We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
To save content items to your account,
please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies.
If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account.
Find out more about saving content to .
To save content items to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected]
is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings
on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part
of your Kindle email address below.
Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations.
‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi.
‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Since the 1980s, the level of activism of regions in European Union policy-making has greatly increased, leading to the emergence of claims that regional governments can and do bypass national government in European negotiations. However, two decades after the emergence of the concept, the debate about the ability of regions to engage successfully in this process of continuous negotiation and to represent their interests on the European stage is ongoing. Due to the scarcity of research looking at regional interest representation in concrete cases of policy-making, it has been difficult to establish to what extent and under which circumstances regions do rely on unmediated channels of interest representation on the European level. This article examines these questions through the activities of seven legislative regions during two negotiations of European Directives, as legislative regions have a wider choice of channels of interest representation. Overall, extensive use of unmediated access in regulatory policy-making is rare and can best be explained with reference to domestic conflict and the level of influence of a region in domestic European policy-making. Differences in the size of a region also influence the ability of a region to represent its interests in the coordination of the national position and at the European level.
In recent years, deliberative democracy has moved from a philosophical ideal into an empirical theory with numerous experiments testing the theoretical assumptions. Despite the wealth of evidence on the potential for deliberation, scholars have remained hesitant to test the theoretical premises under rather more adverse circumstances. This article, in contrast, tries to push deliberative scholarship to its edge by focusing on the viability of citizen deliberation in deeply divided societies. Our research questions are whether contact between citizens of competing segments undermines the potential for deliberation, and under which institutional conditions this is so. Based on a deliberative experiment in Belgium, in which we varied the group composition and the decision-making rule, we argue that decision rules are strong predictors of deliberative quality, but more importantly that the confrontation between citizens from both sides of the divide does not undermine the quality of deliberation. On the contrary even, our results indicate that the quality of intergroup deliberation is higher than that of intragroup deliberation, no matter what the rule.
Media coverage of elections in Europe and North America has increasingly focused on the campaign as a game rather than a policy debate. This is often explained by the changes in media pressures. It may also reflect the narrowing of policy space between left and right and the comparative prosperity enjoyed in Europe and North America. But the relevance of policy varies. The global economic crisis might have led to an increased interest in policy among voters and focus on it by media. Ireland experienced both extremes of boom and crisis between the late 1990s and 2011. The Irish case allows us to test the impact of the crisis on media framing of elections. This article uses original data from the three most recent national elections in Ireland, with a research design that holds other pertinent variables constant. We find empirical support for the theoretical expectation that the context of the election affects the relative focus on campaign or horserace vs. substantive policy issues.
Political scientists have started to focus on ‘practice’ as the smallest unit of analysis. Following a broader turn in the social sciences, the practice focus provides multiple advantages, including better conceptualizations of short-term social change, getting closer to the everyday activities of those speaking, writing and doing politics, appropriate conceptualization of agency-structure dynamics, or forms of analysis resonating with other communities than scholarly ones. This contribution asks what the methodological implications of the practice turn are. It is argued that the practice focus does not only imply a certain ‘theory’ but also a certain methodology. I advance the term praxiography to speak about the forms of analysis produced by practice researchers. I discuss key guidelines of praxiographic research on two levels: first, general research strategies that provide empirical access points, second, guidelines for data collection in the frame of participant observation, expert interviews, and document analysis. I conclude in arguing that although praxiography is context driven, and hence requires to be tailored to the research problem, it is vital to reflect on the methodological repertoire of praxiographic research.
Analyses European Muslim communities' developing involvement in their political environment and related Muslim and public debates. Muslims are increasingly making themselves noticed in the political process of Europe. But what is happening behind the often sensational headlines? This book looks at the processes and realities of Muslim participation in local and national politics: voting patterns in local and national assemblies and the tensions between ethnic, political and religious identities. These developments drive internal Muslim debates including attitudes to the democratic processes and whether Muslims should take part at all, as well as rivalries over who should represent and speak for Muslims. They also inspire sharp European discussion about Muslim political participation - does it signal integration or separation? - and how the European states should view this increasingly active role of Muslims in the public space.
The preceding chapter has discussed the relationship between discourse and politics. It has outlined the crucial and original contribution of discourse analysis in linguistics and social psychology to the study of political language. In this chapter the discussion is broadened to how social and political psychologists have traditionally approached the issue of persuasive communications, and some examples are given of how they treat language and rhetoric. The remainder of the chapter includes a discursive analysis of two selected aspects of political rhetoric: the use of metaphors and identification with an audience. The chapter closes with a discussion of the crucial task of moving towards a genuine political psychology of political rhetoric by anchoring it in the detailed study of the public use of language.
It is perhaps a truism to affirm that in politics, as in other realms of social life, rhetorical commitment and debate are necessary ingredients. Arguably, the political importance of rhetoric and dialogue is a position that does not often require justification. What does require justification is the way in which one thinks about and approaches it empirically. The term ‘political rhetoric’ refers both to the ways in which politicians try to persuade various audiences and to the (academic) study of such oratory (see Billig, 2003; Condor et al., in press). This chapter approaches political rhetoric in the spirit that Aristotle (trans. 1909) first championed. This is an analytic spirit, where the focus is on discovering ‘the available means of persuasion in each case’ (p. 5). This chapter suggests that the phenomenon of political persuasion ‘calls for a psychological approach that is itself rooted within the study of rhetoric’ (Billig, 2003, p. 223).
The American political scientist Charles E. Merriam described psychology as a ‘kindred’ science (Merriam, 1924). McGuire (1993) writes about the ‘long affair’ between psychology and political science underpinned by frequent transformation of topics, procedures and theories. What Merriam and McGuire have in common is that they understand the relationship between psychology and politics as the study of ‘political behaviour’. A variety of ‘definitions’ of this relationship has been suggested. For example, Sears et al. (2003) see the relationship between psychology and politics as the ‘application of what is known about human psychology to the study of politics’ (p. 3). For others, it is about discerning how ‘human cognition and emotion mediate the impact of the environment on political action’ (Stein, 2002, p. 108). According to Lavine (2010), the relationship is ‘defined by a bidirectional influence: just as the psyche influences political orientation, the polity leaves its mark on who we are’ (p. xx, emphasis in original).
This book does not attempt to offer yet another definition. Instead, it tries to qualify the relationship between psychology and politics by proposing alternative approaches, different conceptual tools and a different vision of human psychology and political behaviour with roots in epistemological, theoretical and methodological presuppositions arising from the discursive (Billig, 1987; Harré and Gillett, 1994; Middleton and Edwards, 1990), narrative (Bruner, 1986; Polkinghorne, 1988) and sociocultural (Middleton and Brown, 2005; Valsiner, 2007; Wertsch, 2002) turns in psychology, the human and the social sciences, giving rise to what can be broadly termed an interpretive political psychology. An interpretive political psychology suggests that political psychologists can attain a deep level of understanding of political behaviour by researching different social and political orders – discursive, cultural and semiotic – in their own terms. When political psychologists research attitudes, racism, public opinion, political ideology, and so on, they are, arguably, describing universalistic and particularistic presuppositions of modern culture. An interpretive political psychology likens the work of the political psychologist to that of the anthropologist who uncovers the various meaning-making layers through which society is organised and reproduces itself (Moscovici, 1972).
As the preceding pages have shown, one can gain a deeper understanding of political behaviour and the strength and utility of political psychology by emphasising its diversity of perspectives. Of course, the global world of political psychology extends beyond the boundaries of Europe and North America. Issues, topics, innovations in political psychology are not limited to what European academics and their North American colleagues choose to study. Nor are they limited to psychological issues. Around the world, new and creative ways of understanding the different manifestations of political behaviour are being developed: some are simply borrowing the models and the tools of their more prestigious American colleagues; others proceed independently, developing critiques, finding new gaps and imagining new research tools and hypotheses more suited to researching local social and political contexts. One of the major challenges of political psychology rests with how best to promote alternative ways of doing political psychology.
In its search for integrated and integrative perspectives, contemporary political psychology (especially in North America) is preoccupied with devising new technologies of research that can potentially change or transform the i eld. There is nothing wrong with this approach. The conceptual tools of cognitive science, evolutionary science, genetics, or the tools of neuroscience are pushing political psychology in new exciting directions. But problems can arise when this approach is used to predict and prescribe the future of political psychology. There is a lot of truth in Helen Haste’s statement: ‘predicting the future is hazardous; prescribing the future is a doomed exercise’ (2012, p. 1). It remains to be seen whether the future of political psychology lies with a dialogue with cognitive science, evolutionary science, or the neurosciences, especially when these approaches are drawn upon uncritically. This dialogue can potentially turn political psychology into a system governed by the problems and priorities of other fields. What we can be sure of, nonetheless, is that, as political psychologists, we can always turn to the lives of ‘concrete’ human beings, to describing and interpreting their social practices, social interactions, motivations, representations, as they appear to them in their full contingency.
Personality, political behaviour and the predisposition to intolerance
When political psychologists turn to the study of intolerance, they seem to be mostly preoccupied with what Allport has called ‘the horizontal dimensions that run through all individuals’ (1962, p. 409). Allport was writing about what psychologists routinely refer to as traits, predispositions, cognitions and motives that describe the ‘personality’ of individuals. What Allport described using a spatial metaphor is described by contemporary political psychology in not so different terms, as a ‘multifaceted and enduring internal, or psychological, structure’ (Mondak, 2010, p. 6). This chapter argues that although the influences of Allport’s ‘horizontal dimensions’ are some of the most discernible and significant, they are not the sole determinant of intolerance. This chapter contends that the nature of intolerance (and associated phenomena such as racism and moral exclusion) cannot be reduced to relatively stable inner predispositions or basic personality dispositions, and that the leaning of political psychologists to elucidate the general regularities of political behaviour can be profitably balanced, complemented by attention to culture, language, social interaction and the actual ways in which intolerance is enacted and accomplished by different ways of talking and behaving towards others.
In order to develop this argument this chapter discusses the appeal and manifestations of right-wing extremism in Western Europe, prejudice as collaborative accomplishment, and the extreme, moral exclusionary discourse against the Roma minority in Eastern Europe. One cannot understand fully the plural and contextual manifestations of intolerance if one only studies it as a predisposition underpinned by an authoritarian mindset. Intolerance needs to be studied in its own right – as it manifests itself, and as it is interpreted and enacted by social and political actors in social interaction and social practices. Intolerance is imbued with a variety of sociocultural meanings; it is the foundation and product of social activities.
The collective will and the ‘ideal’ democratic citizen
‘We, the people, feel and know that we have become more significant than ever before, with the narrowing of the barrier that separates “us” and our range of experiences from our elected representatives and their range of experiences.’ This is what social psychologist, Hadley Cantril, in his 1942 paper, ‘Public Opinion in Flux’, was writing about the importance of ‘good morale’ in American democracy, especially ‘national morale’ associated with the war effort. What Cantril acknowledged in 1942 (and he was not the only one) is what politicians, ‘spin doctors’, and so on take for granted today: the fundament of democracy lies in the ‘faith in the judgment of the common man’. Cantril was writing about the person, the ‘citizen’ who ‘given sufficient facts and motivated to pay attention to those facts … will reach a decision based on his [her] own self-interest as a member of a democratic community’ (1942, p. 151). When writing about ‘we, the people’ Cantril points to the direction of political democratic accountability (from citizens to their elected representatives) and thus brings into the foreground one of the most fundamental political hopes – that the will and reason of ‘the people’ ought to prevail. Cantril’s words express faith in the self-governing, autonomous and omnicompetent citizen (Dalton, 2008) – the ‘ideal’ democratic citizen.
This chapter shows how political psychologists’ concern with the ‘collective will’ is paralleled by a concern with, search for and description of the democratic citizen. The first part of the chapter maps the various meanings and expressions of this collective will condensed into the notion of ‘public opinion’. The chapter then goes on to describe the main assumptions behind researching and understanding the democratic competence of citizens, especially those related to political knowledge and political sophistication.
The spectacle of contemporary politics around the world is intimately bound to a ‘multiaxial’ (Delli Carpini and Williams, 2001) media and communication environment. The nature of contemporary political communication is in continuous transformation (Bennett and Iyengar, 2008). This chapter offers a summary of the main tenets of a discursive approach to political communication. It starts by charting the strengths and weaknesses of contemporary psychology of political communication. The chapter then moves on to discuss how political communication can be conceived of as a social accomplishment, and an outcome of, as well as influence on, complex forms and networks of social practices. After offering some empirical examples drawn from work on the ethnography of political processes, discursive research on politicians’ communicative style, political advertising and political humour, the chapter closes with an outline of an alternative approach to political communication that relies specifically on the importance of investigating how political communications are actually produced, circulated and consumed in society. In doing so, this chapter argues that political psychologists can borrow creatively and learn from media, communication and discourse theorists interested in the complexity of political communications.
The rise of ‘self-expressive politics’ (Stanyer, 2007), the increased ‘personalisation’ (Castells, 2011) and ‘professionalisation’ of politics and political communication (Negrine, 2008; Wodak, 2011) are only some examples of how political phenomena do not exist outside communication processes, outside information and communications of and about politics. Politics and political processes need to be ‘packaged’ (Cappella and Jamieson, 1997; Franklin, 2004) in some communicative form or other in order to reach imagined, proximal or distal audiences. In most Western and Eastern European democracies, this is usually the job of politicians themselves, the mass media, ‘spin doctors’ and the increasingly powerful political public relations industry. Their role is to construct, direct, circulate and disseminate political communications (McNair, 2011).
Mass subjectivity and the democratic competence of nations
As argued in Chapter 1, citizens and publics inhabit ‘a political environment where they are continually encouraged by various actors to vocalize their views’ (Stanyer, 2007, p. 157). Some of the ‘actors’ to which Stanyer refers are academic psychologists, experts in public opinion polling who are interested in the aggregate description and distribution of the various dimensions of social and political behaviour: attitudes, motives, preferences, wishes, value orientations, and so on. Value orientations are perhaps the most important to understanding issues around political attitudes, social and political change, and political participation (Inglehart, 2009).
This chapter sketches the various attempts at describing the universal psychological structure of human values and the implications of such attempts for understanding democracy promotion and democratisation. It explores the tension between aggregate and universalistic models and contingent, contextual, particularistic manifestations of political behaviour. By focusing on the issue of questioning democracy promotion, this chapter shows how value orientations cannot be satisfactorily conceptualised outside an inter-subjective framework and ordinary ways of reasoning about values. The promotion of ‘formal democracy’ cannot work outside the framework of mass values offered by the existing socio-political context. The chapter ends by arguing that values should be treated as ideologically and culturally situated argumentative resources, and that researchers should emphasise not only their universalistic, but also their particularistic features, especially the fragmentary, multiple and unfinished nature of value searches, value expressions and value orientations.
From the archive model of memory to lived experience
Memory is at the centre of human experience. Memory is what makes us human. The past is a site of social meaning. These are statements with which the majority of psychologists agree. The truth of these statements rests on two fundamental questions: (a) how to study memory by incorporating the tension and interplay between preservation and loss, remembering and forgetting, the relationship between memory, identity and narrative: and (b) how to reconcile the distinction between memory as individual faculty, and memory as collective or social phenomenon. This chapter outlines some of the issues that arise from various attempts to find answers to these questions, especially those with particular relevance to political psychology. This chapter presents the main tenets of a sociocultural approach to researching social memory, with an emphasis on political narratives, commemoration and national memory of socio-political events and coming to terms with the past. The chapter ends with a brief outline of implications (and recommendations) for a political psychology of collective memory.
In his book The Sense of an Ending, British novelist Julian Barnes writes: ‘As the witnesses to your life diminish, there is less corroboration, and therefore less certainty as to what you are or have been.’ The quotation expresses, in a nutshell, the contingency of selfhood. What it intimates is that biographical time does not correspond exactly to biographical reality, but to the multiple reconstructions of the unfolding past/time by people. What Barnes has found, as so many of us have, is that what we call ‘individual memory’ about one’s life appears only apparently as a ‘property’ of the self. Instead we find it distributed beyond one’s own person, ‘beyond one’s head’ (Bruner, 2001), as it were, and mediated by personal and social relationships, and the material environment. Our relationship to the past and others is an unfinished business. Our memories (and identities) are not essences we carry within us, but rather a result of particular configurations and constellations of the subject in relation to networks of distributed and mediated activities. Our memories are located within mental, material and cultural spaces.