Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T12:32:14.836Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Refined carbohydrate intake in relation to non-verbal intelligence among Tehrani schoolchildren

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 December 2011

Amin Salehi Abargouei
Affiliation:
Food Security Research Center, Isfahan University of Medical Sciences, Isfahan, Islamic Republic of Iran Department of Community Nutrition, School of Nutrition and Food Science, Isfahan University of Medical Sciences, PO Box 81745, Isfahan, Islamic Republic of Iran
Naser Kalantari
Affiliation:
Department of Community Nutrition, Faculty of Nutrition and Food Technology, Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Islamic Republic of Iran
Nasrin Omidvar
Affiliation:
Department of Community Nutrition, Faculty of Nutrition and Food Technology, Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Islamic Republic of Iran
Bahram Rashidkhani
Affiliation:
Department of Community Nutrition, Faculty of Nutrition and Food Technology, Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Islamic Republic of Iran
Anahita Houshiar Rad
Affiliation:
National Nutrition and Food Technology Institute, Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Islamic Republic of Iran
Azizeh Afkham Ebrahimi
Affiliation:
Mental Health Research Center, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Islamic Republic of Iran
Hossein Khosravi-Boroujeni
Affiliation:
Food Security Research Center, Isfahan University of Medical Sciences, Isfahan, Islamic Republic of Iran
Ahmad Esmaillzadeh*
Affiliation:
Food Security Research Center, Isfahan University of Medical Sciences, Isfahan, Islamic Republic of Iran Department of Community Nutrition, School of Nutrition and Food Science, Isfahan University of Medical Sciences, PO Box 81745, Isfahan, Islamic Republic of Iran
*
*Corresponding author: Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

Nutrition has long been considered one of the most important environmental factors affecting human intelligence. Although carbohydrates are the most widely studied nutrient for their possible effects on cognition, limited data are available linking usual refined carbohydrate intake and intelligence. The present study was conducted to examine the relationship between long-term refined carbohydrate intake and non-verbal intelligence among schoolchildren.

Design

Cross-sectional study.

Setting

Tehran, Iran.

Subjects

In this cross-sectional study, 245 students aged 6–7 years were selected from 129 elementary schools in two western regions of Tehran. Anthropometric measurements were carried out. Non-verbal intelligence and refined carbohydrate consumption were determined using Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices test and a modified sixty-seven-item FFQ, respectively. Data about potential confounding variables were collected. Linear regression analysis was applied to examine the relationship between non-verbal intelligence scores and refined carbohydrate consumption.

Results

Individuals in top tertile of refined carbohydrate intake had lower mean non-verbal intelligence scores in the crude model (P < 0·038). This association remained significant after controlling for age, gender, birth date, birth order and breast-feeding pattern (P = 0·045). However, further adjustments for mother's age, mother's education, father's education, parental occupation and BMI made the association statistically non-significant. We found a significant inverse association between refined carbohydrate consumption and non-verbal intelligence scores in regression models (β = −11·359, P < 0·001). This relationship remained significant in multivariate analysis after controlling for potential confounders (β = −8·495, P = 0·038).

Conclusions

The study provides evidence indicating an inverse relationship between refined carbohydrate consumption and non-verbal intelligence among Tehrani children aged 6–7 years. Prospective studies are needed to confirm our findings.

Type
Research paper
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2011

Nutrition has long been considered one of the most important environmental factors affecting human intelligence. Several studies have been performed in an effort to discern the relationship between nutrition and intelligence or cognitive ability, with the majority focusing on childhood as an important part of the life cycle. Some investigations have assessed malnutrition in relation to cognitive ability( Reference Leiva Plaza, Inzunza Brito and Perez Torrejon 1 , Reference Brown and Pollitt 2 ) and demonstrated that nutritional supplementation can improve children's cognitive ability, at least among those with low dietary intakes( Reference Brown and Pollitt 2 Reference Stein, Wang and DiGirolamo 4 ). Other studies have indicated that deficiencies of Fe, iodine, Cu and folic acid are associated with lower intelligence status( Reference Qian, Wang and Chen 5 Reference Christian, Murray-Kolb and Khatry 8 ) and that children with low dietary intakes of these micronutrients would benefit from supplementation( Reference Benton 9 , Reference Schoenthaler, Bier and Young 10 ). In addition to considering nutrients, dietary patterns have also been found to be associated with intelligence among children. Theodore et al. concluded that consuming fish, breads and cereals would probably improve children's cognitive development. In contrast, daily margarine consumption was inversely associated with cognitive functioning( Reference Theodore, Thompson and Waldie 11 ).

Among macronutrients, carbohydrates are the most widely studied nutrient for their possible effects on cognition. Since glucose is the primary source of energy for the brain, several studies have explored the short-term effect of carbohydrates on cognitive ability with inconsistent results( Reference Leigh Gibson and Green 12 Reference Gilsenan, de Bruin and Dye 15 ). The most consistent findings from macronutrient manipulation and meal-based studies are related to the effects of glucose on short-term and delayed verbal and non-verbal memory performance( Reference Hoyland, Lawton and Dye 16 ). Carbohydrate–cognitive function relationships have also been assessed by considering the glycaemic index and glycaemic load of carbohydrates. A recent review has concluded that the evidence on the relationship between dietary glycaemic load and short-term cognitive performance is not sufficient to support the association( Reference Gilsenan, de Bruin and Dye 15 ). Therefore, further studies on the association between dietary glycaemic index and glycaemic load in relation to cognitive function are warranted.

The Iranian diet contains high amounts of carbohydrate from processed grains. High consumption of processed grains has been related to greater risk of obesity( Reference Popkin and Nielsen 17 ), type 2 diabetes( Reference Gross, Li and Ford 18 , Reference Liu 19 ), CVD( Reference Liu 19 ), metabolic syndrome( Reference McKeown, Meigs and Liu 20 ) and high blood pressure( Reference Preuss, Fournier and Chieuh 21 , Reference Fournier, Chiueh and Kopin 22 ) even among Iranians( Reference Sherafat-Kazemzadeh, Egtesadi and Mirmiran 23 ). Limited data are available linking usual carbohydrate intake from processed grains and cognitive performance. An experimental study found that a ‘high fat/refined carbohydrate’ diet can increase the susceptibility to spatial learning deficit in rats compared with a ‘low fat/complex carbohydrate’ diet( Reference Goldbart, Row and Kheirandish-Gozal 24 ). We are aware of just one report that has been done in man. In a recent British cohort, the investigators indicated that adherence to a ‘high fat–high sugar’ dietary pattern in early childhood would unfavourably affect intelligence at 8 years of age( Reference Northstone, Joinson and Emmett 25 ). It must be kept in mind that, in the mentioned study, dietary patterns (instead of nutrients or foods) were regarded as an exposure. In the holistic approach of dietary pattern analysis( Reference Willett 26 ), it is impossible to identify the association of specific foods or nutrients (like carbohydrates) with cognitive function. Therefore, it would be interesting to further explore carbohydrate–intelligence quotient (IQ) relationships. Furthermore, due to high consumption of dietary carbohydrates from processed grains, the Iranian diet provides an excellent opportunity to assess such relationships within the Iranian population. The present study was therefore conducted to examine the relationship between long-term refined carbohydrate intake and IQ among schoolchildren in Tehran, Iran.

Participants and methods

Study population

The current study is a secondary analysis of data from a case–control study aiming to find the relationship between breast-feeding and myopia. In total, 245 students aged 6–7 years were selected from 129 elementary schools in two western regions of Tehran. The inclusion criteria were being 6–7 years old, not following any specific diet and no food avoidance. If the participants had any eye, metabolic or congenital disease they were excluded from the study.

Anthropometric measurements

Weight was measured with the participant wearing light clothes using a digital scale (model 803; Seca, Hamburg, Germany) to the nearest 0·1 kg. Each participant was weighed three times and the mean of these three measurements was considered as the final recorded weight for the participant. The participant's height was measured in standing position by the use of a stiff plastic tape measure fixed on a wall and recorded to the nearest 1 cm. To measure height, the participant removed his/her shoes and stood with his/her head, shoulder blades, buttocks and heels touching the wall where the tape was fixed.

Assessment of refined carbohydrate intake

Dietary intake of refined carbohydrates in the last year was assessed by a modified sixty-seven-item FFQ. To construct this FFQ, we selected eighty-four food items with refined carbohydrate content that had high glycaemic index and were more frequently consumed among children and adolescents. These items consisted of rice, different types of breads, cooked potato, chips, vermicelli or macaroni, white flour and all food items produced from white flour (like biscuits, cakes and crackers), sugar and food items containing high amounts of sugar (like jam, honey, jelly, chocolates and sugar-sweetened beverages). These items were selected from among a list of 168 food items included in a validated FFQ for the Iranian population( Reference Azadbakht, Esmailzadeh and Azizi 27 ). We selected the food items that were more frequently used among elementary-school children( Reference Dadkhah Piraghaj, Amini and Hooshyarrad 28 ). To assess the performance of the eighty-four-item FFQ, we asked forty children (twenty male and twenty female) aged 6–7 years and their parents to fill out the questionnaire. At this time a 5 d food diary was also requested from participants and their parents about foods consumed in these 5 d. This was done to make sure that all food items containing refined carbohydrates had been included in the FFQ. Comparison of the eighty-four-item FFQ with the food diaries made us modify the FFQ to include sixty-seven food items more frequently consumed. The FFQ was self-administered and parents in collaboration with their children reported the frequency of each food item as well as the portion size that the child ate at each time. The reliability of this FFQ for assessing dietary intakes of refined carbohydrates was shown to be reasonable (r = 0·633; P < 0·01). Consumption of all food items was considered in grams per day and then macronutrient intakes for each participant were calculated by use of the Iranian national food composition table. Total macronutrient intake was calculated by summing up macronutrients from all foods.

Measurement of intelligence

Raven's Colorful Progressive Matrices test, which has been validated among schoolchildren in Tehran( Reference Baraheni 29 ), was used as a screening test of cognitive ability. These matrices are non-verbal multiple-choice measures of the reasoning which is often referred to as ‘general intelligence’. Raven's Progressive Matrices measures the ability to make comparisons, to reason by analogy and to organize spatial perceptions into systematically related wholes. These matrices assess the non-verbal intelligence. The test also measures the ability to think clearly and make sense of complexity, as well as the ability to store and reproduce information. The matrices consist of thirty-six colourful puzzles designed to measure a person's ability to form perceptual relationships and to reason by analogy independent of language and formal schooling, and can be used with persons ranging from 6 years of age to adulthood. The candidate is provided with multiple choices to identify the missing segment required to complete a larger pattern in each test item. In the present study, the test was administered individually under the supervision of trained research assistants, and the raw scores were converted to standardized scores based on the age of participants for analysis( Reference Baraheni 29 ). This test was selected because it is a valid, rapid and simple way to measure participants’ intelligence and cognition status at the age of 6–7 years. Individuals with higher scores in the test have been considered to have better intelligence than those with lower scores.

Assessment of other variables

Information about physical activity, birth date, birth weight, maternal age, parental education and occupation, birth order and child's breast-feeding pattern in infancy was assessed by a self-administered questionnaire mailed to parents. Physical activity was assessed by a questionnaire asking parents about types of routine physical activity performed by their children during the day and the time they spent daily or weekly for each activity. Physical activity was then treated as a categorical variable (<1 h/d, 1–3 h/d, >3 h/d). To assess the pattern of breast-feeding during infancy, parents were asked to report the duration of breast-feeding and the time of introducing complementary foods before the age of 6 months. Based on this information, children were categorized into three groups of breast-feeding pattern based on WHO recommendations( 30 ): not breast-fed (did not consume breast milk from birth), predominantly breast-fed (consumed breast milk for less than 6 months or consumed foods other than breast milk before the age of 6 months) and exclusively breast-fed (consumed only breast milk up to 6 months of age). Preterm children were defined as those born earlier than week 32 of gestation. A member of the research team (A.S.A.) was responsible for communicating with parents via telephone to answer parents’ questions.

Statistical analysis

Histograms and the Kolmogrov–Smirnov test were used to assess the normality of the distribution of variables. Log transformation was applied for non-normally distributed variables. Tertiles of refined carbohydrate intake were used for categorization of the participants. Comparison of continuous and categorical variables across tertiles of refined carbohydrate intake was done by the use of one-way ANOVA and the χ 2 test, respectively. Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was used to determine the differences in non-verbal intelligence scores across tertiles of refined carbohydrate intake. We also used multivariate linear regression analysis to assess the relationship in different models. In the first model we controlled for age, gender, birth date, birth order and breast-feeding pattern. In the second model, we further adjusted for mother's age, mother's education, father's education and parental occupation. In the third model, we additionally controlled for BMI. All statistical analyses were done using the SPSS for Windows statistical software package version 15·0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). P values<0·05 were considered statistically significant.

Results

In total, 245 children aged 6–7 years participated in the study; among them, eight had missing data for refined carbohydrate consumption and one reported consumption of more than 1000 g/d. After exclusion of these individuals, 236 children (42·4 % male) remained for the current analysis.

Daily consumption of refined carbohydrates was 285·16 (sd 157·03) g/d. General characteristics of the total study population and across tertiles of refined carbohydrate intake are provided in Table 1. Mean (sd) age and BMI of the participants was 6·34 (sd 0·48) years and 16·25 (sd 2·85) kg/m2, respectively. No significant differences were found in terms of general characteristics across tertiles of refined carbohydrate intake.

Table 1 General characteristics of the study population according to tertile of refined carbohydrate intake: children (n 245) aged 6–7 years, Tehran, Iran

*Preterm children were defined as those born earlier than week 32 of gestation.

†Not-breast-fed: did not consume breast milk from birth; predominantly breast-fed: consumed breast milk for less than 6 months or consumed foods other than breast milk before the age of 6 months; exclusively breast-fed: consumed only breast milk up to 6 months of age.

‡Questions about physical activity were related to the type of routine physical activity performed by children during the day and time they spent daily or weekly for each activity.

The score of non-verbal intelligence ranged from 77 to 146. Table 2 indicates the mean non-verbal intelligence scores in crude and adjusted models across tertiles of refined carbohydrate intake. Individuals in the top tertile of refined carbohydrate intake had lower mean non-verbal intelligence scores in the crude model (P < 0·038). The difference remained significant after controlling for age, gender, birth date, birth order and breast-feeding pattern (P = 0·045). However, when the analysis was further adjusted for mother's age, mother's education, father's education, parental occupation and BMI, the difference between mean intelligence scores across tertiles of refined carbohydrate intake was no longer significant.

Table 2 Multivariate-adjusted means of non-verbal intelligence scores across tertile of refined carbohydrate intake: children (n 245) aged 6–7 years, Tehran, Iran

*Obtained by analysis of covariance.

†Controlled for age, gender, birth date, birth order and breast-feeding pattern.

‡Further adjusted for maternal age, mother's education, father's education and parental occupation.

§Additionally controlled for BMI.

∣∣Mean value was significantly different from that of the first tertile.

Findings from linear regression analysis are provided in Table 3. The relationship between refined carbohydrate consumption and non-verbal intelligence scores was examined in a crude model as well as in additional models controlling for confounders. In the crude model, there was a significant inverse association between refined carbohydrate consumption and non-verbal intelligence scores (β = −11·359, P < 0·001). This relationship remained significant in multivariate analysis after controlling for age, gender, birth date, birth order and breast-feeding pattern (β = −9·047, P = 0·016). Additional adjustment for mother's age, mother's education, father's education and parental occupation did not alter the findings (β = −8·230, P = 0·045). Even after further control for BMI, the inverse association of refined carbohydrate consumption with non-verbal intelligence scores remained statistically significant (β = −8·495, P = 0·038).

Table 3 Regression coefficients and 95 % confidence intervals for the association between refined carbohydrate consumption and non-verbal intelligence scores: children (n 245) aged 6–7 years, Tehran, Iran

*Controlled for age, gender, birth date, birth order and breast-feeding pattern.

†Further adjusted for mother's age, mother's education, father's education and parental occupation.

‡Additionally controlled for BMI.

Discussion

The present cross-sectional study performed among a small group of Tehrani children found an inverse relationship between refined carbohydrate intake and non-verbal intelligence. This association remained statistically significant after adjustment for potential confounders. We found that mean scores for non-verbal intelligence decreased as refined carbohydrate consumption increased. To the best of our knowledge, our study is the first to examine the association between habitual intake of refined carbohydrates and non-verbal intelligence among children.

Few studies are available examining long-term habitual dietary intakes in relation to IQ; however, numerous investigations have explored the effect of short-term dietary intakes on cognitive performance albeit with inconsistent results( Reference Gilsenan, de Bruin and Dye 15 ). For instance, a positive effect of a low-glycaemic-load breakfast on memory has been shown as compared with a high-glycaemic-load breakfast( Reference Wesnes, Pincock and Richardson 31 Reference Ingwersen, Defeyter and Kennedy 33 ). Verbal IQ, like cognition and learning ability, is more affected by environment in comparison with non-verbal IQ while performance IQ is more likely to be innate( Reference Northstone, Joinson and Emmett 25 ). Although Raven's Progressive Matrices is a non-verbal test which measures ‘fluid’ rather than environmentally conditioned ‘crystallized’ intelligence( Reference Stobart 34 ), surprisingly our results indicated that performance IQ can be related to usual refined carbohydrate consumption. Northstone et al.( Reference Northstone, Joinson and Emmett 25 ) found an inverse association between a ‘processed’ dietary pattern at age 3 years and IQ at age 8·5 years. The ‘processed’ dietary pattern in their study was greatly loaded with foods high in sugar and fat. The authors came to the conclusion that diet in terms of dietary patterns can affect IQ, not only verbal but also non-verbal IQ. However, they failed to find a significant relationship between the ‘processed’ dietary pattern at age 4 or 7 years and IQ at age 8·5 years. In the current study, we were not able to calculate the fat, protein and sugar contents of foods included in the FFQ. Therefore, we cannot differentiate the effect of individual macronutrients on intelligence. However, as refined carbohydrate intake in our study is based primarily on processed grains, our findings are not so much different from those reached by Northstone et al.( Reference Northstone, Joinson and Emmett 25 ).

Refined carbohydrate consumption has increased in recent years( Reference Huang, Dhawan and Young 35 ) and has been connected to many chronic diseases. This is particularly relevant for Asian countries where items containing refined carbohydrates, namely rice and white bread, are the staple foods. Findings of the current study support the previous findings on the harmful effects of refined carbohydrate consumption on human health.

The mechanisms by which high intake of refined carbohydrates can affect non-verbal IQ in man remain to be understood. An experimental study has found that consumption of a high-fat, refined carbohydrate diet for 2 months resulted in a significant reduction in hippocampal level of brain-derived neurotrophic factor and spatial learning performance( Reference Molteni, Barnard and Ying 36 ). Maintaining animals on this diet for a duration of 24 months resulted in a significant decrease of synapsin I mRNA and protein (total and phosphorylated), cyclic AMP-response element-binding protein mRNA and protein (total and phosphorylated) and growth-associated protein 43 mRNA. All of these proteins are involved in neurotransmitter release and are required for neurite outgrowth, learning and memory( Reference Molteni, Barnard and Ying 36 ). Furthermore, the short-term beneficial influence of carbohydrate or glucose consumption on cognitive performance has been suggested to occur via affecting serum insulin( Reference Park 37 , Reference Trejo, Piriz and Llorens-Martin 38 ), ghrelin( Reference Diano, Farr and Benoit 39 ) or cortisol( Reference Het, Ramlow and Wolf 40 ) levels. However, it is not clear if habitual dietary intakes of refined carbohydrates exert their effects on intelligence via this mechanism.

Several limitations must be considered in the interpretation of our findings. First is the cross-sectional nature of our study which prohibits inferring a casual relationship between refined carbohydrate consumption and non-verbal IQ. Second, the modified FFQ we used in the study was designed to collect data about refined carbohydrate consumption. Therefore, data on the dietary intakes of other food groups, macronutrients and particularly total energy intake were not available. However, we adjusted our statistical analyses for BMI, as a measure of total energy intake, to control for the effect of energy intake on studied relationships. It must be kept in mind that the protein and fat contents of processed grains (from which we calculated the refined carbohydrate intake) might contribute to the glycaemic index of foods and eventually to cognitive ability. Therefore, future studies exploring the independent association between refined carbohydrate intakes and cognitive ability must consider the confounding effect from dietary fats and proteins. Third, we cannot extrapolate our findings to all students of Tehran because the study participants were confined to two western regions of Tehran. However, socio-economic status was considered in sample selection enabling us to have a broad range of dietary intakes. Fourth, the small sample size in the study might prohibit us from reaching enough statistical power for the study. As studying the association of habitual intakes of refined carbohydrates with intelligence is a novel hypothesis, even findings from small samples are of value. For better understanding of the association, prospective cohort studies with larger sample sizes would be required to confirm our findings.

In conclusion, the present study provides evidence indicating an inverse relationship between refined carbohydrate consumption and non-verbal IQ among 6–7-year-old Tehrani children. These findings support the hypothesis that long-term dietary intakes may have positive or adverse effects on the ‘fluid’ intelligence. More studies are needed to confirm this hypothesis.

Acknowledgements

The data collection phase of the study was funded by the National Nutrition and Food Technology Research Institute, Tehran, Iran. Financial support for drafting the manuscript came from the Food Security Research Center, Isfahan University of Medical Science, Isfahan, Iran. None of the authors had any financial or personal conflicts of interest. All authors were involved in planning the study design. A.S.A. and N.K. contributed to the conception of the study hypothesis. Intelligence testing was supervised by A.A.E. FFQ modification was supervised by A.H.R. The data analysis was performed by A.S.A. and supervised by B.R. and A.E. A.S.A. and A.E. wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors were involved in revisions and final approval of the manuscript. The authors would like to thank Mohammad Afkhamin and Ashkan Ariai Nejad for their assistance in data collection and analysis of the FFQ data.

References

1. Leiva Plaza, B, Inzunza Brito, N, Perez Torrejon, H et al. (2001) The impact of malnutrition on brain development, intelligence and school work performance. Arch Latinoam Nutr 51, 6471.Google ScholarPubMed
2. Brown, JL & Pollitt, E (1996) Malnutrition, poverty and intellectual development. Sci Am 274, 3843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3. Zhao, L, Yu, W, Jia, F et al. (2009) The influence of the nutrient supplement for children on the nutrition and health status and intelligence of children of schooling age in the countryside of children of schooling age during 2 years. Wei Sheng Yan Jiu 38, 696699.Google Scholar
4. Stein, AD, Wang, M, DiGirolamo, A et al. (2008) Nutritional supplementation in early childhood, schooling, and intellectual functioning in adulthood: a prospective study in Guatemala. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 162, 612618.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5. Qian, M, Wang, D & Chen, Z (2000) A preliminary meta-analysis of 36 studies on impairment of intelligence development induced by iodine deficiency. Zhonghua Yu Fang Yi Xue Za Zhi 34, 7577.Google ScholarPubMed
6. Gonzalez, HF, Malpeli, A, Etchegoyen, G et al. (2007) Acquisition of visuomotor abilities and intellectual quotient in children aged 4–10 years: relationship with micronutrient nutritional status. Biol Trace Elem Res 120, 92101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7. Pollitt, E, Saco-Pollitt, C, Jahari, A et al. (2000) Effects of an energy and micronutrient supplement on mental development and behavior under natural conditions in undernourished children in Indonesia. Eur J Clin Nutr 54, Suppl. 2, S80S90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8. Christian, P, Murray-Kolb, LE, Khatry, SK et al. (2010) Prenatal micronutrient supplementation and intellectual and motor function in early school-aged children in Nepal. JAMA 304, 27162723.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9. Benton, D (2001) Micro-nutrient supplementation and the intelligence of children. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 25, 297309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10. Schoenthaler, SJ, Bier, ID, Young, K et al. (2000) The effect of vitamin–mineral supplementation on the intelligence of American schoolchildren: a randomized, double-blind placebo-controlled trial. J Altern Complement Med 6, 1929.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11. Theodore, RF, Thompson, JMD, Waldie, KE et al. (2009) Dietary patterns and intelligence in early and middle childhood. Intelligence 37, 506513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
12. Leigh Gibson, E & Green, MW (2002) Nutritional influences on cognitive function: mechanisms of susceptibility. Nutr Res Rev 15, 169206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13. Messier, C (2004) Glucose improvement of memory: a review. Eur J Pharmacol 490, 3357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14. Dye, L & Blundell, J (2002) Functional foods: psychological and behavioural functions. Br J Nutr 88, Suppl. 2, S187S211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15. Gilsenan, MB, de Bruin, EA & Dye, L (2009) The influence of carbohydrate on cognitive performance: a critical evaluation from the perspective of glycaemic load. Br J Nutr 101, 941949.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16. Hoyland, A, Lawton, CL & Dye, L (2008) Acute effects of macronutrient manipulations on cognitive test performance in healthy young adults: a systematic research review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32, 7285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17. Popkin, BM & Nielsen, SJ (2003) The sweetening of the world's diet. Obes Res 11, 13251332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18. Gross, LS, Li, L, Ford, ES et al. (2004) Increased consumption of refined carbohydrates and the epidemic of type 2 diabetes in the United States: an ecologic assessment. Am J Clin Nutr 79, 774779.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19. Liu, S (2002) Intake of refined carbohydrates and whole grain foods in relation to risk of type 2 diabetes mellitus and coronary heart disease. J Am Coll Nutr 21, 298306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20. McKeown, NM, Meigs, JB, Liu, S et al. (2004) Carbohydrate nutrition, insulin resistance, and the prevalence of the metabolic syndrome in the Framingham Offspring Cohort. Diabetes Care 27, 538546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21. Preuss, HG, Fournier, RD, Chieuh, CC et al. (1986) Refined carbohydrates affect blood pressure and retinal vasculature in spontaneously hypertensive and Wistar-Kyoto rats. J Hypertens Suppl 4, issue 3, S459S462.Google ScholarPubMed
22. Fournier, RD, Chiueh, CC, Kopin, IJ et al. (1986) Refined carbohydrate increases blood pressure and catecholamine excretion in SHR and WKY. Am J Physiol 250, E381E385.Google ScholarPubMed
23. Sherafat-Kazemzadeh, R, Egtesadi, S, Mirmiran, P et al. (2010) Dietary patterns by reduced rank regression predicting changes in obesity indices in a cohort study: Tehran Lipid and Glucose Study. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr 19, 2232.Google Scholar
24. Goldbart, AD, Row, BW, Kheirandish-Gozal, L et al. (2006) High fat/refined carbohydrate diet enhances the susceptibility to spatial learning deficits in rats exposed to intermittent hypoxia. Brain Res 1090, 190196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25. Northstone, K, Joinson, C, Emmett, P et al. (2011) Are dietary patterns in childhood associated with IQ at 8 years of age? A population-based cohort study. J Epidemiol Community Health (Epublication ahead of print version).Google ScholarPubMed
26. Willett, W (1998) Nutritional Epidemiology. New York, Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
27. Azadbakht, LMP, Esmailzadeh, A & Azizi, F (2005) Dairy consumption is inversely associated with the prevalence of the metabolic syndrome in Tehranian adults. Am J Clin Nutr 82, 523530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28. Dadkhah Piraghaj, M, Amini, M, Hooshyarrad, A et al. (2008) Qualitative and quantitative survey of elementary students, Tehran. Iranian J Nutr Food Technol 3, 3134.Google Scholar
29. Baraheni, MN (1974) Raven's progressive matrices as applied to Iranian children. Educ Psychol Meas 34, 983988.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
30. World Health Organization (2001) Global Strategy for Infant and Young Child Feeding: The Optimal Duration of Exclusive Breastfeeding. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
31. Wesnes, KA, Pincock, C, Richardson, D et al. (2003) Breakfast reduces declines in attention and memory over the morning in schoolchildren. Appetite 41, 329331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32. Benton, D, Maconie, A & Williams, C (2007) The influence of the glycaemic load of breakfast on the behaviour of children in school. Physiol Behav 92, 717724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33. Ingwersen, J, Defeyter, MA, Kennedy, DO et al. (2007) A low glycaemic index breakfast cereal preferentially prevents children's cognitive performance from declining throughout the morning. Appetite 49, 240244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34. Stobart, G (2008) Intelligence testing: how to create a monster. In Testing Times: The Uses and Abuses of Assessment, pp. 3056. London: Taylor & Francis.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
35. Huang, D, Dhawan, T, Young, S et al. (2011) Fructose impairs glucose-induced hepatic triglyceride synthesis. Lipids Health Dis 10, 20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36. Molteni, R, Barnard, RJ, Ying, Z et al. (2002) A high-fat, refined sugar diet reduces hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor, neuronal plasticity, and learning. Neuroscience 112, 803814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37. Park, CR (2001) Cognitive effects of insulin in the central nervous system. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 25, 311323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
38. Trejo, JL, Piriz, J, Llorens-Martin, MV et al. (2007) Central actions of liver-derived insulin-like growth factor I underlying its pro-cognitive effects. Mol Psychiatry 12, 11181128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
39. Diano, S, Farr, SA, Benoit, SC et al. (2006) Ghrelin controls hippocampal spine synapse density and memory performance. Nat Neurosci 9, 381388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40. Het, S, Ramlow, G & Wolf, OT (2005) A meta-analytic review of the effects of acute cortisol administration on human memory. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 771784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 General characteristics of the study population according to tertile of refined carbohydrate intake: children (n 245) aged 6–7 years, Tehran, Iran

Figure 1

Table 2 Multivariate-adjusted means of non-verbal intelligence scores across tertile of refined carbohydrate intake: children (n 245) aged 6–7 years, Tehran, Iran

Figure 2

Table 3 Regression coefficients and 95 % confidence intervals for the association between refined carbohydrate consumption and non-verbal intelligence scores: children (n 245) aged 6–7 years, Tehran, Iran