Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T14:14:55.709Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

How does consciousness for action relate to attention for action?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 November 2016

Elizabeth A. Franz*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin 9016, New Zealand. [email protected]://www.otago.ac.nz/psychology/research/otago028082.html

Abstract

The interesting target article by Morsella et al. addresses critical issues that impact our understanding of consciousness. It is surprising, however, to see no treatment of the relationship between attention and consciousness, particularly given available models. Whether olfaction is most suitable as a model system to study consciousness for action also seems questionable. These issues are elaborated in the present commentary.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bednark, J. G. & Franz, E. A. (2014) Agency attribution: Event-related potentials and outcome monitoring. Experimental Brain Research 232:1117–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bednark, J. G., Reynolds, J. N. J, Stafford, T., Redgrave, P. & Franz, E. A. (2013) Creating a movement heuristic for voluntary action: Electrophysiological correlates of movement-outcome learning. Cortex 49(3):771–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franz, E. A. (1997) Spatial coupling in the coordination of complex actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A 50A:684704.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franz, E. A. (2003) Bimanual action representation: A window to human evolution. In: Taking action: Cognitive neuroscience perspectives on intentional acts, ed. Johnston-Frey, S. H., pp. 259–88. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franz, E. A. (2006) Converging evidence of the basal ganglia's role in focused action selection and inhibition of competing information. In: Recent breakthroughs in basal ganglia research, ed. Bezard, E., Ch. 18, pp. 227–39. Nova Science.Google Scholar
Franz, E. A. (2010) A framework for conceptual binding of bimanual actions: Possible applications to neurology and neuro-rehabilitative therapies. Current Trends in Neurology 4:122.Google Scholar
Franz, E. A. (2012) The allocation of attention to learning of goal-directed actions: A cognitive neuroscience framework focusing on the basal ganglia. Frontiers in Psychology 3, article 535:115. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00535.Google Scholar
Franz, E. A., Eliassen, J., Ivry, R. B. & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1996) Dissociation of spatial and temporal coupling in the bimanual movements of callosotomy patients. Psychological Science 7:306–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franz, E. A. & Gillett, G. (2011) John Hughlings Jackson's evolutionary neurology: A unifying framework for cognitive neuroscience. Brain 134(10):3114–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franz, E. A. & McCormick, R. (2010) Conceptual unifying constraints on anticipatory bimanual control. Experimental Brain Research 205(2):273–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franz, E. A. & Ramachandran, V. S. (1998) Bimanual coupling in amputees with phantom limbs. Nature Neuroscience 1:443–44. doi:10.1038/2161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franz, E. A., Waldie, K. & Smith, M. (2000) The effect of callosotomy on novel versus familiar bimanual actions: A neural dissociation between controlled and automatic processes? Psychological Science 11(1):8285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franz, E. A., Zelaznik, H. N. & McCabe, G. (1991) Spatial topological constraints in a bimanual task. Acta Psychologica 77(2):137–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franz, E. A., Zelaznik, H. N., Swinnen, S. & Walter, C. (2001) Spatial conceptual influences on the coordination of bimanual actions: When a dual task becomes a single task. Journal of Motor Behavior 33(1):103–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillett, G. & Franz, E. A. (2013) John Hughlings Jackson: Bridging theory and clinical observation. The Lancet 381(9866):528–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottfried, J. A. & Dolan, R. J. (2003) The nose smells what the eye sees: Crossmodal visual facilitation of human olfactory perception. Neuron 39(2):375–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G. (1972) On doing two things at once: Time sharing as a function of ideomotor compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology 94(1):5257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grossberg, S. (1999) The link between brain learning, attention, and consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition 8(1):144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hommel, B. (2010) Grounding attention in action control: The intentional control of selection. In: Effortless attention: A new perspective in the cognitive science of attention and action, ed. Bruya, B. J., pp. 121–40. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, C. M. L. & Franz, E. A. (2007) Experience-dependent effects in unimanual and bimanual reaction time tasks in musicians. Journal of Motor Behavior 39(1):38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughlings Jackson, J. (1884) On affectations of speech from disease of the brain (Part 2). British Medical Journal 12:703707.Google Scholar
James, W. (1890/1918) The principles of psychology, vols. 1 and 11. Dover. (Original work published in 1890).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, A. (2011) Attention and olfactory consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology 2, article 380. (Online journal). doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelso, J. A. S., Southard, D. L. & Goodman, D. (1979) On the nature of human interlimb coordination. Science 203(4384):1029–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laing, D. G. (1983) Natural sniffing gives optimum odour perception for humans. Perception 12:99117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mainland, J. D. & Sobel, N. (2006) The sniff is part of the olfactory percept. Chemical Senses 31:181–96.Google Scholar
Mink, J. W. (1996) The basal ganglia: Focused selection and inhibition of competing motor programs. Progress in Neurobiology 50:381425.Google Scholar
Shen, Y. C. & Franz, E. A. (2005) Hemispheric competition in left-handers on bimanual reaction time tasks. Journal of Motor Behavior 37:39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sperry, R. W. (1952) Neurology and the mind-brain problem. American Scientist 40:291312.Google Scholar
Tassinari, G. & Berlucchi, G. (1993) Sensory and attentional components of slowing of manual reaction time to non-fixated visual targets by ipsilateral primes. Vision Research 33(11):1525–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed