Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T13:58:39.008Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Serving of free school lunch to secondary-school pupils – a pilot study with health implications

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2009

Anne Selvik Ask
Affiliation:
Faculty of Health and Sport, Agder University, Kristiansand, Norway
Sigrunn Hernes
Affiliation:
Faculty of Health and Sport, Agder University, Kristiansand, Norway
Ingebjørg Aarek
Affiliation:
Faculty of Health and Sport, Agder University, Kristiansand, Norway
Frøydis Vik
Affiliation:
Faculty of Health and Sport, Agder University, Kristiansand, Norway
Cornelia Brodahl
Affiliation:
Faculty of Engineering and Science, Agder University, Kristiansand, Norway
Margaretha Haugen*
Affiliation:
Faculty of Health and Sport, Agder University, Kristiansand, Norway Department of Food Safety and Nutrition, Division of Environmental Medicine, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, PO Box 4404 Nydalen, N-0403 Oslo, Norway
*
*Corresponding author: Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

To study whether service of a free school lunch has an impact on weight development and food intake among pupils at a lower secondary school, and to assess the association between self-perceived school behaviour and food intake.

Design

A controlled intervention study involving service of a free healthy school lunch to 9th grade pupils took place over 4 months, from January to May 2007. Weight and height were measured before and after the intervention. The pupils also completed a short FFQ and a questionnaire concerning self-perceived school behaviour and the classroom environment before and after the intervention. A healthy food score was calculated using the FFQ data.

Setting

All 9th graders at three different lower secondary schools in southern Norway were invited to participate. One school was randomly selected as the intervention school.

Subjects

Fifty-eight pupils (91 %) from the intervention school and ninety-two pupils (77 %) from the control schools participated.

Results

BMI did not increase among the girls at the intervention school, but increased significantly among the boys at the intervention school and among the control school groups. The healthy food score correlated positively with the trait ‘satisfied with schoolwork’ (P < 0·001). Fifteen per cent of the variance in food score could be explained by gender and the trait ‘satisfied with schoolwork’.

Conclusions

Serving of a healthy free school lunch to secondary-school pupils may result in restricted weight gain. Further studies are needed to clarify the impact of school meals on overweight and academic performance.

Type
Research Paper
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2009

A major public health crisis will emerge in many Western countries if steps are not taken to stop the increase in child and youth obesity(Reference Dehghan, Akhtar-Danesh and Merchant1, Reference Varela-Moreiras2). In Norway, overweight/obesity in schoolchildren has increased from 9·8 % in 1993 to 12·3 % in 2000 among 8th graders, and an overall increase in fat mass has been demonstrated(Reference Andersen, Lillegaard, Overby, Lytle, Klepp and Johansson3, Reference Juliusson, Roelants, Eide, Hauspie, Waaler and Bjerknes4). Several governmental strategies have been tried, most of which have been educational and informational. In 2007, a national action plan was launched by the Norwegian government(5) to improve the food intake of the entire population. The action plan discusses the introduction of a free school lunch. We have previously shown that serving a healthy breakfast to lower secondary-school pupils improves overall diet and reduces weight gain(Reference Ask, Hernes, Aarek, Johannessen and Haugen6). Several studies have confirmed the importance of a healthy breakfast in relation to adolescents’ overall nutrient profile and academic performance(Reference Matthys, De Henauw, Bellemans, De Maeyer and De Backer7, Reference Rampersaud, Pereira, Girard, Adams and Metzl8), but no study has reported on lunch introduction in an affluent society. In Norway, pupils usually bring packed lunches from home, because no school lunch is provided by the government(Reference Andersen, Nes, Sandstad, Bjorneboe and Drevon9, Reference Makela, Kjaernes, Pipping, L’orange, Gronow and Holm10). It has been observed among lower secondary-school pupils that packed lunches from home often are thrown away(Reference Andersen, Nes, Sandstad, Bjorneboe and Drevon9, Reference Samuelson11). Pupils instead buy fast food, snacks or sweets from a local kiosk or the school canteen. School canteens are often run by the pupils themselves, and most commonly offer baguettes, waffles, milk (regular or chocolate), juice, cakes and, perhaps, fruit. Accordingly, serving a healthy school lunch has the potential to improve the diets of pupils and the social environmental setting of schools(Reference Lytle, Kubik, Perry, Story, Birnbaum and Murray12Reference Florence, Asbridge and Veugelers14). Academic performance has been explored in several studies concerning breakfast intake, while only two studies have looked at school performance and quality of the entire diet and meal pattern(Reference Florence, Asbridge and Veugelers14, Reference Kim, Frongillo, Han, Oh, Kim, Jang, Won, Lee and Kim15). It is therefore of interest to evaluate if academic performance can be correlated to dietary intake and meal pattern.

The aims of the present study were to evaluate whether serving a free healthy lunch would improve the dietary quality and impact on weight gain among 9th grade pupils. We also wanted to evaluate whether there is an association between the pupils’ food intake and their self-perceived school behaviour and performance.

Methods

Study design

A controlled intervention study was conducted in which a healthy school lunch was served to 9th grade pupils. The study was carried out on 9th grade pupils because pupils at this level have three home economics lessons per week, and lunch preparation was a part of the syllabus. The intervention period was 4 months. Before the study started in January 2007, and after it was completed in May 2007, the pupils answered two short questionnaires during a lesson: an FFQ and a questionnaire concerning self-perceived school behaviour and the classroom environment. Weight and height were also measured. Moreover, the teachers were asked to answer seven questions on class behaviour in an attempt to secure an objective evaluation of changes in behaviour following the service of lunch. Informed written consent was obtained from all parents and pupils. The study was approved by the Regional Ethical Committee and by the National Data Inspectorate.

Subjects

All 184 pupils in the 9th grade at three different lower secondary schools in southern Norway were invited to participate in an intervention study involving free lunches. One hundred and fifty of the pupils chose to participate. One school was randomly selected as the intervention school. At this school, sixty-four pupils were available and sixty-one gave a written consent to participate in the measurements. Three pupils were unavailable at the time of re-examination, resulting in a total of fifty-eight pupils, with a participation rate of 91 %. Of the 119 eligible pupils at the control schools, ninety-five chose to participate but three pupils were not present at the final evaluations, resulting in ninety-two participating control pupils (participation rate 77 %). All sixty-four pupils at the intervention school participated in the lunch programme, and all expressed their satisfaction with the project. Pupils who did not complete the FFQ satisfactorily before and after the intervention were not included in the statistical analyses on food score. This left fifty-three pupils from the intervention school and eighty-eight pupils from the control schools for assessment of the intervention. Five teachers at the intervention school and ten teachers at the control schools chose to complete the teacher’s questionnaire twice.

Measurements

Working in teams of two, five researchers carried out the weight and height examinations. The weight measurements were performed on the same Seca® personal scale and height was measured using a Seca® stadiometer, with pupils being asked to stand up straight. BMI (kg/m2) was calculated, and the International Obesity Taskforce gender-specific cut-offs at age 15 years were used to determine overweight and obesity(Reference Cole, Bellizzi, Flegal and Dietz16).

Food score

The FFQ consisted of three questions on bread consumption, eleven questions on liquid intake, eight questions on different dinner alternatives and four questions on snacks and sweets. Bread intake frequency ranged from never to seven or more times daily. Seven frequencies were used for the questions on drinks and solid foods including never/seldom, weekly and daily. Eleven food items from the FFQ were chosen as the basis for a healthy food score. Intake of bread (half- and wholemeal), low-fat milk, boiled potatoes, vegetables and fruit was given a high score, whereas frequent intake of white bread, full-fat milk, soft drinks, sweets and chocolates was given a low score. Frequent intake of healthy food items was given the highest score (18), and frequent intake of unhealthy food items was given the lowest score (Table 1). Missing data were given a score of 0. The lowest and highest possible scores if pupils filled in the questionnaire correctly were 41 and 135, respectively.

Table 1 Scores given to intake of eleven food items; the scores were added to calculate a healthy food score

Lunch preparation and serving

The offered lunch consisted of wholemeal bread, different kinds of cheese, cold cuts of lean meat, fish and jam. Low-fat milk and fresh fruit and vegetables were also served. The pupils sliced the bread, arranged the cold cuts on serving trays, cut up the fruit and vegetables and served the milk under the supervision of the home economics teacher. They consumed the lunch in their own classroom together with teachers.

Assessment of school behaviour and environment

A questionnaire concerning self-perceived school behaviour, the school environment and achievement was answered on the same day as the FFQ(Reference Roland and Galloway17). The questions were divided into six main categories, covering friendship and support, relationship with classmates, own behaviour and experience with schoolwork, experience and relationship with teachers, and obedience. Each category was divided into between four and seven sub-questions. We also asked the pupils to state their grades in home economics, Norwegian, mathematics and social sciences.

The pupils’ school behaviour characteristics were derived using factor analysis (principal component method with Varimax rotation), based on thirty-three questions from the school environment questionnaire. School behaviour characteristics were derived at the start of the study for all 150 pupils, as follows: using principal component analysis, the data were reduced by forming linear combinations of the original observed variables. Correlated variables were grouped together. The coefficients defining these linear combinations are called ‘factor loadings’ and are actually the correlations of each item with the relevant factor. The factors that best represented the data were chosen on the basis of eigenvalues and the understanding of the factor loadings. Varimax rotation redistributed the explained factor variance in a way that a simpler structure was achieved. We considered the factors for which the loading coefficient was higher than 0·2 or lower than −0·2, as these values roughly correspond to a statistical significance of P = 0·05. Factor scores were created by multiplying factor loadings with the corresponding standardized value for each item, and then summing across the items. For each pupil, the factor scores indicate the extent to which the attitude conforms to the respective patterns. A negative (or positive) factor score means that the behaviour characteristics are inversely (or positively) correlated with the factor.

Statistical analysis

Data are presented as means and standard deviations for weight, BMI and food score. Differences between pre- and post-intervention food score, weight and BMI were assessed using a paired t-test for continuous variables and a Wilcoxon signed-rank test for nominal variables and repeated-measures ANOVA. The Spearman correlation coefficient was used to correlate school behaviour traits with grades and food score. Multiple linear regression analyses were used to investigate the impact of school behaviour traits on food score before study start. The analyses were thoroughly checked for possible violations of the model assumptions. The Spearman correlation coefficient was also used to evaluate reproducibility of the attitude traits before and after the lunch intervention period. All of the statistical tests were two-sided, and P values below 0·05 were considered significant. All statistical analyses were performed using the SPSS statistical software package version 14·0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

Results

At study start, 15 % of the seventy-four participating boys were overweight and 1 % (one case) was obese. Of the seventy-six participating girls, 13 % were overweight while none was obese. In the intervention school, 18 % of the boys and 16 % of the girls were overweight. Thirteen per cent of the boys and 11 % of the girls were overweight in the control schools. However, there were no statistically significant differences in BMI or height between male and female pupils in either the intervention school or the control schools at study start (Mann–Whitney U test). Both the weight and the BMI of boys at the intervention school increased significantly during the intervention period (both P < 0·001), while weight (P = 0·043), but not BMI, increased significantly among the girls there (Tables 2 and 3). In the control group, both weight and BMI increased significantly among the boys (both P < 0·001) and the girls (P < 0·001 and P = 0·017, respectively). Two overweight girls, one in each group, could be reclassified as having a normal weight after the intervention, while two boys in the intervention group and three boys in the control group went from normal weight to overweight during the study period. We found no overall effect of the intervention on weight or BMI (Tables 2 and 3) using repeated-measures ANOVA.

Table 2 Results for 4 months of serving a healthy school lunch to male 9th graders, Norway, January–May 2007

Mean values were significantly different from those before the intervention (Wilcoxon signed-rank test): *P < 0·05, ***P < 0·001.

F statistic for interaction of school and before and after intervention results (repeated-measures ANOVA).

Table 3 Results for 4 months of serving a healthy school lunch to female 9th graders, Norway, January–May 2007

Mean values were significantly different from those before the intervention (Wilcoxon signed-rank test): *P < 0·05, **P < 0·01, ***P < 0·001.

F statistic for interaction of school and before and after intervention results (repeated-measures ANOVA).

At study start, the girls in both groups had a significantly higher healthy food score than the boys (P = 0·003), but this difference disappeared after the intervention period. A reduced food score, which implies less frequent intake of healthy food items and/or more frequent intake of unhealthy food items, was observed after the intervention period for both the intervention and the control groups, and for both girls and boys (Tables 2 and 3). For boys, food score correlated positively with grades in Norwegian (Spearman correlation coefficient, r S = 0·37; P < 0·01), social sciences (r S = 0·35; P < 0·01) and home economics (r S = 0·26; P < 0·05). No correlation between food score and grades was observed for girls.

Weekly intake of breakfast, lunch and dinner by the intervention group did not change to a statistically significant degree. Significantly fewer pupils at the control schools ate breakfast at the end of the study period (P = 0·002), but the frequency of packed lunch brought from home and intake of dinner did not change. At study start, the frequency of breakfast was negatively correlated with BMI among boys (P < 0·05), while lunch frequency was not observed to have any effect on BMI.

We identified three main school attitudes among the pupils. Table 4 presents the factor loading matrix for these three main traits. Trait 1 (positive) characterized pupils who enjoyed schoolwork regardless of other pupils’ views and scored highly on satisfaction with schoolwork. Trait 2 (obedience) characterized well-behaved children, while trait 3 (mischief) characterized obstinate pupils who scored highly on popularity with classmates. The three traits identified at study start correlated well with the same traits 4 months later: for trait 1, r S = 0·67; for trait 2, r S = 0·60; and for trait 3, r S = 0·63 (P < 0·001 for all). Trait 1 correlated positively with grades for mathematics (r S = 0·39; P < 0·001), social sciences (r S = 0·63; P < 0·001) and home economics (r S = 0·39; P < 0·001). Trait 3 correlated positively with grades in home economics (r S = 0·25; P = 0·004). Trait 1 correlated positively with food score (r S = 0·36; P < 0·001). Using a linear regression model, 15 % of the variance in food score could be explained by gender and trait 1 (Table 5). Neither of the two other traits correlated significantly with food score.

Table 4 Factor loading matrix for the three main traits identified by principal component analysis among male and female 9th graders, Norway, January–May 2007

Table 5 Food score as a function of gender and school pattern (r 2 = 0·15, P < 0·001) among male and female 9th graders, Norway, January–May 2007. Beta values are unstandardized coefficients

The teachers’ pupil-behaviour assessment scores were significantly higher for the intervention school than for the control schools at study start (P = 0·007). For both groups, the teachers’ assessment scores were improved following intervention. A statistically significant improvement was observed in the control group (P = 0·04).

Discussion

A free, healthy school lunch, consisting of wholemeal bread, different kinds of unsweetened spread, low-fat milk and fruit/vegetables, was offered to 9th grade pupils for 4 months. At the end of the study period, the BMI of the girls had not increased, while a significant increase was seen among the boys. The service of a free school lunch did not improve the food score, i.e. intakes of fruit, vegetables, low-fat milk and wholegrain bread, or reduce the intake of snacks, sugar-sweetened beverages and candy/chocolate. The healthy food score correlated positively with the trait ‘satisfaction with schoolwork’, which may lead to the conclusion that interested and positively minded pupils have a healthier diet than those with a more troublesome attitude towards school.

The prevalence of overweight among the 150 participating pupils (participation rate 81 %) was higher than previously reported in Norway(Reference Andersen, Lillegaard, Overby, Lytle, Klepp and Johansson3, Reference Juliusson, Roelants, Eide, Hauspie, Waaler and Bjerknes4, Reference Lien, Kumar, Holmboe-Ottesen, Klepp and Wandel18, Reference Groholt, Stigum and Nordhagen19). However, in three of the former studies weight and height were self-reported(Reference Andersen, Lillegaard, Overby, Lytle, Klepp and Johansson3, Reference Lien, Kumar, Holmboe-Ottesen, Klepp and Wandel18, Reference Groholt, Stigum and Nordhagen19), and in the study by Juliusson et al.(Reference Juliusson, Roelants, Eide, Hauspie, Waaler and Bjerknes4) the response rate for the age group 13–15 years was 50 %. Both the self-reported weight and height measurements and the response rate could explain the differences, but the sample size in our study is too small to justify a comparison. However, the prevalence of overweight registered in the present study indicates the importance of prevention strategies(5, Reference Prell, Berg, Jonsson and Lissner13).

Different strategies, including nutritional programmes and physical activity, have been investigated in connection with the prevention of obesity among children and adolescents, but the results are meagre(Reference Thomas20). As the Norwegian school system does not provide any organized lunch service, one of the aims of the current study was to evaluate whether a free lunch could reduce weight gain. This aim may seem too ambitious for a 4-month intervention period, and the results of this pilot study are indeed preliminary. Our finding that weight gain was reduced among girls, but not among boys, after the intervention period may also imply that the study period was too short to evaluate weight development. However, we are not aware of any studies in which such a comprehensive nutritional programme has been introduced, i.e. where there has been a change from a packed lunch from home to a full, free school lunch. In an earlier pilot study, we found that serving breakfast to 10th grade pupils reduced weight gain in both girls and boys(Reference Ask, Hernes, Aarek, Johannessen and Haugen6), and other studies have observed an improved nutritional profile among pupils participating in school breakfast schemes(Reference Matthys, De Henauw, Bellemans, De Maeyer and De Backer7, Reference Moreno and Rodriguez21). Furthermore, in the present study, the frequency of breakfast was negatively correlated with BMI among boys and no effect was observed in relation to lunch frequency. Given that overweight has been directly associated with skipping breakfast, it is interesting to speculate about whether breakfast programmes should be prioritized at the expense of school lunch programmes(Reference Rampersaud, Pereira, Girard, Adams and Metzl8, Reference Fabritius and Rasmussen22), not least in the context of weakening the link between socio-economic status and overweight(Reference Fabritius and Rasmussen22, Reference Kosti, Panagiotakos, Zampelas, Mihas, Alevizos, Leonard, Tountas and Mariolis23). Other research groups have reported that children who were served a nutritious breakfast showed increased cognitive performance. This has been observed in both nutritionally deprived and well-nourished children(Reference Kleinman, Hall, Green, Korzec-Ramirez, Patton, Pagano and Murphy24).

With the service of a free, healthy lunch including fruit/vegetables, we would have expected an increase in food score(Reference Rees, Richards and Gregory25, Reference Cullen, Watson and Zakeri26). Earlier studies in which free fruit was added to school lunches have reported increased fruit intake(Reference Bere, Veierod and Klepp27Reference Bere, Veierod, Skare and Klepp29). One reason for the reduced food score registered in our study could be the different times during the year that registration took place. The first registration was carried out in late January, while the second registration took place at the very end of May. Various national celebrations take place during May, and hot dogs, sugar-sweetened sodas and cakes are often served at such events. The significantly reduced food score of the control groups may confirm that May is a less healthy month regarding food intake. It is therefore possible that the results would have been different if the intervention period had been longer and the second registration had taken place at the same point during the year as the first registration. Furthermore, there was very little potential for improvement among the pupils at the intervention school. Seventy-seven per cent of the pupils brought a packed lunch from home four or five times weekly (data not shown). The corresponding figure for the control schools was 75 %. Only 2 % of all pupils admitted to buying lunch from kiosks or supermarkets every day, which confirmed that the pupils at the three schools already followed good lunch practices. It is also possible that eagerness to please played a part at the time of the first, but not the second, registration.

Nutritional knowledge and dietary locus of control have not been shown to have an impact on BMI(Reference O’Dea and Wilson30). In our study, all three school classes had three home economics lessons a week, covering both nutritional theory and practical cooking skills. However, this was not reflected in the food score. A health promotion programme featuring peer-led teaching on healthy living, covering nutrition, physical activity and a healthy body image, has been shown to have impact on weight among 4th to 7th grade pupils(Reference O’Dea and Wilson30). In an attempt to evaluate whether certain characteristics of school behaviour could predict body weight and food intake, three traits of self-perceived behaviour were identified. None of the scores relating to the three different traits correlated with weight, although a high school satisfaction score was positively correlated with grades and food score. Food score also correlated positively with the grades of boys in our study. Academic performance and diet quality (taking into account the entire diet) have been explored in two studies(Reference Florence, Asbridge and Veugelers14, Reference Kim, Frongillo, Han, Oh, Kim, Jang, Won, Lee and Kim15), and their conclusions are highly consistent with our findings. In one of the studies, academic performance was more strongly correlated with the meal pattern (three main meals daily) than with socio-economic status(Reference Kim, Frongillo, Han, Oh, Kim, Jang, Won, Lee and Kim15). Although our study is a pilot study, we have been able to demonstrate an association between diet quality and academic performance, despite a lack of information on the socio-economic status/cultural level of families(Reference Florence, Asbridge and Veugelers14, Reference Marjoribanks31). This information might be of interest to public bodies when planning school meals or looking for educational goals.

The teachers’ assessments of school behaviour before and after intervention are difficult to interpret, as both groups were more positive after the intervention period. However, the teachers at the intervention school expressed qualitatively a positive attitude towards the serving of lunch, referring to it as a positive break during the school day. The pupils had more social contact and mingled during the lunch.

Challenges will have to be overcome in the fight against obesity in childhood and during adolescence. Several strategies have been tried but no consensus has been reached, and it is obvious that various strategies will have to be employed(Reference Varela-Moreiras2, Reference Thomas20). In Norway, we do not serve any school meals, but the introduction of a cold or warm lunch has been discussed. In the interests of combating obesity and boosting cognitive performance, further investigations of school lunch quality should be carried out before any conclusions are reached or any decisions are made. Breakfast may be a healthier alternative than lunch, but the introduction of additional school hours may be necessitated by the service of both meals.

Acknowledgements

This study was founded through the University of Agder, Kristiansand, Norway. The authors declare that they have no competing interests. All authors made substantial contribution to the study concept and design. A.S.A., S.H., I.A., F.V. and C.B. carried out the practical part, and A.S.A., S.H. and I.A. supervised the breakfast serving in the intervention school. All authors contributed to drafting and critically revised the manuscript. M.H. wrote the manuscript and performed the statistical analyses. BAMA, TINE BA and Moseid Bakery are acknowledged and thanked for subvention of food.

References

1.Dehghan, M, Akhtar-Danesh, N & Merchant, AT (2005) Childhood obesity, prevalence and prevention. Nutr J 4, 24.Google Scholar
2.Varela-Moreiras, G (2006) Controlling obesity: what should be changed? Int J Vitam Nutr Res 76, 262268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3.Andersen, LF, Lillegaard, IT, Overby, N, Lytle, L, Klepp, KI & Johansson, L (2005) Overweight and obesity among Norwegian schoolchildren: changes from 1993 to 2000. Scand J Public Health 33, 99106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4.Juliusson, PB, Roelants, M, Eide, GE, Hauspie, R, Waaler, PE & Bjerknes, R (2007) Overweight and obesity in Norwegian children: secular trends in weight-for-height and skinfolds. Acta Paediatr 96, 13331337.Google Scholar
5.Minister of Health and Care Services (2009) Norwegian Action Plan on Nutrition. www.helsedirektoratet.no/ernaering/publikasjoner/handlingsplaner/norwegian_action_plan_on_nutrition_104884 (accessed January 2009).Google Scholar
6.Ask, AS, Hernes, S, Aarek, I, Johannessen, G & Haugen, M (2006) Changes in dietary pattern in 15 year old adolescents following a 4 month dietary intervention with school breakfast – a pilot study. Nutr J 5, 33.Google Scholar
7.Matthys, C, De Henauw, S, Bellemans, M, De Maeyer, M & De Backer, G (2007) Breakfast habits affect overall nutrient profiles in adolescents. Public Health Nutr 10, 413421.Google Scholar
8.Rampersaud, GC, Pereira, MA, Girard, BL, Adams, J & Metzl, JD (2005) Breakfast habits, nutritional status, body weight, and academic performance in children and adolescents. J Am Diet Assoc 105, 743760.Google Scholar
9.Andersen, LF, Nes, M, Sandstad, B, Bjorneboe, GE & Drevon, CA (1995) Dietary intake among Norwegian adolescents. Eur J Clin Nutr 49, 555564.Google Scholar
10.Makela, J, Kjaernes, U, Pipping, EM, L’orange, FE, Gronow, J & Holm, L (1999) Nordic meals: methodological notes on a comparative survey. Appetite 32, 7379.Google Scholar
11.Samuelson, G (2000) Dietary habits and nutritional status in adolescents over Europe. An overview of current studies in the Nordic countries. Eur J Clin Nutr 54, Suppl. 1, S21S28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12.Lytle, LA, Kubik, MY, Perry, C, Story, M, Birnbaum, AS & Murray, DM (2006) Influencing healthful food choices in school and home environments: results from the TEENS study. Prev Med 43, 813.Google Scholar
13.Prell, HC, Berg, MC, Jonsson, LM & Lissner, L (2005) A school-based intervention to promote dietary change. J Adolesc Health 36, 529.Google Scholar
14.Florence, MD, Asbridge, M & Veugelers, PJ (2008) Diet quality and academic performance. J Sch Health 78, 209215.Google Scholar
15.Kim, HY, Frongillo, EA, Han, SS, Oh, SY, Kim, WK, Jang, YA, Won, HS, Lee, HS & Kim, SH (2003) Academic performance of Korean children is associated with dietary behaviours and physical status. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr 12, 186192.Google Scholar
16.Cole, TJ, Bellizzi, MC, Flegal, KM & Dietz, WH (2000) Establishing a standard definition for child overweight and obesity worldwide: international survey. BMJ 320, 12401243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17.Roland, E & Galloway, D (2002) Classroom influences on bullying. Educ Res 44, 299312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
18.Lien, N, Kumar, BN, Holmboe-Ottesen, G, Klepp, KI & Wandel, M (2007) Assessing social differences in overweight among 15- to 16-year-old ethnic Norwegians from Oslo by register data and adolescent self-reported measures of socio-economic status. Int J Obes (Lond) 31, 3038.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19.Groholt, EK, Stigum, H & Nordhagen, R (2008) Overweight and obesity among adolescents in Norway: cultural and socio-economic differences. J Public Health 30, 258265.Google Scholar
20.Thomas, H (2006) Obesity prevention programs for children and youth: why are their results so modest? Health Educ Res 21, 783795.Google Scholar
21.Moreno, LA & Rodriguez, G (2007) Dietary risk factors for development of childhood obesity. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care 10, 336341.Google Scholar
22.Fabritius, K & Rasmussen, M (2008) Breakfast habits and overweight in Danish schoolchildren. The role of socioeconomic positions. Ugeskr Laeger 170, 25592563.Google Scholar
23.Kosti, RI, Panagiotakos, DB, Zampelas, A, Mihas, C, Alevizos, A, Leonard, C, Tountas, Y & Mariolis, A (2008) The association between consumption of breakfast cereals and BMI in schoolchildren aged 12–17 years: the VYRONAS study. Public Health Nutr 11, 10151021.Google Scholar
24.Kleinman, RE, Hall, S, Green, H, Korzec-Ramirez, D, Patton, K, Pagano, ME & Murphy, JM (2002) Diet, breakfast, and academic performance in children. Ann Nutr Metab 46, Suppl. 1, 2430.Google Scholar
25.Rees, GA, Richards, CJ & Gregory, J (2008) Food and nutrient intakes of primary school children: a comparison of school meals and packed lunches. J Hum Nutr Diet 21, 420427.Google Scholar
26.Cullen, KW, Watson, K & Zakeri, I (2008) Improvements in middle school student dietary intake after implementation of the Texas Public School Nutrition Policy. Am J Public Health 98, 111117.Google Scholar
27.Bere, E, Veierod, MB & Klepp, KI (2005) The Norwegian School Fruit Programme: evaluating paid vs. no-cost subscriptions. Prev Med 41, 463470.Google Scholar
28.Bere, E, Veierod, MB, Bjelland, M & Klepp, KI (2006) Free school fruit – sustained effect 1 year later. Health Educ Res 21, 258267.Google Scholar
29.Bere, E, Veierod, MB, Skare, O & Klepp, KI (2007) Free school fruit – sustained effect three years later. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 4, 5.Google Scholar
30.O’Dea, JA & Wilson, R (2006) Socio-cognitive and nutritional factors associated with body mass index in children and adolescents: possibilities for childhood obesity prevention. Health Educ Res 21, 796805.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31.Marjoribanks, K (2003) Family and ability correlates of academic achievement: social status group differences. Psychol Rep 93, 419422.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Scores given to intake of eleven food items; the scores were added to calculate a healthy food score

Figure 1

Table 2 Results for 4 months of serving a healthy school lunch to male 9th graders, Norway, January–May 2007

Figure 2

Table 3 Results for 4 months of serving a healthy school lunch to female 9th graders, Norway, January–May 2007

Figure 3

Table 4 Factor loading matrix for the three main traits identified by principal component analysis among male and female 9th graders, Norway, January–May 2007

Figure 4

Table 5 Food score as a function of gender and school pattern (r2 = 0·15, P < 0·001) among male and female 9th graders, Norway, January–May 2007. Beta values are unstandardized coefficients