Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T08:48:34.960Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - What young children's understanding of contamination and contagion tells us about their concepts of illness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 July 2010

Michael Siegal
Affiliation:
University of Sheffield
Candida C. Peterson
Affiliation:
University of Queensland
Get access

Summary

Consider the range of theories and beliefs that people bring to bear when reasoning about illness. At a minimum, illness can be thought of in biological, social, psychological, moral/religious, and probabilistic terms. Various aspects of illness maybe understood to be embedded in a number of explanatory contexts or theories. Any one “fact” about illness may involve a number of theories. For example there are several (partial) explanations for why sick children do not go to school. There is a biological rationale: sick people may be contagious and so may contaminate others they contact. There is also a social prohibition. It is a social norm that sick people are released from certain obligations and enjoined to others: one gets to stay home and should avoid making others ill. There also may be a psychological rationale: sick people feel bad and weak and so do not want to go to school.

Conceptions of illness provide researchers an excellent opportunity to study children's understandings of different domains of experience. To what extent, for example, do young children distinguish biological from social or psychological aspects of illness experience? This question has taken on particular importance in light of ongoing debates about the development of biological knowledge. Susan Carey and colleagues (Carey, 1985a, 1995; Solomon et al, 1996) have argued that a differentiated notion of biology does not emerge until middle childhood. Other researchers suggest that some forms of biological knowledge are present much earlier (e.g., Simons and Keil, 1995; Wellman and Gelman, 1992).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1999

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×