Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-w7rtg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-10-05T21:32:25.392Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Fear signals vulnerability and appeasement, not threat

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 May 2023

Abigail A. Marsh*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Georgetown University, Washington, DC 20057, USA [email protected] www.abigailmarsh.com

Abstract

Humans are not only fearful apes, but we also communicate our fear using social cues. Social fear displays typically elicit care and assistance in the real world and the lab. But in the psychology and neuroscience literature fearful expressions are commonly interpreted as “threat cues.” The fearful ape hypothesis suggests that fearful expressions should be instead considered appeasement and vulnerability cues.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Social displays of fearfulness – that is, fearful facial, vocal, or body expressions – are commonly interpreted by psychologists and neuroscientists as “threat cues”; that is, cues that primarily serve to signal that the perceiver may be in danger. Fearful expressions have been variously described as cues that “trigger an automatic response to potential danger” (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Frith, Perrett, Rowland, Young and Calder1996), “implicit signals of broad environmental threat” (e.g., Farber, Kim, Knodt, & Hariri, Reference Farber, Kim, Knodt and Hariri2019), a “cue of potential threat” (Powers & Casey, Reference Powers and Casey2015), and, “threatening” (Imbriano, Sussman, Jin, & Mohanty, Reference Imbriano, Sussman, Jin and Mohanty2020).

Grossmann's compelling synthesis of the social functions of fear in humans may fundamentally change this practice.

The idea that decontextualized fearful expressions primarily represent threat cues can be traced back at least to the early days of human functional brain imaging research, when several seminal studies investigated patterns of brain activation when participants viewed various types of facial expressions (Breiter et al., Reference Breiter, Etcoff, Whalen, Kennedy, Rauch, Buckner and Rosen1996; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Frith, Perrett, Rowland, Young and Calder1996; Phillips et al., Reference Phillips, Young, Senior, Brammer, Andrew, Calder and David1997). All found increased recruitment of the amygdala when participants saw faces expressing fear relative to other emotions (happiness, disgust, or anger) or no emotion (neutral expressions) – a pattern that largely held up in subsequent research (Fusar-Poli et al., Reference Fusar-Poli, Placentino, Carletti, Landi, Allen, Surguladze and Politi2009).

The interpretation of these effects was strongly influenced by Morris et al. (Reference Morris, Frith, Perrett, Rowland, Young and Calder1996), who surmised that, because “integrated responses to threat or danger … can be mediated by the amygdala,” one could infer that, “perceiving an expression of fear in a conspecific may trigger an automatic response to potential danger that accounts for the observed amygdala activation in response to fearful faces” (p. 814). But problems soon became apparent with the reverse inference that an amygdala response to a face means the face signals threat. For example, it was discovered that the amygdala responds more strongly to fearful expressions than to angry expressions (Fusar-Poli et al., Reference Fusar-Poli, Placentino, Carletti, Landi, Allen, Surguladze and Politi2009; Whalen et al., Reference Whalen, Shin, McInerney, Fischer, Wright and Rauch2001), the latter of which clearly signal a direct interpersonal threat (Blair, Reference Blair2012). In an effort to resolve this discrepancy, it was suggested that perhaps fear is the most alarming expression of all because it represents an ambiguous threat (Whalen et al., Reference Whalen, Shin, McInerney, Fischer, Wright and Rauch2001) – that is, the expresser is not themselves the threat but is responding to an unspecified threat elsewhere in the environment. This remains a common interpretation of the social function of fear in psychology and neuroscience to this day (e.g., Farber et al., Reference Farber, Kim, Knodt and Hariri2019; Imbriano et al., Reference Imbriano, Sussman, Jin and Mohanty2020).

But this interpretation has flaws. One is that if someone is looking at you in fear, an obvious potential reason – absent any other contextual cues – is that they are afraid of you (Rhoads et al., Reference Rhoads, Cardinale, O'Connell, Palmer, VanMeter and Marsh2020). Another is that, as Grossmann's review makes clear, the “threat cue” interpretation of fear does not account for how perceivers actually respond to others' fear in the social world, which is often with care and the inhibition of aggression.

The care-eliciting effects of fear have been observed both in various social settings, as Grossmann describes, and in the laboratory. For example, even subliminally presented fearful expressions reliably elicit increased self-reported care and concern (Brethel-Haurwitz et al., Reference Brethel-Haurwitz, O'Connell, Cardinale, Stoianova, Stoycos and Lozier2017; Marsh & Ambady, Reference Marsh and Ambady2007). Consistent with this, expressions of fear also reliably elicit behavioral approach in perceivers, as measured using a lever task (Hammer & Marsh, Reference Hammer and Marsh2015; Marsh, Ambady, & Kleck, Reference Marsh, Ambady and Kleck2005b). This is the opposite response one would expect if perceivers primarily interpreted fearful expressions as threats. Thus, it appears that fear functions more reliably as a cue that conveys vulnerability and appeasement, similar to the bared-teeth displays other primates use to signal submission and appeasement (Vlaeyen et al., Reference Vlaeyen, Heesen, Kret, Clay, Bionda and Kim2022), and to the submission and fear displays used in other alloparenting mammals like wolves (Schenkel, Reference Schenkel1967). Human fearful expressions may elicit caring responses particularly effectively when displayed by children, consistent with Grossmann's hypothesis. But they may also succeed in eliciting care among adults in part because the components of a fearful expression – widened eyes, raised brows, and a rounded lower face – cause the expresser's face to appear more babyish (Marsh, Adams, & Kleck, Reference Marsh, Adams and Kleck2005a) – which may stimulate human adults' alloparental tendencies.

How, then, should observed neural responses to fear cues be understood? Why does the amygdala respond so robustly to fearful expressions? One logical conclusion stemming from Grossmann's hypothesis is that amygdala responses to others' fear are empathic (Marsh, Reference Marsh2016). Just as the regions involved in coordinating the personal experience of pain are recruited when observing another's pain in order to interpret their experience and respond to them adaptively (Lamm et al., Reference Lamm, Rütgen and Wagner2019), so the regions involved in coordinating the personal experience of fear (including the amygdala) may be recruited in response to others' fear in order to interpret their experience and respond adaptively. In many cases, the appropriate response to another's fear is protection and care. That unusually caring adults recruit the amygdala more than typical adults when viewing others' fear supports this conclusion (Marsh et al., Reference Marsh, Stoycos, Brethel-Haurwitz, Robinson and Cardinale2014). Of course, in some contexts the most appropriate response to others' fear might be social fear learning (Olsson & Phelps, Reference Olsson and Phelps2007) or marshaling one's own fearful response.

But fundamentally, if humans are indeed not only fearful apes, but apes endowed with a multitude of social cues to convey our fear to each other, it supports the idea that we are also caring apes who can generally be trusted to respond to others' displays of fear with the help they are seeking rather than selfishly fleeing or exploiting their vulnerability.

Both our fearfulness and our readiness to signal fear to one another can thus be interpreted as evidence of our species' prosocial nature.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Competing interest

None.

References

Blair, R. J. R. (2012). Considering anger from a neuroscience perspective. WIREs Cognitive Science, 3, 6574. doi:10.1002/wcs.154CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breiter, H. C., Etcoff, N. L., Whalen, P. J., Kennedy, W. A., Rauch, S. L., Buckner, R. L., &… Rosen, B. R. (1996). Response and habituation of the human amygdala during visual processing of facial expression. Neuron, 17(5), 875887. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80219-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brethel-Haurwitz, K. M., O'Connell, K., Cardinale, E. M., Stoianova, M., Stoycos, S. A., Lozier, L. M., … Marsh, A. A. (2017). Amygdala–midbrain connectivity indicates a role for the mammalian parental care system in human altruism. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 284, 20171731. doi:10.1098/rspb.2017.1731CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farber, M. J., Kim, M. J., Knodt, A. R., & Hariri, A. R. (2019). Maternal overprotection in childhood is associated with amygdala reactivity and structural connectivity in adulthood. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 40, 100711. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100711CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fusar-Poli, P., Placentino, A., Carletti, F., Landi, P., Allen, P., Surguladze, S., … Politi, P. (2009). Functional atlas of emotional faces processing: A voxel-based meta-analysis of 105 functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 34(6), 418432.Google ScholarPubMed
Hammer, J. L., & Marsh, A. A. (2015). Why do fearful facial expressions elicit behavioral approach? Evidence from a combined approach–avoidance implicit association test. Emotion, 15, 223231. doi:10.1037/emo0000054CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Imbriano, G., Sussman, T. J., Jin, J., & Mohanty, A. (2020). The role of imagery in threat-related perceptual decision making. Emotion, 20(8), 14951501. doi:10.1037/emo0000610CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamm, C., Rütgen, M., & Wagner, I. C. (2019). Imaging empathy and prosocial emotions. Neuroscience Letters, 693, 4953. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2017.06.054CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, A. A. (2016). Understanding amygdala responsiveness to fearful expressions through the lens of psychopathy and altruism. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 94(6), 513525. doi:10.1002/jnr.23668CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, A. A., Adams, R. B. Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2005a). Why do fear and anger look the way they do? Form and social function in facial expressions. Personality & Social Psychological Bulletin, 31(1), 7386. doi:10.1177/0146167204271306CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, A. A., & Ambady, N. (2007). The influence of the fear facial expression on prosocial responding. Cognition & Emotion, 21(2), 225247. doi:10.1080/02699930600652234CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, A. A., Ambady, N., & Kleck, R. E. (2005b). The effects of fear and anger facial expressions on approach- and avoidance-related behaviors. Emotion, 5(1), 119124. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.119CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, A. A., Stoycos, S. A., Brethel-Haurwitz, K. M., Robinson, P., & Cardinale, E. M. (2014). Neural and cognitive characteristics of extraordinary altruists. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(42), 1503615041. doi:10.1073/pnas.1408440111CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, J. S., Frith, C. D., Perrett, D. I., Rowland, D., Young, A. W., Calder, A. J. et al. (1996). A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions. Nature, 383(6603), 812815. doi:10.1038/383812a0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olsson, A., & Phelps, E. A. (2007). Social learning of fear. Nature Neuroscience, 10(9), 10951102. doi:10.1038/nn1968CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, M. L., Young, A. W., Senior, C., Brammer, M., Andrew, C., Calder, A. J., … David, A. S. (1997). A specific neural substrate for perceiving facial expressions of disgust. Nature, 389(6650), 495498. doi:10.1038/39051CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Powers, A., & Casey, B. J. (2015). The adolescent brain and the emergence and peak of psychopathology. Journal of Infant, Child, & Adolescent Psychotherapy, 14(1), 315. doi:10.1038/39051Google Scholar
Rhoads, S. A., Cardinale, E. M., O'Connell, K., Palmer, A. L., VanMeter, J. W., & Marsh, A. A. (2020). Mapping neural activity patterns to contextualized fearful facial expressions onto callous-unemotional (CU) traits: Inter-subject representational similarity analysis reveals less variability among high-CU adolescents. Personality Neuroscience, 3, e12. doi:10.1017/pen.2020.13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schenkel, R. (1967). Submission: Its features in the wolf and dog. American Zoologist, 7, 319329. doi:10.1093/icb/7.2.319CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vlaeyen, J. M. R., Heesen, R., Kret, M. E., Clay, Z., Bionda, T., & Kim, Y. (2022). Bared-teeth displays in bonobos (Pan paniscus): An assessment of the power asymmetry hypothesis. American Journal of Primatology, 84, e23419. doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0522CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whalen, P. J., Shin, L. M., McInerney, S. C., Fischer, H., Wright, C. I., & Rauch, S. L. (2001). A functional MRI study of human amygdala responses to facial expressions of fear versus anger. Emotion, 1(1), 7083. doi:10.1037//1528-3542.1.1.70CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed