Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T05:16:16.190Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Moral emotions underlie puritanical morality

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 October 2023

Ruida Zhu
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, China. [email protected]; https://psy.sysu.edu.cn/teacher/1138 Business School, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
Chao Liu
Affiliation:
State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning and IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China. [email protected]; https://liuchaolab.bnu.edu.cn/mobile/en/ Center for Collaboration and Innovation in Brain and Learning Sciences, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China Beijing Key Laboratory of Brain Imaging and Connectomics, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China

Abstract

Fitouchi et al. illustrate the cognitive and evolutionary foundations of puritanical morality, while leave the emotional foundation unclear. We complement their theory by proposing moral emotions (e.g., guilt and shame) as characteristic emotions underlying puritanical morality. Our proposition is based on the findings that these moral emotions emerge after violations of puritanical norms and promote self-control and cooperation.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

In the target article, Fitouchi et al. build a new theory that puritanical morality is developed for promoting cooperation by facilitating self-control (regardless its efficiency). Besides, they cast doubts on an influential disgust-based account of puritanical morality, which considers the function of puritanical morality as avoiding communicable diseases driven by a feeling of disgust. Although Fitouchi et al. have elucidated the cognitive and evolutionary foundations of their cooperation-based theory with sufficient evidence, they leave the emotional foundation of puritanical morality unclear after denying the role of disgust. Are there any emotions underlying puritanical morality? What are they? We would like to extend Fitouchi et al.'s theory by proposing moral emotions such as guilt and shame as characteristic emotions in puritanical morality. Our proposition is based on three reasons: (1) violations of puritanical norms induce guilt and shame; (2) guilt and shame support self-control; and (3) guilt and shame enhance cooperation.

First, it is widely reported that people feel guilty and ashamed for conducting various behaviors that condemned by puritanical morality, including binge eating, masturbation, gambling, neglecting to study, failing to excise, and so on (Baumeister, Reference Baumeister1995; Berg et al., Reference Berg, Crosby, Cao, Crow, Engel, Wonderlich and Peterson2015; Mageau, Vallerand, Rousseau, Ratelle, & Provencher, Reference Mageau, Vallerand, Rousseau, Ratelle and Provencher2005; Mosher, Reference Mosher1979; Ratelle, Vallerand, Mageau, Rousseau, & Provencher, Reference Ratelle, Vallerand, Mageau, Rousseau and Provencher2004; Sharma & Sharma, Reference Sharma and Sharma1998). Notably, behaviors manifesting lack of self-discipline (e.g., failing to excise) that are moralized by puritanical morality seem unrelated to disgust, but are related to guilt and shame (Baumeister, Reference Baumeister1995; Harman & Burrows, Reference Harman and Burrows2019; Markland & Tobin, Reference Markland and Tobin2004). Thus, guilt and shame compared to disgust has closer associations with violations of puritanical norms.

Second, guilt and (maybe) shame are supposed to help people inhibit selfish impulses and hedonic motives (Baumeister, Reference Baumeister1995; Baumeister & Exline, Reference Baumeister and Exline1999). Supporting this opinion, behavioral experiments found that guilt and shame promote behaviors that need self-control, such as costly apology, help, amend, and self-punishment (de Hooge, Zeelenberg, & Breugelmans, Reference de Hooge, Zeelenberg and Breugelmans2007; Ohtsubo & Yagi, Reference Ohtsubo and Yagi2015; Yu, Hu, Hu, & Zhou, Reference Yu, Hu, Hu and Zhou2014; Zhu et al., Reference Zhu, Jin, Shen, Zhang, Mai and Liu2017). Neuroimaging experiments also provided supportive evidence that guilt compared to other emotions (e.g., sadness and shame) produces stronger activation in brain regions implicated in self-control, such as orbitofrontal cortex and lateral prefrontal cortex (Wagner, N'Diaye, Ethofer, & Vuilleumier, Reference Wagner, N'Diaye, Ethofer and Vuilleumier2011; Zhu, Feng, Zhang, Mai, & Liu, Reference Zhu, Feng, Zhang, Mai and Liu2019) and that shame is associated with activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex related to self-control (Bastin, Harrison, Davey, Moll, & Whittle, Reference Bastin, Harrison, Davey, Moll and Whittle2016). Considering Fitouchi et al. highlight that puritanical morality aims to improve self-control and prevent self-control failures, guilt and shame are conducive to achieving the aim of puritanical morality.

Third, looking at the bigger picture, the social function of guilt and shame (particularly guilt) is maintaining and repairing cooperative relationships (Chang, Smith, Dufwenberg, & Sanfey, Reference Chang, Smith, Dufwenberg and Sanfey2011; Sznycer, Reference Sznycer2019). As moral violations induce guilt and people are guilt averse, people usually act in a moral way that trying to minimize their anticipated guilt regarding their decisions, which promotes greater levels of cooperation (Battigalli & Dufwenberg, Reference Battigalli and Dufwenberg2007; Bellemare, Sebald, & Suetens, Reference Bellemare, Sebald and Suetens2019; Charness & Dufwenberg, Reference Charness and Dufwenberg2006). Guilt avoidance is a crucial mechanism that prevents moral violations, motivates cooperative behavior, and maintains cooperative relationships (Chang et al., Reference Chang, Smith, Dufwenberg and Sanfey2011). Guilt and (maybe) shame not only can maintain cooperative relationships, but also help to restore jeopardized relationships. After violating moral norms, people are faced with blame, punishment, and even exclusion from future cooperation (Boyd, Gintis, Bowles, & Richerson, Reference Boyd, Gintis, Bowles and Richerson2003; Fehr & Gächter, Reference Fehr and Gächter2002; Tomasello & Vaish, Reference Tomasello and Vaish2013). To cope with this problem, guilt and shame urge people to conduct behaviors (e.g., apology, compensation, and self-punishment) that require sacrificing short-term interests (e.g., body pleasure and monetary reward) and weighting long-term benefits (cooperative relationships) (Ghorbani, Liao, Çayköylü, & Chand, Reference Ghorbani, Liao, Çayköylü and Chand2013; Nelissen, Reference Nelissen2011; Nelissen & Zeelenberg, Reference Nelissen and Zeelenberg2009; Watanabe & Ohtsubo, Reference Watanabe and Ohtsubo2012; Yu et al., Reference Yu, Hu, Hu and Zhou2014; Zhu et al., Reference Zhu, Shen, Tang, Ye, Wang, Mai and Liu2017). Studies have found that guilt- and/or shame-induced behaviors (e.g., apology, compensation, and self-punishment) can facilitate forgiveness from others and restore jeopardized relationships (Hechler, Wenzel, Woodyatt, & de Vel-Palumbo, Reference Hechler, Wenzel, Woodyatt and de Vel-Palumbo2022; McCullough, Kurzban, & Tabak, Reference McCullough, Kurzban and Tabak2013; Zhu et al., Reference Zhu, Shen, Tang, Ye, Wang, Mai and Liu2017). Given Fitouchi et al. advocate that the ultimate function of puritanical morality is boosting cooperation, the functions of guilt and shame and puritanical morality coincide well with each other.

It is difficult to judge whether guilt or shame plays a more important role in puritanical morality at the current stage. One problem is that some researchers construed “guilt” as a synonym for “shame” or vice versa. Another problem is that many studies measured only guilt or only shame. Still another problem is that guilt and shame tend to co-occur after moral violations (e.g., Nelissen & Zeelenberg, Reference Nelissen and Zeelenberg2009). We note that guilt and shame have conceptual, theoretical, and neural differences (e.g., Bastin et al., Reference Bastin, Harrison, Davey, Moll and Whittle2016; Tangney & Dearing, Reference Tangney and Dearing2003; Tangney, Miller, Flicker, & Barlow, Reference Tangney, Miller, Flicker and Barlow1996). To distinguish the influences of guilt and shame on puritanical morality, we encourage future studies to (1) measure both guilt and shame feelings (e.g., Ghorbani et al., Reference Ghorbani, Liao, Çayköylü and Chand2013) and (2) create both guilt and shame conditions, in which guilt and shame are respectively the dominant emotion (e.g., Wagner et al., Reference Wagner, N'Diaye, Ethofer and Vuilleumier2011; Xu et al., Reference Xu, Zhu, Zhang, Zhang, Liang, Mai and Liu2022).

Additionally, we keep an open mind about whether other moral emotions are involved in puritanical morality. For instance, several studies have demonstrated a link between gratitude and self-control (Desteno, Li, Dickens, & Lerner, Reference Desteno, Li, Dickens and Lerner2014; Dickens & DeSteno, Reference Dickens and DeSteno2016). Thorough explorations on the associations between various moral emotions and puritanical morality are needed in the future.

Moral emotions are vital elements of moral apparatus linking moral norms and moral behaviors (Tangney, Stuewig, & Mashek, Reference Tangney, Stuewig and Mashek2007). A moral theory without any concern about emotion is probably incomplete. We propose that moral emotions such as guilt and shame are characteristic emotions underlying puritanical morality, especially within the theoretical framework constructed by Fitouchi et al. We clarify the close associations among moral emotions, puritanical morality, self-control, and cooperation. Our extension contributes to filling in the missing part of Fitouchi et al.'s theory (i.e., the emotional foundation of puritanical morality) and setting a new direction for future research.

Financial support

Chao Liu was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (32271092, 32130045), the Major Project of National Social Science Foundation (19ZDA363), and the Beijing Municipal Science and Technology Commission (Z151100003915122). Ruida Zhu was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (32200884) and the International Postdoctoral Exchange Fellowship Program (No. 2020002).

Competing interest

None.

References

Bastin, C., Harrison, B. J., Davey, C. G., Moll, J., & Whittle, S. (2016). Feelings of shame, embarrassment and guilt and their neural correlates: A systematic review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 71, 455471. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Battigalli, P., & Dufwenberg, M. (2007). Guilt in games. American Economic Review, 97(2), 170176. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.97.2.170CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. (1995). Transcendence, guilt, and self-control. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18(1), 122123. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00037626CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., & Exline, J. J. (1999). Virtue, personality, and social relations: Self-control as the moral muscle. Journal of Personality, 67(6), 11651194. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-6494.00086CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellemare, C., Sebald, A., & Suetens, S. (2019). Guilt aversion in economics and psychology. Journal of Economic Psychology, 73(452), 5259. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2019.05.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berg, K. C., Crosby, R. D., Cao, L., Crow, S. J., Engel, S. G., Wonderlich, S. A., & Peterson, C. B. (2015). Negative affect prior to and following overeating-only, loss of control eating-only, and binge eating episodes in obese adults. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 48(6), 641653. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22401CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyd, R., Gintis, H., Bowles, S., & Richerson, P. J. (2003). The evolution of altruistic punishment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100(6), 35313535. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0630443100CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, L. J., Smith, A., Dufwenberg, M., & Sanfey, A. G. (2011). Triangulating the neural, psychological, and economic bases of guilt aversion. Neuron, 70(3), 560572. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.056CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charness, G., & Dufwenberg, M. (2006). Promises and partnership. Econometrica, 74(6), 15791601. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0262.2006.00719.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Hooge, I. E., Zeelenberg, M., & Breugelmans, S. M. (2007). Moral sentiments and cooperation: Differential influences of shame and guilt. Cognition & Emotion, 21(5), 10251042. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930600980874CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Desteno, D., Li, Y., Dickens, L., & Lerner, J. S. (2014). Gratitude: A tool for reducing economic impatience. Psychological Science, 25(6), 12621267. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614529979CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickens, L., & DeSteno, D. (2016). The grateful are patient: Heightened daily gratitude is associated with attenuated temporal discounting. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 16(4), 421425. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000176CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2002). Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature, 415(6868), 137140. https://doi.org/10.1038/415137aCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghorbani, M., Liao, Y., Çayköylü, S., & Chand, M. (2013). Guilt, shame, and reparative behavior: The effect of psychological proximity. Journal of Business Ethics, 114(2), 311323. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-012-1350-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harman, A., & Burrows, L. (2019). Leaning in while sticking out: Fat, exercise, and guilt. Fat Studies, 8(2), 187202. https://doi.org/10.1080/21604851.2019.1562838CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hechler, S., Wenzel, M., Woodyatt, L., & de Vel-Palumbo, M. (2022). What does being hard on yourself communicate to others? The role of symbolic implications of self-punishment in attributions of remorse. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 101, 104305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2022.104305CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mageau, G. A., Vallerand, R. J., Rousseau, F. L., Ratelle, C. F., & Provencher, P. J. (2005). Passion and gambling: Investigating the divergent affective and cognitive consequences of gambling. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 35(1), 100118. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2005.tb02095.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markland, D., & Tobin, V. (2004). A modification to the behavioural regulation in exercise questionnaire to include an assessment of amotivation. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 26(2), 191196. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.26.2.191CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCullough, M. E., Kurzban, R., & Tabak, B. A. (2013). Cognitive systems for revenge and forgiveness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(1), 115. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11002160CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mosher, D. L. (1979). Negative attitudes toward masturbation in sex therapy. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 5(4), 315333. https://doi.org/10.1080/00926237908407076CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelissen, R. M. A. (2011). Guilt-induced self-punishment as a sign of remorse. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(2), 139144. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550611411520CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelissen, R. M. A., & Zeelenberg, M. (2009). When guilt evokes self-punishment: Evidence for the existence of a Dobby effect. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 9(1), 118122. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014540CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ohtsubo, Y., & Yagi, A. (2015). Relationship value promotes costly apology-making: Testing the valuable relationships hypothesis from the perpetrator's perspective. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(3), 232239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.11.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ratelle, C. F., Vallerand, R. J., Mageau, G. A., Rousseau, F. L., & Provencher, P. (2004). When passion leads to problematic outcomes: A look at gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 20(2), 105119. https://doi.org/1050-5350/04/0600-0105/0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharma, V., & Sharma, A. (1998). The guilt and pleasure of masturbation: A study of college girls in Gujarat, India. Sexual and Marital Therapy, 13(1), 6370. https://doi.org/10.1080/02674659808406544CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sznycer, D. (2019). Forms and functions of the self-conscious emotions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(2), 143157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.11.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tangney, J. P., & Dearing, R. L. (2003). Shame and guilt. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P., Miller, R. S., Flicker, L., & Barlow, D. H. (1996). Are shame, guilt, and embarrassment distinct emotions? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(6), 12561269. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1256CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 345372. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070145CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., & Vaish, A. (2013). Origins of human cooperation and morality. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 231255. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143812CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, U., N'Diaye, K., Ethofer, T., & Vuilleumier, P. (2011). Guilt-specific processing in the prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 21(11), 24612470. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watanabe, E., & Ohtsubo, Y. (2012). Costly apology and self-punishment after an unintentional transgression. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 10(3), 87105. https://doi.org/10.1556/JEP.10.2012.3.1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xu, Z., Zhu, R., Zhang, S., Zhang, S., Liang, Z., Mai, X., & Liu, C. (2022). Mortality salience enhances neural activities related to guilt and shame when recalling the past. Cerebral Cortex, 32(22), 51455162. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhac004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yu, H., Hu, J., Hu, L., & Zhou, X. (2014). The voice of conscience: Neural bases of interpersonal guilt and compensation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(8), 11501158. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst090CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhu, R., Feng, C., Zhang, S., Mai, X., & Liu, C. (2019). Differentiating guilt and shame in an interpersonal context with univariate activation and multivariate pattern analyses. NeuroImage, 186, 476486. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.11.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhu, R., Jin, T., Shen, X., Zhang, S., Mai, X., & Liu, C. (2017). Relational utility affects self-punishment in direct and indirect reciprocity situations. Social Psychology, 48(1), 1927. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000291CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhu, R., Shen, X., Tang, H., Ye, P., Wang, H., Mai, X., & Liu, C. (2017). Self-punishment promotes forgiveness in the direct and indirect reciprocity contexts. Psychological Reports, 120(3), 408422. https://doi.org/10.1177/0033294117697087CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed