Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-04T19:46:32.632Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Considerations for the study of “incentive hope” and sign-tracking behaviors in humans

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 March 2019

Eva R. Pool
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Geneva, CH-1205 Geneva, Switzerland. [email protected]://www.unige.ch/fapse/e3lab/members1/senior-researchers-and-lecturers/dr-eva-r-pool/
David Sander
Affiliation:
Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, University of Geneva, CH-1202 Geneva, Switzerland. [email protected]://www.unige.ch/fapse/e3lab/director/

Abstract

The amplification of reward-seeking behavior under uncertainty described by Anselme & Güntürkün is based on the animal literature. However, this phenomenon could provide valuable information for the understanding of several dysfunctional human behaviors such as overeating and gambling. Therefore, we formulated some considerations on how the “incentive hope” hypothesis could be tested on a human population.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Balleine, B. W. & Killcross, S. (2006) Parallel incentive processing: An integrated view of amygdala function. Trends in Neurosciences 29(5):272–79. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.03.002.Google Scholar
Balleine, B. W. & O'Doherty, J. P. (2010) Human and rodent homologies in action control: Corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacology 35(1):4869. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.131.Google Scholar
Bucker, B. & Theeuwes, J. (2017) Stimulus-driven and goal-driven effects on Pavlovian associative reward learning. Visual Cognition 26(2):118. doi:10.1080/13506285.2017.1399948.Google Scholar
Delamater, A. R. (2012) On the nature of CS and US representations in Pavlovian learning. Learning & Behavior 40(1):123. doi:10.3758/s13420-011-0036-4.Google Scholar
Delamater, A. R. & Oakeshott, S. (2007) Learning about multiple attributes of reward in Pavlovian conditioning. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1104(1):120. doi:10.1196/annals.1390.008.Google Scholar
Dickinson, A. & Balleine, B. (1994) Motivational control of goal-directed action. Animal Learning & Behavior 22(1):118. doi:10.3758/Bf03199951.Google Scholar
Garofalo, S. & di Pellegrino, G. (2015) Individual differences in the influence of task-irrelevant Pavlovian cues on human behavior. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 9:163. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00163.Google Scholar
Joyner, M. A., Gearhardt, A. N. & Flagel, S. B. (2018) A translational model to assess sign-tracking and goal-tracking behavior in children. Neuropsychopharmacology 43(1):228–29. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.196.Google Scholar
Konorski, J. (1967) Integrative activity of the brain: An interdisciplinary approach. University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Linnet, J., Mouridsen, K., Peterson, E., Møller, A., Doudet, D. J. & Gjedde, A. (2012) Striatal dopamine release codes uncertainty in pathological gambling. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 204:5560. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.04.012.Google Scholar
Moors, A. (2017) The integrated theory of emotional behavior follows a radically goal-directed approach. Psychological Inquiry 28(1):6875. doi:10.1080/1047840X.2017.1275207.Google Scholar
O'Doherty, J. P., Cockburn, J. & Pauli, W. M. (2017) Learning, reward, and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology 68:73100. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044216.Google Scholar
Ostlund, S. B. & Balleine, B. W. (2008) The disunity of Pavlovian and instrumental values. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31(4):456–57. doi:10.1017/S0140525x08004925.Google Scholar
Pool, E., Brosch, T., Delplanque, S. & Sander, D. (2014) Where is the chocolate? Rapid spatial orienting toward stimuli associated with primary rewards. Cognition 130(3):348–59. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.12.002.Google Scholar
Sennwald, V., Pool, E. & Sander, D. (2017) Considering the influence of the Pavlovian system on behavior: Appraisal and value representation. Psychological Inquiry 28(1):5255. doi:10.1080/1047840X.2017.1259951.Google Scholar
Zhang, S., Mano, H., Ganesh, G., Robbins, T. & Seymour, B. (2016) Dissociable learning processes underlie human pain conditioning. Current Biology 26(1):5258. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2015.10.066.Google Scholar