Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T05:07:11.727Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neuropsychological predictions on involuntary autobiographical memory and déjà vu

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 November 2023

Christine Bastin*
Affiliation:
GIGA-Cyclotron Research Center-In Vivo Imaging (GIGA Institute), University of Liège, Liège, Belgium [email protected] https://www.gigaaml.uliege.be

Abstract

I strongly support Barzykowski and Moulin in their proposal that common retrieval mechanisms can lead to distinct phenomenological memory experiences. I emphasize the importance of one of these mechanisms, namely the attribution system. Neuropsychological studies should help clarifying the role of these retrieval mechanisms, notably in cases of medial temporal-lobe lesions and cases of dementia.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

A fundamental tenet in Barzykowski and Moulin's (B&M) proposal is the idea of common retrieval mechanisms leading to distinct phenomenological memory experiences depending on the success/failure and degree of involvement of these mechanisms. In both involuntary autobiographical memory and déjà vu, there is a rapid cue-generated automatic search in memory, a feeling of familiarity and attribution processes that come into play. I agree with B&M that a key difference lies in the content reactivation, with content that comes successfully to mind in the case of involuntary autobiographical memories and failure to find any content in the case of déjà vu. However, more emphasis could be made on the fact that the two types of memory experiences critically differ in the extent of contribution of the attribution system. Of note, the notion of attribution system refers here to the cognitive appraisal of the result of the memory search to generate an output (Bastin et al., Reference Bastin, Besson, Simon, Delhaye, Geurten, Willems and Salmon2019). In other words, this corresponds to the set of inferential and monitoring processes that evaluate retrieved contents before deciding about the old/new status of the information or expressing a subjective feeling of memory. In involuntary autobiographical memories, the retrieval cue leads to the rapid reactivation of some content that is compatible with expectations that there is indeed a memory trace. Here, the match between content reactivation and feelings of familiarity is satisfactory and not surprising, so that attribution processes take the form of relatively automatic and unconscious inferential processes. In contrast, in déjà vu, some cues generate expectation of processing fluency and feeling of familiarity, but the search fails to find any content. Attribution processes become hyperactive because they detect a mismatch between content and feeling. Moreover, there is knowledge that it is not possible to have content. This conflict reaches consciousness and is likely responsible for the unique phenomenology of déjà vu. Another common mechanism that is evoked in B&M is that elements of internal and external context are automatically weighted against prior knowledge, and this generates expectations. There are similarities with the ideas proposed in the predictive interactive multiple memory systems model (Henson & Gagnepain, Reference Henson and Gagnepain2010). Notably, Henson and Gagnepain (Reference Henson and Gagnepain2010) suggest that the content retrieved in a memory system is compared to context-based predictions. In this view, déjà vu arises from a prediction error as the absence of memory content mismatches the expectations.

If involuntary autobiographical memory and déjà vu rely on the same neurocognitive architecture, one expects that selective dysfunction of some retrieval mechanisms following brain damage impacts the expression of these phenomenological experiences. Predictions for brain-damaged patients is only briefly mentioned in B&M's article, but this is an important avenue for future research as it provides a way to test predictions emerging from the proposal. B&M predict that patients with medial temporal-lobe damage should show similar changes in déjà vu and involuntary autobiographical memories. For instance, temporal-lobe epilepsy would be associated with more déjà vu experiences and more involuntary autobiographical memories. However, if the two memory phenomena differ critically on the access to content, one may predict a different pattern. Considering that the medial temporal lobe is critical to store contents of past episodes (Davachi, Reference Davachi2006; Eichenbaum, Yonelinas, & Ranganath, Reference Eichenbaum, Yonelinas and Ranganath2007; Ranganath & Ritchey, Reference Ranganath and Ritchey2012), medial temporal-lobe damage should decrease involuntary autobiographical memories; and as metacognitive appraisal would partly depend on frontal areas (Chua, Pergolizzi, & Weintraub, Reference Chua, Pergolizzi, Weintraub, Fleming and Frith2014; Kurilla & Gonsalves, Reference Kurilla and Gonsalves2012) and is not expected to be impaired in medial temporal-lobe pathology, such patients may show preserved or more déjà vu experiences. The opposite pattern would be expected in patients with frontal-lobe damage, with the disappearance of déjà vu experiences and intact or increased involuntary autobiographical memories.

An interesting case is the one of patients with dementia as the extent of the neuropathology disrupts several retrieval mechanisms evoked as the building blocks for involuntary autobiographical memory and déjà vu. Patients with dementia sometimes present with persistent déjà vu whereby they have the feeling that everyday life events constantly repeat (Moulin, Reference Moulin2013). These patients typically come up with some explanations about their pervasive feeling of memory (i.e., confabulations) and do not report knowing that this feeling of memory is false. This is likely because both memory content and attribution processes are altered because of medial temporal and frontal pathology (Moulin, Reference Moulin2013). Another line of research suggests that demented patients have preserved involuntary autobiographical memories evoked by nostalgia films or music (El Haj, Fasotti, & Allain, Reference El Haj, Fasotti and Allain2012; Rasmussen, Salgado, Daustrand, & Berntsen, Reference Rasmussen, Salgado, Daustrand and Berntsen2021), which may seem surprising if these memories rely on the same retrieval mechanisms that are impaired and cause persistent déjà vu. Nevertheless, it is likely that those memories are recollection of remote memories, mostly belonging to young adulthood, whose content is consolidated (Berntsen, Kirk, & Kopelman, Reference Berntsen, Kirk and Kopelman2022). So, the question remains as to the integrity of involuntary autobiographical recall of recent memories in demented patients. This also raises the possibility that involuntary autobiographical memory and déjà vu differ on another dimension, which is temporality. Whereas involuntary autobiographical memory brings back to mind events from any time in the past, déjà vu seems anchored in the present (i.e., “the situation I experience now feels familiar even if I know it is new”) with some glimpse into the future with the feeling of prescience. I agree with B&M that considering both involuntary autobiographical memory and déjà vu experiences within the same sample would provide insights as to their common mechanisms.

Financial support

This work was supported by the F.R.S.-FNRS (C. B. is a senior research associate at F.R.S.-FNRS).

Competing interest

None.

References

Bastin, C., Besson, G., Simon, J., Delhaye, E., Geurten, M., Willems, S., & Salmon, E. (2019). An integrative memory model of recollection and familiarity to understand memory deficits. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 42(e281), 160. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X19000621CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berntsen, D., Kirk, M., & Kopelman, M. D. (2022). Autobiographical memory loss in Alzheimer's disease: The role of the reminiscence bump. Cortex, 150, 137148. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2022.02.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chua, E., Pergolizzi, D., & Weintraub, R. R. (2014). The cognitive neuroscience of metamemory monitoring: Understanding metamemory processes, subjective levels expressed, and metacognitive accuracy. In Fleming, S. M. & Frith, C. D. (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of metacognition (pp. 267291). Springer Berlin Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-45190-4_12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davachi, L. (2006). Item, context and relational episodic encoding in humans. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16(6), 693700. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2006.10.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eichenbaum, H., Yonelinas, A. P., & Ranganath, C. (2007). The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 30, 123152. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094328CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
El Haj, M., Fasotti, L., & Allain, P. (2012). The involuntary nature of music-evoked autobiographical memories in Alzheimer's disease. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(1), 238246. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.12.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henson, R. N., & Gagnepain, P. (2010). Predictive, interactive multiple memory systems. Hippocampus, 20(11), 13151326. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.20857CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurilla, B. P., & Gonsalves, B. D. (2012). An ERP investigation into the strategic regulation of the fluency heuristic during recognition memory [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural]. Brain Research, 1442, 3646. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2011.12.060CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moulin, C. J. (2013). Disordered recognition memory: Recollective confabulation. Cortex, 49(6), 15411552. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.01.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ranganath, C., & Ritchey, M. (2012). Two cortical systems for memory-guided behaviour. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 13, 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rasmussen, K. W., Salgado, S., Daustrand, M., & Berntsen, D. (2021). Using nostalgia films to stimulate spontaneous autobiographical remembering in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 10(3), 400411. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2020.11.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar