists, we find this disappointing, as neither fearfulness, nor cooperative care (which is distinct from cooperative breeding, as Grossmann points out; Clutton-Brock, Reference Clutton-Brock, Kappeler and van Schaik2006; König, Reference König1997), are unique to humans. There are many examples of fear (as measured by neophobia, shyness) and recognition of emotions, for example, in response to predators, in several other primate species. Likewise, there is a large body of evidence for cooperative care, sometimes even independently of kinship (Clutton-Brock, Reference Clutton-Brock, Kappeler and van Schaik2006; Koenig & Dickinson, Reference Koenig and Dickinson2016). The socioecological drivers of neophobia and the role of individual differences have also been well studied in corvids (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Lambert, Frohnwieser, Brecht, Bugnyar, Crampton and Clayton2022; Vernouillet & Kelly, Reference Vernouillet and Kelly2020). Indeed, some species show high levels of both neophobia and cooperative care (Horn et al., Reference Horn, Bugnyar, Griesser, Hengl, Izawa, Oortwijn and Massen2020), which would seem to be highly relevant to the proposed hypothesis. Comparative analyses have also been reported for other families of birds, as well as for ungulates (Blumstein, Reference Blumstein2006; Mettke-Hofmann, Winkler, & Leisler, Reference Mettke-Hofmann, Winkler and Leisler2002; Schaffer et al., Reference Schaffer, Caicoya, Colell, Holland, von Fersen, Widdig and Amici2021), but are not considered in Grossmann's review. There is a growing corpus of data on this topic from which to develop more robust hypotheses and more formal empirical analyses. Therefore, a broader comparative analysis, controlling for phylogenetic relatedness, would surely provide much greater insight into the evolution of fear responses.

Competing interest

None.

Footnotes

*

These authors contributed equally.

References

Birch, L. L. (1995). Research in review. Children's eating: The development of food-acceptance patterns. Young Children, 50(2), 7178.Google Scholar
Blumstein, D. T. (2006). Developing an evolutionary ecology of fear: How life history and natural history traits affect disturbance tolerance in birds. Animal Behaviour, 71(2), 389399. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.05.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boissy, A. (1995). Fear and fearfulness in animals. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 70(2), 165191. https://doi.org/10.1086/418981CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clutton-Brock, T. H. (2006). Cooperative breeding in mammals. In Kappeler, P. M. & van Schaik, C. P. (Eds.), Cooperation in primates and humans: Mechanisms and evolution (pp. 173190). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/3-540-28277-7_10CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coleman, S. L., & Mellgren, R. L. (1994). Neophobia when feeding alone or in flocks in zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata. Animal Behaviour, 48(4), 903907. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1994.1315CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crane, A. L., Brown, G. E., Chivers, D. P., & Ferrari, M. C. O. (2020). An ecological framework of neophobia: From cells to organisms to populations. Biological Reviews, 95(1), 218231. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12560CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forss, S. I. F., Koski, S. E., & van Schaik, C. P. (2017). Explaining the paradox of neophobic explorers: The social information hypothesis. International Journal of Primatology, 38(5), 799822. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-017-9984-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gould, S. J., & Lewontin, R. C. (1979). The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 205(1161), 581598. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1979.0086Google Scholar
Greggor, A. L., Clayton, N. S., Fulford, A. J. C., & Thornton, A. (2016). Street smart: Faster approach towards litter in urban areas by highly neophobic corvids and less fearful birds. Animal Behaviour, 117, 123133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.03.029CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greggor, A. L., Thornton, A., & Clayton, N. S. (2015). Neophobia is not only avoidance: Improving neophobia tests by combining cognition and ecology. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 6, 8289. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.10.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). Beyond WEIRD: Towards a broad-based behavioral science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 111135. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X10000725CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrmann, E., Hare, B., Cissewski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2011). A comparison of temperament in nonhuman apes and human infants. Developmental Science, 14(6), 13931405. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01082.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horn, L., Bugnyar, T., Griesser, M., Hengl, M., Izawa, E.-I., Oortwijn, T., … Massen, J. J. (2020). Sex-specific effects of cooperative breeding and colonial nesting on prosociality in corvids. eLife, 9, e58139. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.58139CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isbell, L. A. (2006). Snakes as agents of evolutionary change in primate brains. Journal of Human Evolution, 51(1), 135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2005.12.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kawai, N., & Koda, H. (2016). Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata) quickly detect snakes but not spiders: Evolutionary origins of fear-relevant animals. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 130(3), 299303. https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000032CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koenig, W. D., & Dickinson, J. L. (Eds.) (2016). Cooperative breeding in vertebrates: Studies of ecology, evolution, and behavior. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107338357CrossRefGoogle Scholar
König, B. (1997). Cooperative care of young in mammals. Die Naturwissenschaften, 84(3), 95104. https://doi.org/10.1007/s001140050356Google ScholarPubMed
Laundre, J. W., Hernandez, L., & Ripple, W. J. (2010). The landscape of fear: Ecological implications of being afraid. The Open Ecology Journal, 3, 17. https://benthamopen.com/ABSTRACT/TOECOLJ-3-3-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mettke-Hofmann, C., Winkler, H., & Leisler, B. (2002). The significance of ecological factors for exploration and neophobia in parrots. Ethology, 108(3), 249272. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1439-0310.2002.00773.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, K. A., Garner, J. P., & Mench, J. A. (2006). Is fearfulness a trait that can be measured with behavioural tests? A validation of four fear tests for Japanese quail. Animal Behaviour, 71(6), 13231334. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.08.018CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, R., Bugnyar, T., Pölzl, K., & Schwab, C. (2015). Differences in exploration behaviour in common ravens and carrion crows during development and across social context. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 69(7), 12091220. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-015-1935-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, R., Lambert, M. L., Frohnwieser, A., Brecht, K. F., Bugnyar, T., Crampton, I., … Clayton, N. S. (2022). Socioecological correlates of neophobia in corvids. Current Biology, 32(1), 7485.e4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2021.10.045CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mobbs, D., Hagan, C. C., Dalgleish, T., Silston, B., & Prévost, C. (2015). The ecology of human fear: Survival optimization and the nervous system. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 9, 55. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2015.00055CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moretti, L., Hentrup, M., Kotrschal, K., & Range, F. (2015). The influence of relationships on neophobia and exploration in wolves and dogs. Animal Behaviour, 107, 159173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.06.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Öhman, A. (2009). Of snakes and faces: An evolutionary perspective on the psychology of fear. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 50(6), 543552. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00784.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paul, E. S., Harding, E. J., & Mendl, M. (2005). Measuring emotional processes in animals: The utility of a cognitive approach. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 29(3), 469491. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.01.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pellman, B. A., & Kim, J. J. (2016). What can ethobehavioral studies tell us about the brain's fear system? Trends in Neurosciences, 39(6), 420431. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2016.04.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Safryghin, A., Hebesberger, D. V., & Wascher, C. A. F. (2019). Testing for behavioral and physiological responses of domestic horses (Equus caballus) across different contexts – Consistency over time and effects of context. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 849. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00849CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaffer, A., Caicoya, A. L., Colell, M., Holland, R., von Fersen, L., Widdig, A., & Amici, F. (2021). Neophobia in 10 ungulate species – A comparative approach. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 75(7), 102. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-021-03041-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Takola, E., Krause, E. T., Müller, C., & Schielzeth, H. (2021). Novelty at second glance: A critical appraisal of the novel object paradigm based on meta-analysis. Animal Behaviour, 180, 123142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2021.07.018CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vernouillet, A., & Kelly, D. M. (2020). Individual exploratory responses are not repeatable across time or context for four species of food-storing corvid. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 394. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-56138-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webster, M. M., & Rutz, C. (2020). How STRANGE are your study animals? Nature, 582(7812), 337340. https://doi.org/10.1038/d41586-020-01751-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webster, S. J., & Lefebvre, L. (2000). Neophobia by the lesser-Antillean bullfinch, a foraging generalist, and the bananaquit, a nectar specialist. The Wilson Bulletin, 112(3), 424427. https://doi.org/10.1676/0043-5643(2000)112[0424:NBTLAB]2.0.CO;2CrossRefGoogle Scholar