Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T08:36:12.129Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Attention and memory-driven effects in action studies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 January 2017

Philip Tseng
Affiliation:
Institute of Humanities in Medicine, Taipei Medical University, Taipei City 11031, Taiwan. [email protected]://medhuman1.tmu.edu.tw/philip/ Brain & Consciousness Research Center, Shuang-Ho Hospital, Taipei Medical University, New Taipei City 23561, Taiwan. [email protected]://chss.tmu.edu.tw/intro/super_pages.php?ID=intro3
Timothy Lane
Affiliation:
Institute of Humanities in Medicine, Taipei Medical University, Taipei City 11031, Taiwan. [email protected]://medhuman1.tmu.edu.tw/philip/ Brain & Consciousness Research Center, Shuang-Ho Hospital, Taipei Medical University, New Taipei City 23561, Taiwan. [email protected]://chss.tmu.edu.tw/intro/super_pages.php?ID=intro3 Institute of European and American Studies, Academia Sinica, Taipei City 11529, Taiwan Research Center for Mind, Brain, and Learning, National Chengchi University, Taipei City 11605, Taiwan
Bruce Bridgeman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California, Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, CA 95064. [email protected]://people.ucsc.edu/~bruceb/

Abstract

We provide empirical examples to conceptually clarify some items on Firestone & Scholl's (F&S's) checklist, and to explain perceptual effects from an attentional and memory perspective. We also note that action and embodied cognition studies seem to be most susceptible to misattributing attentional and memory effects as perceptual, and identify four characteristics unique to action studies and possibly responsible for misattributions.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Banerjee, P., Chatterjee, P. & Sinha, J. (2012) Is it light or dark? Recalling moral behavior changes perception of brightness. Psychological Science 23:407409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blaesi, S. & Bridgeman, B. (2015) Perceived difficulty of a motor task affects memory but not action. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics 77(3):972–77.Google Scholar
Cooper, A. D., Sterling, C. P., Bacon, M. P. & Bridgeman, B. (2012) Does action affect perception or memory? Vision Research 62:235–40.Google Scholar
Durgin, F. H., DeWald, D., Lechich, S., Li, Z. & Ontiveros, Z. (2011a) Action and motivation: Measuring perception or strategies? Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 18(6):1077–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fodor, J. A. & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1981) How direct is visual perception? Some reflections on Gibson's “ecological approach.Cognition 9(2):139–96.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1966) The senses considered as perceptual systems. Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1979) The ecological approach to perception and action. Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Graziano, M. S. A. & Botvinick, M. (2002) How the brain represents the body: Insights from neurophysiology and psychology. In: Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention and performance XIX, ed. Prinz, W. & Hommel, B., pp. 136–57. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Meier, B. P., Robinson, M. D., Crawford, L. E. & Ahlvers, W. J. (2007) When “light” and “dark” thoughts become light and dark responses: Affect biases brightness judgments. Emotion 7(2):366–76. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.366.Google Scholar
Tseng, P. & Bridgeman, B. (2011) Improved change detection with nearby hands. Experimental Brain Research 209:257–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tseng, P., Bridgeman, B. & Juan, C. H. (2012) Take the matter into your own hands: A brief review of the effect of nearby-hands on visual processing. Vision Research 72:7477.Google Scholar
Turvey, M. T., Shaw, R. E., Reed, E. S. & Mace, W. M. (1981) Ecological laws of perceiving and acting: In reply to Fodor and Pylyshyn (1981). Cognition 9(3):237304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed