No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Unifying theories of reasoning and decision making
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 18 July 2023
Abstract
De Neys offers a welcome departure from the dual-process accounts that have dominated theorizing about reasoning. However, we see little justification for retaining the distinction between intuition and deliberation. Instead, reasoning can be treated as a case of multiple-cue decision making. Reasoning phenomena can then be explained by decision-making models that supply the processing details missing from De Neys's framework.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Busemeyer, J. R., & Townsend, J. T. (1993). Decision field theory: A dynamic-cognitive approach to decision making in an uncertain environment. Psychological Review, 100(3), 432–459. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.100.3.432CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cushman, F., Young, L., & Hauser, M. (2006). The role of conscious reasoning and intuition in moral judgment: Testing three principles of harm. Psychological Science, 17(12), 1082–1089. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01834.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dube, C., Rotello, C. M., & Heit, E. (2010). Assessing the belief bias effect with ROCs: It's a response bias effect. Psychological Review, 117(3), 831–863. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/a0019634CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. St. B. T., & Curtis-Holmes, J. (2005). Rapid responding increases belief bias: Evidence for the dual-process theory of reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning, 11, 382–389. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546780542000005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, G. E., Hayes, B. K., & Heit, E. (2016). A dynamic model of reasoning and memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 155–180. doi: 10.1037/xge0000113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, B. K., Stephens, R. G., Lee, M. D., Dunn, J. C., Kaluve, A., Choi-Christou, J., & Cruz, N. (2022). Always look on the bright side of logic? Testing explanations of intuitive sensitivity to logic in perceptual tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 48(11), 1598–1617. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001105Google ScholarPubMed
Hayes, B. K., Stephens, R. G., Ngo, J., & Dunn, J. C. (2018). The dimensionality of reasoning: Evidence for a single process account of inductive and deductive inference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 44, (9), 1333–1351. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000527Google Scholar
Krajbich, I., Bartling, B., Hare, T., & Fehr, E. (2015). Rethinking fast and slow based on a critique of reaction-time reverse inference. Nature Communications, 6(1), 1–9. doi: 10.1038/ncomms8455CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logan, G. D., & Klapp, S. T. (1991). Automatizing alphabet arithmetic: I. Is extended practice necessary to produce automaticity? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(2), 179–195. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0278-7393.17.2.179Google Scholar
Palada, H., Neal, A., Vuckovic, A., Martin, R., Samuels, K., & Heathcote, A. (2016). Evidence accumulation in a complex task: Making choices about concurrent multi-attribute stimuli under time pressure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 22(1), 1–23. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/xap0000074Google Scholar
Ratcliff, R., Smith, P. L., Brown, S. D., & McKoon, G. (2016). Diffusion decision model: Current issues and history. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(4), 260–281. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.01.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Target article
Advancing theorizing about fast-and-slow thinking
Related commentaries (34)
A good architecture for fast and slow thinking, but exclusivity is exclusively in the past
A tale of two histories: Dual-system architectures in modular perspective
A view from mindreading on fast-and-slow thinking
Advancing theorizing about fast-and-slow thinking: The interplay between fast and slow processing
Automatic threat processing shows evidence of exclusivity
Categorizing judgments as likely to be selected by intuition or deliberation
Conflict paradigms cannot reveal competence
Correction, uncertainty, and anchoring effects
Could Bayesian cognitive science undermine dual-process theories of reasoning?
Deliberation is (probably) triggered and sustained by multiple mechanisms
Deliberative control is more than just reactive: Insights from sequential sampling models
Dual-process moral judgment beyond fast and slow
Dual-process theory is Barbapapa
Explaining normative–deliberative gaps is essential to dual-process theorizing
Fast and slow language processing: A window into dual-process models of cognition
Hoist by its own petard: The ironic and fatal flaws of dual-process theory
How research on persuasion can inform dual-process models of judgment
Illusory intuitions: Challenging the claim of non-exclusivity
Individual differences and multi-step thinking
Learning how to reason and deciding when to decide
More than two intuitions
Not feeling right about uncertainty monitoring
Switching between system 1 and system 2: The nature of competing intuitions and the role of disfluency
Switching: Cultural fluency sustains and cultural disfluency disrupts thinking fast
The distinction between long-term knowledge and short-term control processes is valid and useful
The dual-system approach is a useful heuristic but does not accurately describe behavior
Toward dual-process theory 3.0
Unifying theories of reasoning and decision making
Using the study of reasoning to address the age of unreason
We know what stops you from thinking forever: A metacognitive perspective
What is intuiting and deliberating? A functional–cognitive perspective
When a thinker does not want to think: Adding meta-control into the working model
Why is system 1/system 2 switching affectively loaded?
“Switching” between fast and slow processes is just reward-based branching
Author response
Further advancing fast-and-slow theorizing