Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:16:58.079Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neonate rats demonstrate high levels of retention for odour preference when a substantially reduced training trial of two minutes is used

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2023

RP Ogeil*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Psychiatry & Psychological Medicine, Monash University, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia
TM Edwards
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Psychiatry & Psychological Medicine, Monash University, Clayton, VIC 3800, Australia Tabor College, 222 Oban Road, PO Box 2223, Ringwood North, VIC 3134, Australia
*
* Contact for correspondence and requests for reprints: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

Neonate rats have played an important and unique role within the memory literature. Specifically, they are relatively naïve to experience, can be trained in an ethologically-valid way, using a single trial, which is not aversive and can demonstrate retention for at least 24 h, post-training. As such, they have provided salient insights into the biological mechanisms underlying the memory trace and brain development. The task of choice for rat pups is one using odour preference requiring the removal of each pup from the nest/dam for a ten-minute training trial. For such young animals this may reasonably lead to undue stress and the potential of subsequent rejection by the dam. Little research has considered whether the training duration could be substantially shortened to provide significant animal welfare benefits while maintaining, if not improving, task efficacy. This issue was addressed in the current paper using six-day old Wistar rats (n = 175) exposed to a single-trial, odour-preference task using either a standard ten-minute training trial or a shorter two-minute training trial. Exposing rat pups to the training odour for two minutes did not compromise the level of discrimination observed at test 24 h later. This finding suggests that significantly shorter training trials can be used without compromising retention levels at test. This not only has obvious welfare benefits, but may reasonably be considered to reduce pups’ stress levels which are known to alter both the strength and timing of the memory trace.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2009 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

References

Beylin, AV and Shors, TJ 2003 Glucocorticoids are necessary for enhancing the acquisition of associative memories after acute stressful experience. Hormones and Behavior 43: 124131CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crowe, SF, Ng, KT and Gibbs, ME 1991a Forebrain noradrenaline concentration following weakly reinforced training. Pharmacology, Biochemistry & Behavior 40: 173176CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowe, SF, Ng, KT and Gibbs, ME 1991b Possible noradrenergic involvement in training stimulus intensity. Pharmacology, Biochemistry & Behavior 39: 717722CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Vaus, JE, Gibbs, ME and Ng, KT 1980 Effects of social isolation on memory formation. Behavioral and Neural Biology 29: 473480CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbs, ME and Ng, KT 1979 Behavioural stages in memory formation. Neuroscience Letters 13: 279283CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gulpinar, MA and Yegen, BC 2004 The physiology of learning and memory: Role of peptides and stress. Current Protein and Peptide Science 5: 457473CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaneko, WM, Riley, EP and Ehlers, CL 1996 Effects of artificial rearing on electrophysiology and behaviour in adult rats. Depression and Anxiety 4: 2792883.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhn, CM and Schanberg, SM 1998 Responses to maternal separation: mechanisms and mediators. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience 16: 267270CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moriceau, S and Sullivan, RM 2005 Neurobiology of infant attachment. Developmental Psychobiology 47: 230242CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ng, KT, Gibbs, ME, Crowe, SF, Sedman, GL, Hua, F, Zhao, W, O’Dowd, B, Rickard, N, Gibbs, CL, Sykova, E, Svoboda, J and Jendelova, P 1991 Molecular mechanisms of memory formation. Molecular Neurobiology 5: 333350CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
NHMRC 2004 Australian Code of Practice for the Care and Use of Animals for Scientific Purposes, Seventh Edition. Australian Government Publishing Services: Canberra, AustraliaGoogle Scholar
Ogeil, RP and Edwards, TM 2007 The development of a single trial odour discrimination task for neonate rats. Australian Journal of Psychology 59(1): 322Google Scholar
Quiroz, C, Martinez, I, Quirarte, GL, Morales, T, Diaz-Cintra, S and Prado-Alcala, RA 2003 Enhanced inhibitory avoidance learning prevents the memory-impairing effects of post-training hippocampal inactivation. Experimental Brain Research 153: 400402CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, WA 1979 Memory for lists of spatial events in the rat. Learning & Motivation 10: 117140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roman, E, Hyytiä, P and Nylander, I 2003 Maternal separation alters acquisition of ethanol intake in male ethanol-preferring AA rats. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 27: 3137CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose, SPR 1991 How chicks make memories: The cellular cascade from c-fos to dendritic remodelling. Trends in Neuroscience 14: 390397CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose, SPR 2000 God's organism? The chick as a model system for memory studies. Learning & Memory 7: 117CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, RM and Leon, M 1986 Early olfactory learning induces an enhanced olfactory bulb response in young rats. Developmental Brain Research 27: 278282CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sullivan, RM, McGaugh, JL and Leon, M 1991 Norepinephrine-induced plasticity and one-trial olfactory learning in neonatal rats. Developmental Brain Research 60: 219228CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, RM and Wilson, DA 1994 The locus coeruleus, norepinephrine, and memory in newborns. Brain Research Bulletin 35: 467472CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, RM, Wilson, DA and Leon, M 1989 Associative processes in early olfactory preference acquisition: Neural and behavioral consequences. Psychobiology 17: 2933CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, RM and Wilson, DA 2003 Molecular biology of early olfactory memory. Learning & Memory 10: 14CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yerkes, RM and Dodson, JD 1908 The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology 18: 459482CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, WN, Murphy, CA and Feldon, J 2004 Behavioural and cadiovascular responses during latent inhibition of conditioned fear: Measurement by telemetry and conditioned freezing. Behavioural Brain Research 154: 199209CrossRefGoogle Scholar