Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T08:46:45.045Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Involuntary memories are not déjà vu

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 November 2023

Sami Gülgöz
Affiliation:
Psychology Department, Koç University, Rumeli Feneri Yolu, Sariyer, Istanbul, Turkey [email protected] [email protected] kuram.ku.edu.tr
Irem Ergen
Affiliation:
Psychology Department, Koç University, Rumeli Feneri Yolu, Sariyer, Istanbul, Turkey [email protected] [email protected] kuram.ku.edu.tr

Abstract

The proposed framework can benefit from integrating predictive processing into the explanation of déjà vu which corresponds to interrupted prediction. Déjà vu is also accompanied by familiarity. However, considerable ambiguity is inherent in familiarity, which necessitates elaboration of this construct. Research findings on involuntary autobiographical memories and déjà vu show discrepancies, and clustering these constructs can be counterproductive for research.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

We believe that the authors have proposed a comprehensive approach to integrating the process of memory retrieval to account for both involuntary autobiographical memories (IAMs) and déjà vu experiences. In the current commentary, we focus on four points that may contribute to the discussion. These are: (a) The need to integrate predictive processing into the explanatory framework for déjà vu, (b) the need to further specify the concepts of weak activation and familiarity, (c) the findings conflicting with the idea of common origins for déjà vu and IAM, and (d) the possible detriments of clustering constructs.

In recent years, the concept of prediction emerged with high impact to the extent that some researchers consider the study of prediction a new paradigm (Hutchinson & Barrett, Reference Hutchinson and Barrett2019). Predictive processing emerges in areas of study from perception and imagery (Grush, Reference Grush2004) to visual recognition (Bar et al., Reference Bar, Kassam, Ghuman, Boshyan, Schmid, Dale and Halgren2006) and decision-making (Doya, Reference Doya2008). Cleary and Claxton (Reference Cleary and Claxton2018) suggested that déjà vu, too, could be a function of predictive processing. Even in novel circumstances, top-down processes seek contextual cues to past contexts to activate relevant past experiences. We use these past experiences or schema representations to predict current experiences. The déjà vu experience may happen when this prediction is erroneous. The mismatch between the prediction and the actuality would dissipate the fleeting feeling of familiarity, and the individual reports a déjà vu. Cleary et al. (Reference Cleary, Brown, Sawyer, Nomi, Ajoku and Ryals2021) emphasized the familiarity component and showed that not all déjà vu experiences are accompanied by a feeling of prediction, but the feeling of familiarity typically characterizes them. Predictive processing studies rely on self-report making them difficult to observe because prediction remains implicit in daily life, whereas déjà vu is defined by awareness.

Researchers employing event-related potentials to study familiarity more directly obtained varying results. For example, familiar faces show N250, whereas participants' own faces do not (Sommer et al., Reference Sommer, Stapor, Kończak, Kotowski, Fabian, Ochab and Ślusarczyk2021; but also Wiese et al., Reference Wiese, Hobden, Siilbek, Martignac, Flack, Ritchie and Burton2022). Caharel and Rossion (Reference Caharel and Rossion2021) showed that when long-term familiarity exists, the response is much sooner. Leynes and Upadhyay (Reference Leynes and Upadhyay2022) used familiar words and showed that responses changed depending on the context. Therefore, one must use caution when considering familiarity as a singular construct.

The concepts of familiarity and weak activation form the main tenets of the authors' hypotheses. These constructs require further elaboration because several factors may be influential, for example, insufficient cues, the inadequacy of cues, threshold for activation, or activation level. In addition, weak familiarity may arise because memory cells or ensembles have weak connectivity, cell clusters are partially damaged, or the representation is temporarily inaccessible (Josselyn & Tonegawa, Reference Josselyn and Tonegawa2020). If familiarity is a function of contextual cues, there must be a sufficient level of cues to trigger familiarity, but the configuration of these cues may not match the activated representation. We have also seen that schema activation and remembering specific memories cannot be distinguished easily in recalling past events (Ece & Gülgöz, Reference Ece and Gülgöz2021). In déjà vu, it is likely that the cues activate a schema rather than a specific event, and thus, an actual memory may not be available. In IAMs, on the other hand, what is remembered is a previously experienced specific event. It is also possible that déjà vu results from the hippocampus being unable to reinstate the original memory representation or to reconstruct it (Barry & Maguire, Reference Barry and Maguire2019; Frankland, Josselyn, & Köhler, Reference Frankland, Josselyn and Köhler2019). With IAMs, on the other hand, memory is reinstated without any deliberation.

Another point is the discrepancies between IAM and déjà vu findings. Berntsen (Reference Berntsen2010) proposed that IAM and voluntary recall share the same encoding and storage principles with four claims. First, the IAM recall experience is universal, and they are observed in most individuals (Ball & Little, Reference Ball and Little2006; Rubin & Berntsen, Reference Rubin and Berntsen2009), whereas Brown (Reference Brown2003) reported the rate of people experiencing déjà vu at least once in their lifetime to be 67%. Another discrepancy is the decrease in the frequency of déjà vu experience with age. No such decrease is observed in IAM experience (Berntsen et al., Reference Berntsen, Rubin and Salgado2015). One would expect that the likelihood of having a déjà vu would increase with experience and the possibility of an associative match, according to configural similarity and familiarity theories. Berntsen (Reference Berntsen2010) also proposed that IAM recall is frequent, with around 20 memories per day on average (Rasmussen & Berntsen, Reference Rasmussen and Berntsen2011), but déjà vu experience is very rare, a few times a year (Brown, Reference Brown2003). If familiarity increases déjà vu experience, then déjà vu experience should be more frequent because our environment is full of cues overlapping with our experience (Cleary et al., Reference Cleary, Brown, Sawyer, Nomi, Ajoku and Ryals2012; Cleary & Claxton, Reference Cleary and Claxton2018). Berntsen (Reference Berntsen2010) argued that involuntary memories belong to the same memory system as voluntary ones. However, IAMs differ in retrieval processes (Barzykowski & Staugaard, Reference Barzykowski and Staugaard2016). Déjà vu phenomenon is difficult to categorize within the memory system because it does not include any key processes observed in memory, such as encoding, maintenance, or retrieval. In déjà vu, what is lacking is not “access to content” (see Fig. 1 in the main article), but the content itself. Conversely, there is an actual recall of “content” in IAM. Therefore, déjà vu is merely a spontaneous and false belief of familiarity.

Although trying to explicate memory mechanisms within a single framework and to have a general model of retrieval is valuable, clustering qualitatively distinct processes as on a continuum may blind researchers to nuances in the underlying processes. There seem to be similar problems with other constructs. For example, false memory is an overarching term that subsumes memory implantation, recovered memories, imagination inflation, misinformation effect, and the Deese, Roediger and McDermott (DRM) task although there is evidence that they may be products of different mechanisms. Patihis, Frenda, and Loftus (Reference Patihis, Frenda and Loftus2018) studied the correlations between different false memory paradigms and observed no relationship. In order to avoid such pitfalls, we should focus on particular mechanisms rather than constructing general frameworks, at least for now.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Competing interest

None.

References

Ball, C. T., & Little, J. C. (2006). A comparison of involuntary autobiographical memory retrievals. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20(9), 11671179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bar, M., Kassam, K. S., Ghuman, A. S., Boshyan, J., Schmid, A. M., Dale, A. M., … Halgren, E. (2006). Top-down facilitation of visual recognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(2), 449454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barry, D. N., & Maguire, E. A. (2019). Remote memory and the hippocampus: A constructive critique. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(2), 128142.10.1016/j.tics.2018.11.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barzykowski, K., & Staugaard, S. R. (2016). Does retrieval intentionality really matter? Similarities and differences between involuntary memories and directly and generatively retrieved voluntary memories. British Journal of Psychology, 107(3), 519536.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berntsen, D. (2010). The unbidden past: Involuntary autobiographical memories as a basic mode of remembering. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(3), 138142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berntsen, D., Rubin, D. C., & Salgado, S. (2015). The frequency of involuntary autobiographical memories and future thoughts in relation to daydreaming, emotional distress, and age. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 352372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, A. S. (2003). A review of the déjà vu experience. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 394413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caharel, S., & Rossion, B. (2021). The N170 is sensitive to long-term (personal) familiarity of a face identity. Neuroscience, 458, 244255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cleary, A. M., Brown, A. S., Sawyer, B. D., Nomi, J. S., Ajoku, A. C., & Ryals, A. J. (2012). Familiarity from the configuration of objects in 3-dimensional space and its relation to déjà vu: A virtual reality investigation. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(2), 969975.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cleary, A. M., & Claxton, A. B. (2018). Déjà vu: An illusion of prediction. Psychological Science, 29(4), 635644.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doya, K. (2008). Modulators of decision making. Nature Neuroscience, 11(4), 410416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ece, B., & Gülgöz, S. (2021). Autobiographical memory for repeated events: Remembering our vacations. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 33(1), 111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frankland, P. W., Josselyn, S. A., & Köhler, S. (2019). The neurobiological foundation of memory retrieval. Nature Neuroscience, 22(10), 15761585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grush, R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(3), 377396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hutchinson, J. B., & Barrett, L. F. (2019). The power of predictions: An emerging paradigm for psychological research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(3), 280291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Josselyn, S. A., & Tonegawa, S. (2020). Memory engrams: Recalling the past and imagining the future. Science, 367(6473), eaaw4325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leynes, P. A., & Upadhyay, T. (2022). Context dissociations of the FN400 and N400 are evidence for recognition based on relative or absolute familiarity. Brain and Cognition, 162, 105903.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patihis, L., Frenda, S. J., & Loftus, E. F. (2018). False memory tasks do not reliably predict other false memories. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 5(2), 140.Google Scholar
Rasmussen, A. S., & Berntsen, D. (2011). The unpredictable past: Spontaneous autobiographical memories outnumber autobiographical memories retrieved strategically. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 18421846.10.1016/j.concog.2011.07.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, D. C., & Berntsen, D. (2009). The frequency of voluntary and involuntary autobiographical memories across the life span. Memory & Cognition, 37(5), 679688.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sommer, W., Stapor, K., Kończak, G., Kotowski, K., Fabian, P., Ochab, J., … Ślusarczyk, G. (2021). The N250 event-related potential as an index of face familiarity: A replication study. Royal Society Open Science, 8(6), 202356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiese, H., Hobden, G., Siilbek, E., Martignac, V., Flack, T. R., Ritchie, K. L., … Burton, A. M. (2022). Familiarity is familiarity is familiarity: Event-related brain potentials reveal qualitatively similar representations of personally familiar and famous faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 48(8), 11441164.Google ScholarPubMed