Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T03:23:24.387Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Making a stronger case for comparative research to investigate the behavioral and neurological bases of three-dimensional navigation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 October 2013

Daniele Nardi
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Sapienza Università di Roma, 00185 Rome, Italy. [email protected]
Verner P. Bingman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and J.P. Scott Center for Neuroscience, Mind, and Behavior, Bowling Green State University, Bowling Green, OH 43403. [email protected]

Abstract

The rich diversity of avian natural history provides exciting possibilities for comparative research aimed at understanding three-dimensional navigation. We propose some hypotheses relating differences in natural history to potential behavioral and neurological adaptations possessed by contrasting bird species. This comparative approach may offer unique insights into some of the important questions raised by Jeffery et al.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Gagliaro, A., Ioalè, P. & Bingman, V. P. (1999) Homing in pigeons: The role of the hippocampal formation in the representation of landmarks used for navigation. Journal of Neuroscience 19:311–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grobéty, M. C. & Schenk, F. (1992a) Spatial learning in a three-dimensional maze. Animal Behaviour 43(6):1011–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harvey, P. H. & Pagel, M. D. (1991) The comparative method in evolutionary biology. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Jovalekic, A., Hayman, R., Becares, N., Reid, H., Thomas, G., Wilson, J. & Jeffery, K. (2011) Horizontal biases in rats' use of three-dimensional space. Behavioural Brain Research 222(2):279–88.Google Scholar
Kamil, A. C., Balda, R. P. & Olson, D. J. (1994) Performance of four seed-caching corvid species in the radial arm-maze analog. Journal of Comparative Psychology 108:385–93.Google Scholar
Nardi, D. & Bingman, V. P. (2009a) Pigeon (Columba livia) encoding of a goal location: The relative importance of shape geometry and slope information. Journal of Comparative Psychology 123:204–16.Google Scholar
Nardi, D. & Bingman, V. P. (2009b) Slope-based encoding of a goal location is unaffected by hippocampal lesions in homing pigeons (Columba livia). Behavioural Brain Research 205(1):322–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nardi, D., Mauch, R. J., Klimas, D. B. & Bingman, V. P. (2012) Use of slope and feature cues in pigeon (Columba livia) goal-searching behavior. Journal of Comparative Psychology 126:288–93.Google Scholar
Nardi, D., Nitsch, K. P. & Bingman, V. P. (2010) Slope-driven goal location behavior in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes 36(4):430–42.Google Scholar
Pravosudov, V. V. & Clayton, N. S. (2002) A test of the adaptive specialization hypothesis: Population differences in caching, memory, and the hippocampus in black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapilla). Behavioral Neuroscience 116:515–22.Google Scholar
Sherry, D. F. & Vaccarino, A. L. (1989) Hippocampus and memory for food caches in black-capped chickadees. Behavioral Neuroscience 103:308–18.Google Scholar
Vargas, J. P., Petruso, E. J. & Bingman, V. P. (2004) Hippocampal formation is required for geometric navigation in pigeons. European Journal of Neuroscience 20:1937–44.Google Scholar