Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T04:13:38.352Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A psycho-historical research program for the integrative science of art

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 March 2013

Nicolas J. Bullot
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive Science, ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and Its Disorders, Macquarie University, NSW 2109, [email protected]://www.maccs.mq.edu.au/members/profile.html?memberID=521
Rolf Reber
Affiliation:
Department of Education, University of Bergen, Postboks 7807, N-5020 Bergen, Norway. [email protected]://h.uib.no/examplewiki/en/index.php/Rolf_Reber

Abstract

Critics of the target article objected to our account of art appreciators' sensitivity to art-historical contexts and functions, the relations among the modes of artistic appreciation, and the weaknesses of aesthetic science. To rebut these objections and justify our program, we argue that the current neglect of sensitivity to art-historical contexts persists as a result of a pervasive aesthetic–artistic confound; we further specify our claim that basic exposure and the design stance are necessary conditions of artistic understanding; and we explain why many experimental studies do not belong to a psycho-historical science of art.

Type
Authors' Response
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Azzouni, J. (2004) Theory, observation and scientific realism. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 55(3):371–92. DOI: 10.1093/bjps/55.3.371.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. (1999) Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22(04):577660.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. (2008) Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology 59(1):617–45. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639.Google Scholar
Beardsley, M. C. (1958/1981) Aesthetics: Problems in the philosophy of criticism (second edition). Hackett Publishing.Google Scholar
Beardsley, M. C. (1983) An aesthetic definition of art. In: What is art?, ed. Curtler, H. M., pp. 1529. Haven.Google Scholar
Bechtel, W. (2008) Mental mechanisms: Philosophical perspectives on cognitive neuroscience. Routledge.Google Scholar
Bechtel, W. & Richardson, R. C. (1993) Discovering complexity: Decomposition and localization as strategies in scientific research. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Belke, B., Leder, H. & Augustin, D. (2006) Mastering style. Effects of explicit style-related information, art knowledge and affective state on appreciation of abstract paintings. Psychology Science 48:115–35.Google Scholar
Bering, J. M. (2006) The folk psychology of souls. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29(5):453–98. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X06499106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berlyne, D. E. (1971) Aesthetics and psychobiology. Meredith.Google Scholar
Block, B. A., Teo, S. L. H., Walli, A., Boustany, A. M., Stokesbury, M. J. W., Farwell, C. J. & Williams, T. D. (2005) Electronic tagging and population structure of Atlantic bluefin tuna. Nature 434:1121–27.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (1996a) Intention, history, and artifact concepts. Cognition 60:129. DOI: 10.1016/0010-0277(95)00699-0.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (1996b) Possible individuals in language and cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science 5(3):9094. DOI: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10772823.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (1998) Theories of artifact categorization. Cognition 66:8793. DOI: 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00003-1.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2004) Descartes' baby: How the science of child development explains what makes us human. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2010) How pleasure works: The new science of why we like what we like. Norton.Google Scholar
Bohlman, P. V. (1988) The study of folk music in the modern world. Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Botvinick, M. M. (2008) Hierarchical models of behavior and prefrontal function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12:201208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bourdieu, P. (1979/1987) Distinction: A social critique of the judgment of taste, trans. Nice, R.. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. & Wertsch, J. V. (2009) Memory in mind and culture. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Breton, A. (1924/1988) Manifeste du surréalisme Œuvres complètes, I, pp. 310–46. Gallimard.Google Scholar
Brinck, I. (2007) Situated cognition, dynamic systems, and art: On artistic creativity and aesthetic experience. Janus Head, 9(2):407–31.Google Scholar
Buckland, T. J., ed. (2006) Dancing from past to present: Nation, culture, identities. University of Wisconsin Press.Google Scholar
Bullot, N. J. (2006) The principle of ontological commitment in pre- and postmortem multiple agent tracking. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29:466–68.Google Scholar
Bullot, N. J. (2009a) Material anamnesis and the prompting of aesthetic worlds: The psycho-historical theory of artworks. Journal of Consciousness Studies 16(1):85109. Available at: http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/imp/jcs/2009/00000016/00000001/art00004.Google Scholar
Bullot, N. J. (2009b) Toward a theory of the empirical tracking of individuals: Cognitive flexibility and the functions of attention in integrated tracking. Philosophical Psychology 22(3):353–87. DOI: 10.1080/09515080902969006.Google Scholar
Bullot, N. J. & Droulez, J. (2008) Keeping track of invisible individuals while exploring a spatial layout with partial cues: Location-based and deictic direction-based strategies. Philosophical Psychology 21(1):1546. DOI: 10.1080/09515080701840861.Google Scholar
Bullot, N. J. & Égré, P. (2010) Editorial: Objects and sound perception. Review of Philosophy and Psychology 1(1):517. DOI: 10.1007/s13164-009-0006-3.Google Scholar
Bullot, N. J. & Rysiew, P. (2007) A study in the cognition of individuals' identity: Solving the problem of singular cognition in object and agent tracking. Consciousness and Cognition 16(2):276–93. DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2006.09.006.Google Scholar
Carroll, N. (1986) Art and interaction. The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 45(1):5768.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, N. (2000a) Art and the domain of the aesthetic. The British Journal of Aesthetics 40(2):191208. DOI: 10.1093/bjaesthetics/40.2.191.Google Scholar
Carroll, N. (2000b) Interpretation and intention: The debate between hypothetical and actual intentionalism. Metaphilosophy 31(1–2), 7595. DOI: 10.1111/1467-9973.00131.Google Scholar
Carroll, N. (2001) Beyond aesthetics: Philosophical essays. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, N. (2002) Aesthetic experience revisited. British Journal of Aesthetics 42(2):145–68. DOI: 10.1093/bjaesthetics/42.2.145.Google Scholar
Carruthers, P. (2009) Mindreading underlies metacognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32(02):164–82. DOI:10.1017/S0140525X09000831.Google Scholar
Clark, A. J. (2008) Supersizing the mind: Embodiement, action, and cognitive extension. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. J. & Chalmers, D. J. (1998) The extended mind. Analysis 58:1023.Google Scholar
Clark, T. J. (1973) Image of the people: Gustave Courbet and the Second French Republic 1848–1851. New York Graphic Society.Google Scholar
Clark, T. J. (1982) Clement Greenberg's theory of art. In: The politics of interpretation, ed. Mitchell, W. J. T., pp. 203–20. The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Clark, T. J. (2001) Farewell to an idea: Episodes from a history of modernism. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Connell, J. & Gibson, C. (2003) Sound tracks: Popular music, identity and place. Routledge.Google Scholar
Conway, M. A. (1990) Autobiographical memory: An introduction. Open University Press.Google Scholar
Conway, M. A. (2005) Memory and the self. Journal of Memory and Language 53(4):594628. DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2005.08.005.Google Scholar
Craig, B., Kernot, B., & Anderson, C. (1999) Art and performance in oceania. University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar
Currie, G. (2004) Arts and minds. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danto, A. C. (1966) The historical individual. In: Philosophical analysis and history, ed. Dray, W. H., pp. 265–96. Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Danto, A. C. (1981) The transfiguration of the commonplace: A philosophy of art. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Danto, A. C. (2003) The abuse of beauty: Aesthetics and the concept of art. Open Court.Google Scholar
Davies, S. (1982) The aesthetic relevance of authors' and painters' intentions. The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 41(1):6576.Google Scholar
Davies, S. (1991b) The ontology of musical works and the authenticity of their performances. Nous 25(1):2141.Google Scholar
Davies, S. (1996) Interpreting contextualities. Philosophy and Literature 20(1):2038.Google Scholar
Davies, S. (2012) The artful species: Aesthetics, art, and evolution. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dickie, G. (1964) The myth of the aesthetic attitude. American Philosophical Quarterly 1:5665.Google Scholar
Dissanayake, E. (1992) Homo aestheticus: Where art comes from and why. University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Dretske, F. I. (1981) Knowledge and the flow of information. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dretske, F. I. (1994) The explanatory role of information. Philosophical Transactions: Physical Sciences and Engineering 349(1689):5969.Google Scholar
Dubuffet, J. (1986) Asphyxiating culture and other writings. Four Walls Eight Windows.Google Scholar
Dutton, D. (1993) Tribal art and artifact. The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 51(1):1321.Google Scholar
Ellsworth, P. C. & Scherer, K. R. (2003) Appraisal processes in emotion. In: Handbook of affective sciences, ed. Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R. & Goldsmith, H. H., pp. 572–95. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fechner, G. T. (1876) Vorschule der Äesthetik [Elements of aesthetics]. Druck und Verlag von Breitkopf & Härtel.Google Scholar
Fischer, D. H. (1971) Historians' fallacies: Toward a logic of historical thought. Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Franklin, M. B., Becklen, R. C. & Doyle, C. L. (1993) The influence of titles on how paintings are seen. Leonardo 26(2):103108.Google Scholar
Fried, M. (1967/1998) Art and objecthood: Essays and reviews. The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Gelman, S. A. (2003) The essential child: Origins of essentialism in everyday thought. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelman, S. A. & Bloom, P. (2000) Young children are sensitive to how an object was created when deciding what to name it. Cognition 76:91103. DOI: 10.1016/s0010-0277(00)00071-8.Google Scholar
Gelman, S. A. & Bloom, P. (2007) Developmental changes in the understanding of generics. Cognition 105(1):166–83. DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.09.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelman, S. A., Coley, J. D. & Gottfried, G. M. (1994) Essentialist beliefs in children: The acquisition of concepts and theories. In: Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture, ed. Hirschfeld, L. A. & Gelman, S. A., pp. 341–65. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gelman, S. A. & Ebeling, K. S. (1998) Shape and representational status in children's early naming. Cognition 66(2):B3547. DOI: 10.1016/s0010-0277(98)00022-5.Google Scholar
Gelman, S. A. & Wellman, H. M. (1991) Insides and essences: Early understandings of the non-obvious. Cognition 38(3):213–44. DOI: 10.1016/0010-0277(91)90007-q.Google Scholar
Gendler, T. S. (2010) Intuition, imagination, and philosophical methodology. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. W. (2006) Embodiment and cognitive science. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gilmore, J. (2000) The life of a style: Beginnings and endings in the narrative history of art. Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Gilmore, J. (2011) A functional view of artistic evaluation. Philosophical Studies 155(2):289305. DOI: 10.1007/s11098-010-9570-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gobbini, M. I. & Haxby, J. V. (2007) Neural systems for recognition of familiar faces. Neuropsychologia 45(1):3241. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.015.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. I. (1967) A causal theory of knowing. Journal of Philosophy 64(12):357–72.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. I. (1999) Internalism exposed. Journal of Philosophy 95(6):271–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottschalk, L. (1950/1969) Understanding history: A primer of historical method (second ed.). Alfred A. Knopf.Google Scholar
Green, H. (2007) Wood: Craft, culture, history: Penguin Group USA.Google Scholar
Greenberg, C. (1961) Art and culture. Beacon Press.Google Scholar
Gutheil, G., Gelman, S. A., Klein, E., Michos, K. & Kelaita, K. (2008) Preschoolers' use of spatiotemporal history, appearance, and proper name in determining individual idenity. Cognition 107:366–80.Google Scholar
Hacking, I. (1995) Two kinds of “new historicism” for philosophers. In: History and … : histories within the human sciences, ed. Cohen, R. & Roth, M. S., pp. 296318. University Press of Virginia.Google Scholar
Hacking, I. (1999) The social construction of what? Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hacking, I. (2002) Historical ontology. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Heider, F. & Simmel, M. (1944) An experimental study of apparent behavior. American Journal of Psychology 57(2):243–59.Google Scholar
Heinich, N. (1996a) “C'est un oiseau!” Brancusi vs Etats-Unis, ou quand la loi définit l'art. Droit en Société, 34:649–72.Google Scholar
Hekkert, P. & van Wieringen, P. C. W. (1990) Complexity and prototypicality as determinants of appraisal of cubist paintings. British Journal of Psychology 81:483–95.Google Scholar
Hogan, P. C. (2011) What literature teaches us about emotion. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hood, B. M. & Bloom, P. (2008) Children prefer certain individuals over perfect duplicates. Cognition 106(1):455–62. DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.01.012.Google Scholar
Howell, M. & Prevenier, W. (2001) From reliable sources: An introduction to historical methods. Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Hyman, J. (2006) In search of the big picture. New Scientist 191(2563):4445.Google Scholar
Hyman, J. (2010) Art and Neuroscience. In: Beyond mimesis and convention, vol. 262, ed. Frigg, R. & Hunter, M., pp. 245–61. Springer Netherlands.Google Scholar
Jacobsen, T. (2006) Bridging the arts and sciences: A framework for the psychology of aesthetics. Leonardo 39(2):155–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobsen, T., Schubotz, R. I., Höfel, L. & von Cramon, D. Y. (2006) Brain correlates of aesthetic judgment of beauty. NeuroImage 29(1):276–85. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.010.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Treisman, A. & Gibbs, B. J. (1992) The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information. Cognitive Psychology 24(2):175219.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1793/2000) Critique of the power of judgment, trans. Guyer, P. & Matthews, E.. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kirk, U., Skov, M., Christensen, M. S. & Nygaard, N. (2009a) Brain correlates of aesthetic expertise: A parametric fMRI study. Brain and Cognition 69(2):306–15. DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.004. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0278262608002443.Google Scholar
Kirk, U., Skov, M., Hulme, O., Christensen, M. S. & Zeki, S. (2009b) Modulation of aesthetic value by semantic context: An fMRI study. NeuroImage 44(3):1125–32. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.009. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053811908011099.Google Scholar
Knobe, J. & Nichols, S. (2008) Experimental philosophy. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kornblith, H., ed. (2001) Epistemology: Internalism and externalism. Blackwell Publishers Ltd.Google Scholar
Krauss, R. E. (1979) Grids. October 9(50):64.Google Scholar
Kripke, S. (1980) Naming and necessity. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kruger, J., Wirtz, D., Van Boven, L. & Altermatt, T. W. (2004) The effort heuristic. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 40(1):9198. DOI: 10.1016/s0022-1031(03)00065-9.Google Scholar
Lackey, J. & Sosa, E., eds. (2006) The epistemology of testimony. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lambie, J. A. & Marcel, A. J. (2002) Consciousness and the varieties of emotion experience: A theoretical framework. Psychological Review 109(2):219–59. DOI: 10.1037//0033-295X.109.2.219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazarus, R. S. (1991) Emotion and adaptation. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Leder, H., Belke, B., Oeberst, A. & Augustin, D. (2004) A model of aesthetic appreciation and aesthetic judgments. British Journal of Psychology 95:489508. DOI: 10.1348/0007126042369811.Google Scholar
Leder, H., Carbon, C.-C. & Ripsas, A.-L. (2006) Entitling art: Influence of title information on understanding and appreciation of paintings. Acta Psychologica 121(2):176–98. DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.08.005.Google Scholar
Levinson, J. (1996b) The pleasures of aesthetics: Philosophical essays. Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Levinson, J. (2010) Defending hypothetical intentionalism. The British Journal of Aesthetics 50(2):139–50. DOI: 10.1093/aesthj/ayp072.Google Scholar
Leyton, M. (1992) Symmetry, causality, mind. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Liebenberg, L. (1990) The art of tracking: The origin of science. David Philip.Google Scholar
Lindell, A. K. & Mueller, J. (2011) Can science account for taste? Psychological insights into art appreciation. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 23(4):453–75. DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2011.539556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manzotti, R., ed. (2011) Situated aesthetics: Art beyond the skin. Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
Martindale, C. (1990) The clockwork muse: The predictability of artistic change. Basic Books.Google Scholar
McCullagh, C. B. (1984) Justifying historical descriptions. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McManus, I. C., Cheema, B. & Stoker, J. (1993) The aesthetics of composition: A study of Mondrian. Empirical Studies of the Arts 11(2):8394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menary, R., ed. (2010) The extended mind. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Millikan, R. G. (1999) Historical kinds and the special sciences. Philosophical Studies 95(1–2):4565.Google Scholar
Millikan, R. G. (2000) On clear and confused ideas: An essay about substance concepts. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Millis, K. (2001) Making meaning brings pleasure: The influence of titles on aesthetic experiences. Emotion, 3:320–29. DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.1.3.320.Google Scholar
Mitchell, S. D. (2009) Unsimple truths: Science, complexity, and policy. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Munro, T. (1951) Aesthetics as science: Its development in America. The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 9(3):161207.Google Scholar
Murphy, G. L. & Medin, D. L. (1985) The role of theories in conceptual coherence. Psychological Review 92(3):289316.Google Scholar
Newman, G. E. & Bloom, P. (2012) Art and authenticity: The importance of originals in judgments of value. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 141(3):558–69. DOI: 10.1037/a0026035.Google Scholar
Newman, G. E., Diesendruck, G. & Bloom, P. (2011) Celebrity contagion and the value of objects. Journal of Consumer Research 38:215–28.Google Scholar
Noë, A. (2011) Art and the limits of neuroscience. Opinionator, The New York Times, (December 4). Available at: http://opinionator.blogs.nytimes.com/2011/2012/2004/art-and-the-limits-of-neuroscience.Google Scholar
Ògúnjìmí, B. & Na'allah, A. R. (2005) Introduction to African oral literature and performance. Africa World Press.Google Scholar
Osborne, H. (1981) What is a work of art? The British Journal of Aesthetics 21(1):311. DOI: 10.1093/bjaesthetics/21.1.3.Google Scholar
Parsons, G. & Carlson, A. (2008) Functional beauty. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Plassmann, H., O'Doherty, J., Shiv, B. & Rangel, A. (2008) Marketing actions can modulate neural representations of experienced pleasantness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105(3):1050–54. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0706929105.Google Scholar
Preston, B. (1998) Why is a wing like a spoon? A pluralist theory of function. The Journal of Philosophy 95(5):215–54.Google Scholar
Prins, G. (1991) Oral history. In: New perspectives on historical writing, ed. Burke, P., pp. 114–39. Pennsylvania State University Press.Google Scholar
Putnam, H. (1975) Mind, language and reality: Philosophical papers, vol. 2. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Putnam, H. (1981) Reason, truth and history. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. W. & Storm, R. W. (1988) Tracking multiple independent targets: Evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. Spatial Vision 3(3):179–97. DOI: 10.1163/156856888x00122.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S. (2011) The tell-tale brain: A neuroscientist's quest for what makes us human. Norton.Google Scholar
Rappaport, R. A. (1999) Ritual and religion in the making of humanity. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Reber, R. (2002) Reasons for the preference for symmetry. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25:415–16.Google Scholar
Reber, R. (2008) Art in its experience: Can empirical psychology help assess artistic value? Leonardo 41(4):367–72. DOI: 10.1162/leon.2008.41.4.367.Google Scholar
Reber, R. (2012) Processing fluency, aesthetic pleasure, and culturally shared taste. In: Aesthetic science: Connecting minds, brains, and experience, ed. Shimamura, A. P. & Palmer, S. E., pp. 223–49. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Reber, R. & Unkelbach, C. (2010) The epistemic status of processing fluency as source for judgments of truth. Review of Philosophy and Psychology 1(4):563–81. DOI: 10.1007/s13164-010-0039-7.Google Scholar
Rhodes, G. (2006) The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. Annual Review of Psychology 57(1):199226. DOI:10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190208.Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J. & Boyd, R. (2005) Not by genes alone, how culture transformed human evolution. The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Rips, L. J., Blok, S. & Newman, G. (2006) Tracing the identity of objects. Psychological Review 113(1):130. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.113.1.1.Google Scholar
Robinson, J. (2005) Deeper than reason: Emotion and its role in literature, music, and art. Clarendon.Google Scholar
Rölleke, H. (1991) New results of research on Grimm's Fairy Tales. In: The Brothers Grimm and Folktale, ed. McGlathery, J. M.. University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Rollins, M. (2003a) Perceptual strategies and pictorial content. In: Looking into pictures: An interdisciplinary approach to pictorial space, ed. Hecht, H., Schwartz, R. & Atherton, M., pp. 99122. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rollins, M. (2003b) The mind in pictures: Perceptual strategies and the interpretation of visual art. The Monist 86(4):608–31.Google Scholar
Rollins, M. (2004) What Monet meant: Intention and attention in understanding art. The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 62(2):175–88. DOI: 10.1111/j.1540-594X.2004.00150.Google Scholar
Rollins, M. (2011) Neurology and the new riddle of pictorial style. In: The aesthetic mind: Philosophy and psychology, ed. Schellekens, E. & Goldie, P., pp. 391413. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rowell, M. (1999) Brancusi vs. United States, the historic trial, 1928. Adam Biro.Google Scholar
Russell, P. A. (2003) Effort after meaning and the hedonic value of paintings. British Journal of Psychology 94(1):99110. DOI: 10.1348/000712603762842138 Google Scholar
Shafer, R. J., ed. (1969/1974) A guide to historical method. Dorsey Press.Google Scholar
Shimamura, A. P., ed. (2012) Toward a science of aesthetics. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shimamura, A. P. & Palmer, S. E., eds. (2012) Aesthetic science: Connecting minds, brains, and experience. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shiner, L. (2003) The invention of art: A cultural history. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J. (2005a) Cognitive appraisals and interest in visual art: Exploring an appraisal theory of aesthetic emotions. Empirical Studies of the Arts 23:119–33.Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J. (2005b) Emotional responses to art: From collation and arousal to cognition and emotion. Review of General Psychology 9(4):342–57.Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J. (2005c) What is interesting? Exploring the appraisal structure of interest. Emotion 5:89102. DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.89.Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1969/1996). The sciences of the artificial, third edition. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Skov, M. & Vartanian, O. (2009) Neuroaesthetics. Baywood.Google Scholar
Slingerland, E. G. & Collard, M. (2011) Creating consilience: Integrating the sciences and the humanities. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Smail, D. L. (2008) On deep history and the brain. University of California Press.Google Scholar
Smith, L. F., Bousquet, S. G., Chang, G. & Smith, J. K. (2006) Effects of time and information on perception of art. Empirical Studies of the Arts 24(2):229–42. DOI: 10.2190/DJM0-QBDW-03V7-BLRM.Google Scholar
Snow, C. P. (1959) The two cultures. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Specht, S. M. (2010) Artists' statements can influence perceptions of artwork. Empirical Studies of the Arts 28(2):193206. DOI: 10.2190/EM.28.2.e.Google Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2012) The evolved apprentice: How evolution made humans unique. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Stolnitz, J. (1960) Aesthetics and philosophy of art criticism: A critical introduction. Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Strawson, P. F. (1959) Individuals: An essay in descriptive metaphysics. Methuen.Google Scholar
Sutton, J. (2000) Body, mind, and order: Local memory and the control of mental representations in medieval and Renaissance sciences of self. In: 1543 and all that: Word and image in the proto-scientific revolution, ed. Freeland, G. & Corones, A., Kluwer, pp. 117–50.Google Scholar
Sutton, J. (2006) Spongy brains and material memories. In: Embodiment and environment in early modern drama and performance, ed. Floyd-Wilson, M. & Sullivan, G. A., pp. 1434. Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Sutton, J. (2008) Material agency, skills and history: Distributed cognition and the archaeology of memory. In: Material agency: Towards a non-anthropocentric approach, ed. Knappett, C. & Malafouris, L., pp. 3755. Springer.Google Scholar
Sutton, J. (2010) Exograms and interdisciplinarity: History, the extended mind, and the civilizing process. In: The extended mind, ed. Menary, R., pp. 189225. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Takahashi, S. (1995) Aesthetic properties of pictorial perception. Psychological Review 102(4):671–83.Google Scholar
Tatarkiewicz, W. (1971) What is art? The problem of definition today. The British Journal of Aesthetics 11(2):134–53. DOI: 10.1093/bjaesthetics/11.2.134.Google Scholar
Temme, J. E. V. (1992) Amount and kind of information in museums: Its effects on visitors satisfaction and appreciation of art. Visual Arts Research 18(2):2836.Google Scholar
Thompson, W. F. (2008) Music, thought, and feeling: Understanding the psychology of music. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tolhurst, W. (1984) Toward an aesthetic account of the nature of art. The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 42(3):261–69.Google Scholar
Tribble, E. & Sutton, J. (2011) Cognitive ecology as a framework for Shakespearean studies. Shakespeare Studies 39:94(10).Google Scholar
Vlach, J. M. (1990) The Afro-American tradition in decorative arts. University of Georgia Press.Google Scholar
White, M. (1965) Foundations of historical knowledge. Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Wiesmann, M. & Ishai, A. (2010) Training facilitates object recognition in Cubist paintings. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 4:11. doi:10.3389/neuro.09.011.2010. Available at: http://www.frontiersin.org/Journal/Abstract.aspx?s=537&name=human_neuroscience&ART_DOI=10.3389/neuro.09.011.2010.Google Scholar
Wigmore, J. H. (1913) The principles of judicial proof as given by logic, psychology, and general experience and illustrated in judicial trials. Little, Brown.Google Scholar
Wilson, R. A. (2004) Boundaries of the mind: The individual in the fragile sciences: Cognition. Cambridge University Press, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, R. A. & Clark, A. (2009) How to situate cognition: Letting nature take its course. In: The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition, ed. Aydede, M. & Robbins, P., pp. 5577. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, T. D. (2002) Strangers to ourselves: Discovering the adaptive unconscious. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Yassif, E. (1999) The Hebrew folktale: History, genre, meaning. Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Zeki, S. (1999) Inner vision: An exploration of art and the brain. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zipes, J. D. (2006) Why fairy tales stick: The evolution and relevance of a genre. Routledge.Google Scholar