Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T16:58:16.809Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Closing the symbolic reference gap to support flexible reasoning about the passage of time

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 December 2019

Danielle DeNigris
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology & Counseling, Fairleigh Dickinson University, Madison, NJ07940. [email protected]://view2.fdu.edu/academics/becton-college/psychology-and-counseling/faculty/
Patricia J. Brooks
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, College of Staten Island & The Graduate Center, CUNY, Staten Island, NY10314. [email protected]/campus-directory/patricia-j-brooks

Abstract

This commentary relates Hoerl & McCormack's dual systems perspective to models of cognitive development emphasizing representational redescription and the role of culturally constructed tools, including language, in providing flexible formats for thinking. We describe developmental processes that enable children to construct a mental time line, situate themselves in time, and overcome the primacy of the here and now.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ames, L. B. (1946) The development of the sense of time in the young child. The Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic Psychology 68(1):97125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burny, E., Valcke, M. & Desoete, A. (2009) Towards an agenda for studying learning and instruction focusing on time-related competences in children. Educational Studies 35(5):481–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D. & Boroditsky, L. (2008) Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition 106(2):579–93. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, A. & Chalmers, D. (1998) The extended mind. Analysis 58(1):719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003) Hearing gesture: How our hands help us think. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goodnow, J. J. (1977) Children's drawing. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harner, L. (1975) Yesterday and tomorrow: Development of early understanding of the terms. Developmental Psychology 11:864865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudson, J. (2002) “Do you know what we're going to do this summer?” Mothers’ talk to preschool children about future events. Journal of Cognition and Development 3(1):4971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudson, J. A. (2006) The development of future time concepts through mother-child conversation. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 52(1):7095.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1979) Micro- and macrodevelopmental changes in language acquisition and other representational systems. Cognitive Science 3:91118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992) Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Levy, E. & Nelson, K. (1994) Words in discourse: A dialectical approach to the acquisition of meaning and use. Journal of Child Language 21:367–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, J. M. (2004) The foundations of mind: Origins of conceptual thought. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1985) Making sense: The acquisition of shared meaning. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1991) The matter of time: Interdependencies between language and thought in development. In: Perspectives on language and cognition: Interrelations in development, ed. Gelman, S. A. & Byrnes, J. P., pp. 278318. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1996) Language in cognitive development: The emergence of the mediated mind. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. & Fivush, R. (2004) The emergence of autobiographical memory: A social cultural developmental theory. Psychological Review 111(2):486511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, K. & Gruendel, J. (1981) Generalized event representations: Basic building blocks of cognitive development. In: Advances in developmental psychology, ed. Lamb, M. E. & Brown, A. L., pp. 131–58. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nelson, K., Skwerer, D. P., Goldman, S., Henseler, S., Presler, N. & Walkenfeld, F. F. (2003) Entering a community of minds: An experiential approach to “theory of mind.” Human Development 46(1):2446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Núñez, R. E. (2011) No innate number line in the human brain. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 42(4):651–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Núñez, R. E. (2017) Is there really an evolved capacity for number? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 21(6):409–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Núñez, R. & Cooperrider, K. (2013) The tangle of space and time in human cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17(5):220–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plunkett, K., Hu, J. F. & Cohen, L. B. (2008) Labels can override perceptual categories in early infancy. Cognition 106(2):665–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamariz, M. & Kirby, S. (2016) The cultural evolution of language. Current Opinion in Psychology 8:3743.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tenbrink, T. (2007) Space, time, and the use of language: An investigation of relationships. de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wagner, L. (2001) Aspectual influences on early tense comprehension. Journal of Child Language 28(3):661–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waxman, S. R. & Markow, D. B. (1995) Words as invitations to form categories: Evidence from 12-to 13-month-old infants. Cognitive Psychology 29(3):257302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weist, R. M. (1989) Time concepts in language and thought: Filling the Piagetian void between two and five years. In: Time and human cognition: A life-span perspective. Advances in psychology, vol. 59, ed. Levin, T. & Zakay, D., pp. 63118. Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar