Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T03:23:30.214Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Pragmatics and social motivation in autism

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 July 2019

Mikhail Kissine*
Affiliation:
ACTE at LaDisco and ULB Neuroscience Institute, Université libre de Bruxelles, 1050 Brussels, Belgium. [email protected]

Abstract

Pragmatic deficits constitute a central feature of autism, which is highly relevant to Jaswal & Akhtar's criticisms of the social motivation theory of autism. Recent research reveals that while certain context-based interpretations are accessible, more complex pragmatic phenomena remain challenging for people on the spectrum. Such a selective pragmatic impairment is difficult to account for in motivational terms.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed., DSM-5). American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Chevallier, C., Noveck, I., Happé, F. G. E. & Wilson, D. (2011) What's in a voice? Prosody as a test case for the theory of mind account of autism. Neuropsychologia 49(3):507–17.Google Scholar
Chevallier, C., Wilson, D., Happé, F. G. E. & Noveck, I. (2010) Scalar inferences in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 40:1104–17.Google Scholar
Deliens, G., Antoniou, K., Clin, E., Ostashchenko, E. & Kissine, M. (2018a) Context, facial expression and prosody in irony processing. Journal of Memory and Language 99(Suppl. C):3548. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2017.10.001Google Scholar
Deliens, G., Papastamou, F., Ruytenbeek, N., Geelhand, P. & Kissine, M. (2018b) Selective pragmatic impairment in autism spectrum disorder: Indirect requests versus irony. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 48(9):2938–52. doi:10.1007/s10803-018-3561-6Google Scholar
Hermann, I., Haser, V., Van Elst, L. T., Ebert, D., Müller-Feldmeth, D., Riedel, A. & Konieczny, L. (2013) Automatic metaphor processing in adults with Asperger syndrome: A metaphor interference effect task. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 263:S17787. doi:10.1007/s00406-013-0453-9.Google Scholar
Kissine, M., Cano-Chervel, J., Carlier, S., De Brabanter, P., Ducenne, L., Pairon, M.-C., Delvenne, V. & Leybaert, J. (2015) Children with autism understand indirect speech acts: Evidence from a semi-structured act-out task. PLOS ONE 10:e0142191. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0142191.Google Scholar
Kissine, M., De Brabanter, P. & Leybaert, J. (2012) The interpretation of requests in children with autism: The effect of the sentence-type. Autism (16):523–32.Google Scholar
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S. & Gotham, K. (2012) Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule – Second Edition (ADOS-2). Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Norbury, C. F. (2005) The relationship between theory of mind and metaphor: Evidence from children with language impairement and autistic spectrum disorder. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 23:383–99.Google Scholar
Pijnacker, J., Hagoort, P., Buitelaar, J., Teunisse, J.-P. & Geurts, B. (2009) Pragmatic inferences in high-functionning adults with autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 39:607–18.Google Scholar
van Tiel, B. & Kissine, M. (2018) Quantity-based reasoning in the broader autism phenotype: A web-based study. Applied Psycholinguistics 39(6):1373–403.Google Scholar