Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T18:09:07.095Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Have we outgrown the reduced social motivation theory of autism?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 July 2019

Kristen Gillespie-Lynch*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, College of Staten Island, CUNY, Staten Island, NY 10314; The Graduate Center, CUNY, New York, NY 10016. [email protected]

Abstract

Although refreshing, Jaswal & Akhtar's critique of the reduced social motivation theory omits reference to Asperger's work and to changes in the diagnostic criteria over time. I situate the theory in the historical contexts that shaped – and eventually contradicted – it to highlight its dehumanizing aspects while emphasizing that critiques should be rooted in recognition of the diversity of the spectrum.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association. (1980) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd edition, DSM-III). American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (1994) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., DSM-IV). American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed., DSM-5). American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Asperger, H. (1944/1991) Die “Autistischen Psychopathen” im Kindesalter. Archiv für psychiatrie und nervenkrankheiten 117(1):76–136. Translated as “Autistic psychopathy” in childhood, In: Autism and Asperger syndrome, trans. U. Frith, pp. 37–92. Cambridge University Press. (Original work published 1944; translation published 1991.)Google Scholar
Bleuler, E. ([1950]1911) Dementia praecox or the group of schizophrenias. International Universities.Google Scholar
Bottini, S. (2018) Social reward processing in individuals with autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review of the social motivation hypothesis. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 45:926. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2017.10.001.Google Scholar
Clements, C. C., Zoltowski, A. R., Yankowitz, L. D., Yerys, B. E., Schultz, R. T. & Herrington, J. D. (2018) Evaluation of the social motivation hypothesis of autism: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 75(8):797808. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.1100.Google Scholar
Czech, H. (2018) Hans Asperger, National Socialism, and “race hygiene” in Nazi-era Vienna. Molecular Autism 9(1):29. DOI: 10.1186/s13229-018-0208-6.Google Scholar
Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A. & Liaw, J. (2004) Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology 40(2):271.Google Scholar
Donvan, J. & Zucker, C. (2010) Autism's first child. Atlantic Monthly 7:7890. Available at: https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/2010/10/autisms-first-child/308227/.Google Scholar
Elsabbagh, M., Gliga, T., Pickles, A., Hudry, K., Charman, T., Johnson, M. H. & BASIS Team. (2013) The development of face orienting mechanisms in infants at-risk for autism. Behavioural Brain Research 251:147–54.Google Scholar
Fletcher-Watson, S., Adams, J., Brook, K., Charman, T., Crane, L., Cusack, J., Leekam, S., Milton, D., Parr, J. R. & Pellicano, E. (2018) Making the future together: Shaping autism research through meaningful participation. Autism. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361318786721.Google Scholar
Jones, W. & Klin, A. (2013) Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2–6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature 504(7480):427–31.Google Scholar
Kanner, L. (1943) Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child 2(3):217–50. Available at: https://simonsfoundation.s3.amazonaws.com/share/071207-leo-kanner-autistic-affective-contact.pdf.Google Scholar
Prizant, B. M. (2015) Uniquely human: A different way of seeing autism. Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Rimland, B. (1964) Infantile autism: The syndrome and its implications for a neural theory of behavior. Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Robison, J. E. (2017) Kanner, Asperger, and Frankl: A third man at the genesis of the autism diagnosis. Autism 21(7):862–71.Google Scholar
Rumsey, J. M., Rapoport, J. L. & Sceery, W. R. (1985) Autistic children as adults: Psychiatric, social, and behavioral outcomes. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry 24(4):465–73.Google Scholar
Schopler, E. & Reichler, R. J. (1968) Psycholobiological referents for the treatment of autism. Paper presented at Indiana University Colloquium on Infantile Autism, Indiana University Medical Center, Indianapolis, IN.Google Scholar
Sheffer, E. (2018) Asperger's children: The origins of autism in Nazi Vienna. Norton.Google Scholar
Silberman, S. (2015) Neurotribes: The legacy of autism and the future of neurodiversity. Penguin.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T. & Moll, H. (2005) In search of the uniquely human. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28(5):721–27.Google Scholar
Wing, L. (1981) Asperger's syndrome: A clinical account. Psychological Medicine 11(1):115–29.Google Scholar
Wing, L. & Gould, J. (1979) Severe impairments of social interaction and associated abnormalities in children: Epidemiology and classification. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 9(1):1129. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01531288.Google Scholar
Young, G. S., Merin, N., Rogers, S. J. & Ozonoff, S. (2009) Gaze behavior and affect at 6 months: Predicting clinical outcomes and language development in typically developing infants and infants at risk for autism. Developmental Science 12(5):798814.Google Scholar