Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T02:14:48.590Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mothers’ self-reported grocery shopping behaviours with their 2- to 7-year-old children: relationship between feeding practices and mothers’ willingness to purchase child-requested nutrient-poor, marketed foods, and fruits and vegetables

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 September 2017

Kathryn Lively
Affiliation:
Department of Nutritional Sciences, University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center, Oklahoma City, OK, USA
Oluborode Babawale
Affiliation:
Department of Biostatistics and Epidemiology, University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center, Oklahoma City, OK, USA
David M Thompson
Affiliation:
Department of Biostatistics and Epidemiology, University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center, Oklahoma City, OK, USA
Amanda S Morris
Affiliation:
Human Development and Family Science, Oklahoma State University, Tulsa, OK, USA
Jennifer L Harris
Affiliation:
Rudd Center for Food Policy and Obesity, Hartford, CT, USA
Susan B Sisson
Affiliation:
Department of Nutritional Sciences, University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center, Oklahoma City, OK, USA
Marshall K Cheney
Affiliation:
Department of Health and Exercise Science, Norman, OK, USA
Karina R Lora*
Affiliation:
Center for Public Health and Health Policy, University of Connecticut Health, 195 Farmington Avenue, Suite 2100, Farmington, CT 06032, USA
*
*Corresponding author: Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

To assess relationships between mothers’ feeding practices (food as a reward, food for emotion regulation, modelling of healthy eating) and mothers’ willingness to purchase child-marketed foods and fruits/vegetables (F&V) requested by their children during grocery co-shopping.

Design

Cross-sectional. Mothers completed an online survey that included questions about feeding practices and willingness (i.e. intentions) to purchase child-requested foods during grocery co-shopping. Feeding practices scores were dichotomized at the median. Foods were grouped as nutrient-poor or nutrient-dense (F&V) based on national nutrition guidelines. Regression models compared mothers with above-the-median v. at-or-below-the-median feeding practices scores on their willingness to purchase child-requested food groupings, adjusting for demographic covariates.

Setting

Participants completed an online survey generated at a public university in the USA.

Subjects

Mothers (n 318) of 2- to 7-year-old children.

Results

Mothers who scored above-the-median on using food as a reward were more willing to purchase nutrient-poor foods (β=0·60, P<0·0001), mothers who scored above-the-median on use of food for emotion regulation were more willing to purchase nutrient-poor foods (β=0·29, P<0·0031) and mothers who scored above-the-median on modelling of healthy eating were more willing to purchase nutrient-dense foods (β=0·22, P<0·001) than were mothers with at-or-below-the-median scores, adjusting for demographic covariates.

Conclusions

Mothers who reported using food to control children’s behaviour were more willing to purchase child-requested, nutrient-poor foods. Parental feeding practices may facilitate or limit children’s foods requested in grocery stores. Parent–child food consumer behaviours should be investigated as a route that may contribute to children’s eating patterns.

Type
Short Communication
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2017 

Parenting behaviours that respond to children’s food requests at the grocery store may provide important information for the development of nutrition programmes to promote children’s healthy eating habits. Child participation in family food shopping facilitates their food consumer socialization experiences and introduces them to the retail food environment( Reference Gram 1 , Reference Marshall 2 ). Parents, peers and the media are the main consumer socialization agents of children( Reference Ward 3 ). Children aged 2–7 years make more food purchase requests than do older children( Reference Calderon, Ayala and Elder 4 , Reference Buijzen and Valkenburg 5 ). This characteristic, coupled with the amount of time children spend with their caregivers in grocery stores( Reference Sandberg and Hofferth 6 , Reference Hofferth 7 ), creates an opportunity for caregivers to model healthy food selection( Reference O’Dougherty, Story and Stang 8 ).

Pester power, ‘a child’s attempt to exert control over a purchase situation as a simple battle of wills (between children and parents)’( Reference Nicholls and Cullen 9 ), is one of the most successful strategies children use to influence parent purchases( Reference Nicholls and Cullen 9 ). Usually, children request unhealthy, highly marketed foods (i.e. prepared meals, sweets), not healthy options, such as fruits and vegetables (F&V)( Reference Nørgaard, Bruns and Christensen 10 ). Marketing nutrient-poor foods to children influences obesity-related dietary behaviours( 11 , Reference Sonntag, Schneider and Mdege 12 ).

Parental feeding practices influence children’s health outcomes (i.e. food intake, weight)( Reference Wardle, Carnell and Cooke 13 Reference Tschann, Martinez and Penilla 17 ). Use of food as a reward( Reference Anez, Remington and Wardle 18 ) and food for emotion regulation( Reference Wardle, Sanderson and Guthrie 19 ) may cause children to rely on external cues, hinder their ability to self-regulate food intake( Reference Fisher and Birch 20 ), and have been inconsistently associated with child weight( Reference Rodgers, Paxton and Massey 21 , Reference Carnell and Wardle 22 ). Food to control behaviour is commonly used with children of various ages and in different settings (i.e. home, school)( Reference De Decker, Sioen and Verbeken 23 Reference Lu, Tao and Hou 27 ). Modelling of healthy eating, in which parents purposely demonstrate eating behaviours and food choices to encourage similar behaviours( Reference Vaughn, Ward and Fisher 28 ), is positively associated with children’s intake of healthy foods( Reference Tibbs, Haire-Joshu and Schechtman 29 , Reference Campbell, Crawford and Ball 30 ). Except for two studies that reported associations of parental dimensions of feeding styles with children’s influence on beverage purchases( Reference Lora, Hubbs-Tait and Guzman 31 ) or parenting styles in relation to the healthfulness of parent–child food choices in grocery stores( Reference Lucas-Thompson, Graham and Ullrich 32 ), most research into parental food socialization behaviours has been conducted in the home.

Examining parental feeding practices that may be associated with parents’ purchases of highly marketed, nutrient-poor foods or low-marketed, nutrient-dense foods that children request when they co-shop together may be a route to explore understudied factors that influence children’s eating behaviours. The present study’s purpose was to assess the relationships of mothers’ use of food for emotion regulation, food as a reward and modelling of healthy eating with mothers’ self-reported willingness (i.e. intentions) to purchase nutrient-poor marketed foods and nutrient-dense foods (F&V) requested by their 2- to- 7-year-old children during grocery co-shopping. We assessed differences between mothers with above-the-median v. at-or-below-the-median scores of self-reported feeding practices regarding frequency of willingness to purchase child-requested foods.

Methods

Study design and setting

Data for the current cross-sectional study were collected through an online survey administered using Qualtrics™. Participants were mothers of 2- to 7-year-old children (n 318). The University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center Institutional Review Board approved the protocol.

Procedure and measures

The online survey included questions about mother and child demographics, family characteristics, feeding practices and mothers’ willingness to purchase child-requested foods during grocery co-shopping. Willingness is ‘an individual’s openness to opportunity, that is, his or her willingness to perform a certain behaviour in situations that are conducive to the behaviour’( Reference Pomery, Gibbons and Reis-Bergan 33 ). Willingness is an alternative form of behavioural intentions( Reference Ajzen and Fishbein 34 , Reference Fishbein 35 ), tends to be highly correlated with intentions( Reference Pomery, Gibbons and Reis-Bergan 33 , Reference Gerrard, Gibbons and Houlihan 36 ) and has effectively predicted health behaviours( Reference Gibbons, Gerrard and Lane 37 , Reference Gerrard, Gibbons and Brody 38 ). Additionally, participants reported the frequency their child (i) went grocery shopping and (ii) made a food request during a grocery shopping trip in the past 30 d.

Mothers’ feeding practices

Emotion regulation, food as a reward and modelling of healthy eating were assessed using the Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire( Reference Musher-Eizenman and Holub 39 ). Response options were structured in a 5-point Likert-type scale, as follows: food for emotion regulation from ‘never’ to ‘always’; and food as a reward and modelling from ‘disagree’ to ‘agree’. Composite scores for each of the three feeding practices were computed by averaging their item values.

Frequency of mothers’ willingness to purchase child-requested foods

Mothers responded to the hypothetical scenario: ‘If your 2- to 7-year-old child would ask you to buy these foods when grocery shopping together, how often do you buy them?’ Response options to the question were structured on a 5-point Likert scale with word anchors from ‘never’ (=1) to ‘always’ (=5). Mothers provided responses to a list of twenty-four items. Pictorial examples for each food item were provided. Foods were then categorized into nutrient-dense (e.g. F&V) and nutrient-poor (e.g. carbonated beverages) based on nutrient information provided by the manufacturer’s Nutrition Facts label (see online supplementary material, Supplemental Table 1).

Table 1 Characteristics of the sample of mothers, and their 2- to 7-year-old children, who completed an online survey generated at a public university in the USA (n 318)

* Information not reported for one participant.

Information not reported for two participants.

Data analyses

For each of the three feeding practices (use of food as a reward, use of food for emotion regulation, modelling of healthy eating), participants were separately categorized according to that practice’s median composite score. Groups (those with scores above-the-median and those with scores at-or-below-the-median) were compared on the frequency with which they described their willingness to purchase food items requested by a child. Between-group comparisons were assessed first using non-parametric permutation tests and then in multivariable regression models. Multivariable models adjusted for potential confounders (mother’s age, employment status, education, child’s age, household income, frequency of family dinners) that were identified using Kruskal–Wallis or Pearson’s correlation tests. Coefficients from the final regression models, which included only covariates that were significantly associated with feeding practices (P<0·05), represent adjusted between-group differences in the mean frequency of willingness to purchase a particular food. All analyses were performed using the statistical software package SAS version 9.4.

Detailed information on the methodology is presented in the online supplementary material, Supplemental File 1.

Results

Table 1 displays demographic characteristics and frequency of children’s grocery shopping with mother and food requests during grocery shopping. Overall means (SD) of reported feeding practices were: food as a reward, 2·80 (1·18); food for emotion regulation, 1·65 (0·57); and modelling of healthy eating, 4·27 (0·71). Unadjusted comparisons of mothers’ willingness to purchase nutrient-dense and nutrient-poor foods above-the-median and at-or-below-the-median use of the three feeding practices are provided in the online supplementary material, Supplemental Table 2.

Table 2 Associations of mothers’ feeding practices with frequency of mothers’ willingness to purchase foods requested by the child during grocery store trips among of the sample of mothers of 2- to 7-year-old children (n 318)

* Includes fresh fruit and fresh/frozen vegetables.

Includes carbonated beverages, baked goods, juice drinks and non-carbonated beverages, high-sugar breakfast cereals, snack foods, prepared foods and meals, and high-sugar, high-fat dairy products.

A positive between-group difference estimate denotes a higher frequency of food purchase among mothers with high feeding practice scores (above-the-median) than among mothers with low scores (at-or-below-the-median).

§ Models were individually adjusted for variables determined to be potential cofounders. Depending on the model, these variables included child’s age, frequency of family dinners, household income, employment status and/or highest education completed.

In the adjusted analysis (Table 2), mothers with above-the-median use of food as a reward were less willing to purchase child-requested, nutrient-dense foods (β (mean between-group difference)=−0·20, P=0·005) than mothers with scores at-or-below-the-median for use of food as a reward, but more willing to purchase child-requested, nutrient-poor foods (β=0·60, P<0·0001). Further, mothers with above-the-median use of food for emotion regulation were more willing to purchase nutrient-poor foods requested by the child (β=0·29, P=0·0031) than mothers scoring at-or-below-the-median use for emotion regulation. Mothers with above-the-median modelling of healthy eating scores were more willing to purchase child-requested, nutrient-dense foods (β=0·22, P=0·001) than mothers scoring at-or-below-the-median for modelling, but less willing to purchase child-requested, nutrient-poor foods (β=−0·36, P<0·0001).

Discussion

Mothers scoring above-the-median on use food as a reward and food for emotion regulation were more willing to purchase child-requested, nutrient-poor, marketed foods than mothers with at-or-below-the-median scores for these feeding behaviours. Above-the-median use of modelling healthy eating habits was positively associated with mothers’ willingness to purchase nutrient-dense foods and negatively associated with willingness to purchase nutrient-poor foods in response to children’s requests.

The grocery store is considered a stressful shopping environment for a parent and a child( Reference Gelperowic and Beharrell 40 ). While parents may seek to shop quickly and efficiently, children may focus on taking part in the shopping activity by trying to influence food purchases( Reference Nicholls and Cullen 9 ). The instant gratification that characterizes young children’s behaviours makes it difficult for them to resist a stimulus; if their requests are ignored or denied, they may respond by whining, screaming or crying( Reference Valkenburg and Cantor 41 ). Although mothers did not report on strategies their children employed to request food purchases, nagging or ‘just asking’ strategies, especially for nutrient-dense, marketed foods, have been reported in children of similar ages( Reference Calderon, Ayala and Elder 4 , Reference O’Dougherty, Story and Stang 8 , Reference Nørgaard, Bruns and Christensen 10 ).

In the present study, children may have been more successful at influencing purchases of the foods that were used to control their behaviour. Mothers scoring above-the-median on use of food as a reward and food for emotion regulation were more willing to purchase child-requested, nutrient-poor marketed foods than mothers with at-or-below-the-median scores. Parenting traits exhibited by mothers at home may also occur when making food purchase decisions. Maternal use of food to control children’s behaviour has previously been associated with the development of obesogenic eating behaviour, such as overeating and eating in the absence of hunger, in children( Reference Rodgers, Paxton and Massey 21 ). To date, parents’ use of these feeding practices has been mostly examined at home. The present study makes a contribution by reporting parenting food behaviours occurring in environments where food selection for the home takes place. Conversely, scores above-the-median in modelling of healthy eating habits were positively associated with mothers’ willingness to purchase child-requested nutrient-dense foods. Parents’ modelling of healthy eating has been positively associated with children’s F&V consumption( Reference Fisher, Mitchell and Smiciklas-Wright 14 , Reference Tibbs, Haire-Joshu and Schechtman 29 ), and inversely associated with children’s fatty snack intake( Reference Eisenberg, Ayala and Crespo 42 ), dietary fat intake and lower weight( Reference Matheson, Robinson and Varady 43 ). Mothers who intentionally model healthy dietary behaviours for their children may view the grocery store as an opportunity to talk about healthy foods and model purchasing of nutrient-dense foods (e.g. F&V) more frequently and nutrient-poor foods less frequently.

The role of children as influencers of parental food purchase decisions has gained attention( Reference Wingert, Zachary and Fox 44 ). Helping parents better understand how their children influence what they purchase and then how those purchases may be used to reward children could assist families in modifying unhealthy family feeding practices and ultimately prevent child obesity. Although the study sample was mostly high socio-economic status, findings highlight potential areas for investigation of parent–child co-shopping behaviours that may also occur among low-income families. This could increase our understanding of how the economic limitations these families face may affect caregivers’ willingness to purchase foods their children request.

Study strengths include the assessment of reported maternal feeding practices at the grocery store when co-shopping with their children and the large sample size used for analyses. Limitations of the study include the potential for social desirability bias in responding to the survey questions. One of the recruitment strategies included advertising the study on the Department of Nutritional Sciences’ Facebook page. Thus, some participants might have been more motivated to provide nutrition information than others. The sample was mostly White, high socio-economic status; maternal feeding behaviours differ by income level of the family( Reference Gross, Mendelsohn and Fierman 45 , Reference Baughcum, Powers and Johnson 46 ). Findings cannot be generalized to other populations. Mothers’ choices of which child to report on may have influenced their responses. Frozen fruit was not included in the food categories. Participation was not restricted to living in Oklahoma; other geographical locations in which mothers resided might have influenced mother–child purchase behaviours. Mothers’ feeding practice scores for use of food for emotion regulation and modelling did not vary much. This homogeneity prevented a more definite picture of the outcomes of the child purchase requests with mothers with high or low scores of these practices. Actual behaviours were not assessed. Instead, mothers were asked to report their willingness to perform a behaviour in response to a hypothetical scenario. However, the question format is in agreement with how willingness behaviours are assessed in that participants were given a description of a hypothetical situation to which they willingly responded( Reference Gerrard, Gibbons and Houlihan 36 ). Willingness has effectively predicted various health behaviours( Reference Gibbons, Gerrard and Lane 37 , Reference Gerrard, Gibbons and Brody 38 ).

Conclusion

In conclusion, mothers’ use of food to control children’s behaviours was positively associated with mothers’ willingness to purchase child-requested, nutrient-poor foods. Mothers with above-the-median modelling of healthy eating scores were more willing to purchase child-requested nutrient-dense foods than were mothers with at-or-below-the-median scores. Parents’ food socialization practices related to children’s influence over food purchases should be investigated in grocery stores to elucidate factors that may contribute to children’s unhealthy eating behaviours. The role of young children in purchase outcomes opens new possibilities for health promotion. Allowing children to actively participate in healthy food (i.e. F&V) selection while engaging with their parents in healthy food purchase modelling( Reference O’Dougherty, Story and Stang 8 ) may influence availability of foods in the home and, thus, consumption.

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements: The authors thank the participants of the study and Kathy Kyler for editing the manuscript. Financial support: This work was supported by the Department of Nutritional Sciences at the University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center and the National Institute of General Medical Sciences of the National Institutes of Health (under award number U54GM104938). The funders had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article. Conflicts of interest: The authors declare no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship and/or publication of this article. Authorship: K.L. was responsible for management of data collection, literature review and manuscript writing. O.B. was responsible for statistical analysis and interpretation. D.M.T. contributed to overseeing statistical analysis and provided statistical advice. A.S.M., J.L.H., S.B.S. and M.K.C. contributed to study design and critically revised the manuscript. K.R.L. conceptualized the research, led the study design, critically revised the manuscript and has final responsibility for the study. Ethics of human subject participation: This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures involving human subjects/patients were approved by the University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center Institutional Review Board. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980017002142

References

1. Gram, M (2010) Self‐reporting vs. observation: some cautionary examples from parent/child food shopping behaviour. Int J Consum Stud 34, 394399.Google Scholar
2. Marshall, D (2014) Co-operation in the supermarket aisle: young children’s accounts of family food shopping. Int J Retail Distrib Manage 42, 9901003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
3. Ward, S (1974) Consumer socialization. J Consum Res 1, 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
4. Calderon, J, Ayala, GX, Elder, JP et al. (2017) What happens when parents and children go grocery shopping? An observational study of Latino Dyads in Southern California, USA. Health Educ Behav 44, 512.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5. Buijzen, M & Valkenburg, PM (2008) Observing purchase‐related parent–child communication in retail environments: a developmental and socialization perspective. Hum Commun Res 34, 5069.Google Scholar
6. Sandberg, JF & Hofferth, SL (2001) Changes in children’s time with parents: United States, 1981–1997. Demography 38, 423436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7. Hofferth, SL (2009) Changes in American children’s time – 1997 to 2003. Electron Int J Time Use Res 6, 26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8. O’Dougherty, M, Story, M & Stang, J (2006) Observations of parent–child co-shoppers in supermarkets: children’s involvement in food selections, parental yielding, and refusal strategies. J Nutr Educ Behav 38, 183188.Google Scholar
9. Nicholls, AJ & Cullen, P (2004) The child–parent purchase relationship: ‘pester power’, human rights and retail ethics. J Retail Consum Serv 11, 7586.Google Scholar
10. Nørgaard, K, Bruns, K, Christensen, H et al. (2007) Children’s influence on and participation in the family decision process during food buying. Young Consum 8, 197216.Google Scholar
11. Institute of Medicine (2005) Food Marketing to Children and Youth: Threat or Opportunity?. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.Google Scholar
12. Sonntag, D, Schneider, S, Mdege, N et al. (2015) Beyond food promotion: a systematic review on the influence of the food industry on obesity-related dietary behaviour among children. Nutrients 7, 85658576.Google Scholar
13. Wardle, J, Carnell, S & Cooke, L (2005) Parental control over feeding and children’s fruit and vegetable intake: how are they related? J Am Diet Assoc 105, 227232.Google Scholar
14. Fisher, JO, Mitchell, DC, Smiciklas-Wright, H et al. (2002) Parental influences on young girls’ fruit and vegetable, micronutrient, and fat intakes. J Am Diet Assoc 102, 5864.Google Scholar
15. Bante, H, Elliott, M, Harrod, A et al. (2008) The use of inappropriate feeding practices by rural parents and their effect on preschoolers’ fruit and vegetable preferences and intake. J Nutr Educ Behav 40, 2833.Google Scholar
16. Galloway, A, Fiorito, L, Francis, L et al. (2006) ‘Finish your soup’: counterproductive effects of pressuring children to eat on intake and affect. Appetite 46, 318323.Google Scholar
17. Tschann, JM, Martinez, SM, Penilla, C et al. (2015) Parental feeding practices and child weight status in Mexican American families: a longitudinal analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 12, 66.Google Scholar
18. Anez, E, Remington, A, Wardle, J et al. (2013) The impact of instrumental feeding on children’s responses to taste exposure. J Hum Nutr Diet 26, 415420.Google Scholar
19. Wardle, J, Sanderson, S, Guthrie, CA et al. (2002) Parental feeding style and the inter-generational transmission of obesity risk. Obes Res 10, 453462.Google Scholar
20. Fisher, JO & Birch, LL (1999) Restricting access to foods and children’s eating. Appetite 32, 405419.Google Scholar
21. Rodgers, RF, Paxton, SJ, Massey, R et al. (2013) Maternal feeding practices predict weight gain and obesogenic eating behaviors in young children: a prospective study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 10, 24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22. Carnell, S & Wardle, J (2007) Associations between multiple measures of parental feeding and children’s adiposity in United Kingdom preschoolers. Obesity (Silver Spring) 15, 137144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23. De Decker, A, Sioen, I, Verbeken, S et al. (2016) Associations of reward sensitivity with food consumption, activity pattern, and BMI in children. Appetite 100, 189196.Google Scholar
24. van den Berg, L, Pieterse, K, Malik, JA et al. (2011) Association between impulsivity, reward responsiveness and body mass index in children. Int J Obes (Lond) 35, 13011307.Google Scholar
25. Fedewa, AL & Davis, MC (2015) How food as a reward is detrimental to children’s health, learning, and behavior. J Sch Health 85, 648658.Google Scholar
26. Lora, KR, Hubbs-Tait, L, Ferris, AM et al. (2016) African-American and Hispanic children’s beverage intake: differences in associations with desire to drink, fathers’ feeding practices, and weight concerns. Appetite 107, 558567.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27. Lu, Q, Tao, F, Hou, F et al. (2016) Emotion regulation, emotional eating and the energy-rich dietary pattern. A population-based study in Chinese adolescents. Appetite 99, 149156.Google Scholar
28. Vaughn, AE, Ward, DS, Fisher, JO et al. (2016) Fundamental constructs in food parenting practices: a content map to guide future research. Nutr Rev 74, 98117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29. Tibbs, T, Haire-Joshu, D, Schechtman, KB et al. (2001) The relationship between parental modeling, eating patterns, and dietary intake among African-American parents. J Am Diet Assoc 101, 535541.Google Scholar
30. Campbell, KJ, Crawford, DA & Ball, K (2006) Family food environment and dietary behaviors likely to promote fatness in 5–6 year-old children. Int J Obes (Lond) 30, 12721280.Google Scholar
31. Lora, KR, Hubbs-Tait, L, Guzman, M et al. (2016) Preschoolers’ influence on and help with beverage selection at the grocery store is linked to maternal responsiveness and child beverage intake: an exploratory study. Eat Behav 23, 1923.Google Scholar
32. Lucas-Thompson, RG, Graham, DJ, Ullrich, E et al. (2017) General and food-selection specific parenting style in relation to the healthfulness of parent–child choices while grocery shopping. Appetite 108, 353360.Google Scholar
33. Pomery, EA, Gibbons, FX, Reis-Bergan, M et al. (2009) From willingness to intention: experience moderates the shift from reactive to reasoned behavior. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 35, 894908.Google Scholar
34. Ajzen, IF & Fishbein, M (2005) The influence of attitudes on behavior. In The Handbook of Attitudes, pp. 173221 [D Albarracin, B Johnson and M Zanna, editors]. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
35. Fishbein, M (2008) A reasoned action approach to health promotion. Med Decis Making 28, 834844.Google Scholar
36. Gerrard, M, Gibbons, FX, Houlihan, AE et al. (2008) A dual-process approach to health risk decision making: the prototype willingness model. Dev Rev 28, 2961.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
37. Gibbons, FX, Gerrard, M & Lane, DJ (2003) A social reaction model of adolescent health risk. In Social Psychological Foundations of Health and Illness, pp. 107136 [JM Suls and K Wallston, editors]. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
38. Gerrard, M, Gibbons, FX, Brody, GH et al. (2006) A theory-based dual-focus alcohol intervention for preadolescents: the Strong African American Families Program. Psychol Addict Behav 20, 185195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
39. Musher-Eizenman, D & Holub, S (2007) Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire: validation of a new measure of parental feeding practices. J Pediatr Psychol 32, 960972.Google Scholar
40. Gelperowic, R & Beharrell, B (1994) Healthy food products for children: packaging and mothers’ purchase decisions. Br Food J 96, 48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
41. Valkenburg, PM & Cantor, J (2001) The development of a child into a consumer. J Appl Dev Psychol 22, 6172.Google Scholar
42. Eisenberg, CM, Ayala, GX, Crespo, NC et al. (2012) Examining multiple parenting behaviors on young children’s dietary fat consumption. J Nutr Educ Behav 44, 302309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
43. Matheson, D, Robinson, T, Varady, A et al. (2006) Do Mexican-American mothers’ food-related parenting practices influence their children’s weight and dietary intake? J Am Diet Assoc 106, 18611865.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
44. Wingert, K, Zachary, DA, Fox, M et al. (2014) Child as change agent. The potential of children to increase healthy food purchasing. Appetite 81, 330336.Google Scholar
45. Gross, RS, Mendelsohn, AL, Fierman, AH et al. (2014) Maternal infant feeding behaviors and disparities in early child obesity. Child Obes 10, 145152.Google Scholar
46. Baughcum, AE, Powers, SW, Johnson, SB et al. (2001) Maternal feeding practices and beliefs and their relationships to overweight in early childhood. J Dev Behav Pediatr 22, 391408.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Characteristics of the sample of mothers, and their 2- to 7-year-old children, who completed an online survey generated at a public university in the USA (n 318)

Figure 1

Table 2 Associations of mothers’ feeding practices with frequency of mothers’ willingness to purchase foods requested by the child during grocery store trips among of the sample of mothers of 2- to 7-year-old children (n 318)

Supplementary material: File

Lively et al supplementary material

Lively et al supplementary material 1

Download Lively et al supplementary material(File)
File 33.2 KB
Supplementary material: File

Lively et al supplementary material

Tables S1-S2

Download Lively et al supplementary material(File)
File 41.5 KB