Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T15:19:04.484Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Children’s propensity to consume sugar and fat predicts regular alcohol consumption in adolescence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 August 2018

Kirsten Mehlig*
Affiliation:
Section for Epidemiology and Social Medicine, Institute of Medicine, Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg, Box 453, SE-405 30, Gothenburg, Sweden
Leonie H Bogl
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland Leibniz Institute for Prevention Research and Epidemiology – BIPS, Bremen, Germany
Monica Hunsberger
Affiliation:
Section for Epidemiology and Social Medicine, Institute of Medicine, Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg, Box 453, SE-405 30, Gothenburg, Sweden
Wolfgang Ahrens
Affiliation:
Leibniz Institute for Prevention Research and Epidemiology – BIPS, Bremen, Germany Institute of Statistics, University of Bremen, Bremen, Germany
Stefaan De Henauw
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
Isabel Iguacel
Affiliation:
Growth, Exercise, Nutrition and Development (GENUD) Research Group, University of Zaragoza, Zaragoza, Spain
Hannah Jilani
Affiliation:
Leibniz Institute for Prevention Research and Epidemiology – BIPS, Bremen, Germany
Dénes Molnár
Affiliation:
Department of Pediatrics, Medical School, University of Pécs, Pécs, Hungary
Valeria Pala
Affiliation:
Epidemiology and Prevention Unit, Fondazione IRCCS Istituto Nazionale dei Tumori, Milan, Italy
Paola Russo
Affiliation:
Institute of Food Sciences, CNR, Avellino, Italy
Michael Tornaritis
Affiliation:
Research and Education Institute of Child Health, Strovolos, Cyprus
Toomas Veidebaum
Affiliation:
National Institute for Health Development, Tallinn, Estonia
Jaakko Kaprio
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland Institute for Molecular Medicine FIMM, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
Lauren Lissner
Affiliation:
Section for Epidemiology and Social Medicine, Institute of Medicine, Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg, Box 453, SE-405 30, Gothenburg, Sweden
*
*Corresponding author: Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

The present study investigated the association between sugar and fat intake in childhood in relation to alcohol use in adolescence. We hypothesized that early exposure to diets high in fat and sugar may affect ingestive behaviours later in life, including alcohol use.

Design/Setting/Subjects

Children from the European IDEFICS/I.Family cohort study were examined at ages 5–9 years and followed up at ages 11–16 years. FFQ were completed by parents on behalf of children, and later by adolescents themselves. Complete data were available in 2263 participants. Children’s propensities to consume foods high in fat and sugar were calculated and dichotomized at median values. Adolescents’ use of alcohol was classified as at least weekly v. less frequent use. Log-binomial regression linked sugar and fat consumption in childhood to risk of alcohol use in adolescence, adjusted for relevant covariates.

Results

Five per cent of adolescents reported weekly alcohol consumption. Children with high propensity to consume sugar and fat were at greater risk of later alcohol use, compared with children with low fat and low sugar propensity (relative risk=2·46; 95 % CI 1·47, 4·12), independent of age, sex and survey country. The association was not explained by parental income and education, strict parenting style or child's health-related quality of life and was only partly mediated by sustained consumption of sugar and fat into adolescence.

Conclusions

Frequent consumption of foods high in fat and sugar in childhood predicted regular use of alcohol in adolescence.

Type
Research paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Authors 2018

Early initiation and habituation to alcohol can change the developing brain and potentially increase the likelihood of addiction to alcohol and other substances( Reference Guerri and Pascual 1 Reference Jacobus and Tapert 3 ). Although alcohol use and alcoholism have strong hereditary origins, environmental factors may independently affect risk or modify genetic and other familial influences( Reference Pagan, Rose and Viken 4 Reference Murray, Tulloch and Chen 7 ). In this context, diet is a potentially modifiable early-life factor that might have an influence on later alcohol use. Animal experiments suggest that excessive sugar consumption can lead to addictive behaviour like the consumption of alcohol or drugs( Reference Vendruscolo, Gueye and Darnaudery 6 Reference Avena, Rada and Hoebel 8 ), and it has been proposed that sugar addiction is also observed in man( Reference Fortuna 9 Reference Lustig 12 ). This claim was criticized because sugar consumption is associated with reward and craving, but other symptoms of addiction such as tolerance or withdrawal are less clear( Reference Benton 13 ). It has also been argued that the compulsive behaviour observed in rodents was not triggered by sucrose itself but by the palatability of it, particularly in combination with fat( Reference Benton 13 , Reference Levine, Kotz and Gosnell 14 ). Although there is evidence for associations between sweet preference and alcohol dependence in human subjects( Reference Fortuna 9 , Reference Eiler, Dzemidzic and Soeurt 15 ), and between fat and ethanol intake in rodents( Reference Barson, Karatayev and Chang 16 ), addictive-like behaviour may manifest itself in overconsumption of preferred foods rather than of a single nutrient( Reference Levine, Kotz and Gosnell 14 ). These overconsumed foods are typically high in sugar, fat and salt, and this pattern of consumption has been summarized as ‘food addiction’( Reference Gearhardt, Davis and Kuschner 17 , Reference Blumenthal and Gold 18 ). Hebebrand et al. highlight the behavioural aspects of overeating and provide evidence for ‘eating addiction’ associated with palatable foods high in fat and sugar in contrast to purely substance-related addictive-like behaviour( Reference Hebebrand, Albayrak and Adan 19 ). Thus, extreme eating behaviours may be indicative of loss of control and craving, which are central features of addiction, and excessive consumption of palatable foods that are high in fat and sugar may serve as one such signal.

Identification of modifiable environmental factors early in life is central to prevention of later alcohol use and abuse. The European IDEFICS/I.Family cohort study was designed to observe developments and changes in health-related behaviours between childhood and adolescence( Reference Ahrens, Siani and Adan 20 ). The present study covers a variety of geographical areas with distinctly different alcohol and food cultures, and has collected information on critical covarying factors such as parenting style, parental alcohol use and socio-economic conditions. Our aim is to explore the relationship between sugar and fat consumption in childhood and subsequent alcohol intake in adolescence, with emphasis on measured confounding and mediating factors.

Participants and methods

The IDEFICS (Identification and prevention of Dietary- and lifestyle-induced health EFfects In Children and infantS) study was initiated in 2006 with survey centres in eight European countries (Belgium, Cyprus, Estonia, Germany, Hungary, Italy, Spain and Sweden). A total of 16 224 children were examined at baseline (2007–08; ages 2–9 years), providing data on diet, lifestyle and anthropometry( Reference Ahrens, Bammann and Siani 21 ). The subsequent I.Family study included a longitudinal follow-up of the baseline cohort (2013–14; ages 8–16 years) as well as information on family members( Reference Ahrens, Siani and Adan 20 ). At the time of the first follow-up (2009–10), the participation rate was 68 %, and attrition was associated with overweight, lower well-being scores and lower parental education and income( Reference Hense, Michels and Mårild 22 ). The final follow-up had a participation rate of 55 %, indicating further attrition( Reference Ahrens, Siani and Adan 20 ).

Assessment of exposure and outcome

Children's dietary exposure to sugar and fat at baseline was based on answers to a forty-three-item FFQ that was filled in by 15 168 parents( Reference Ahrens, Bammann and Siani 21 , Reference Lanfer, Hebestreit and Ahrens 23 Reference Lissner, Lanfer and Gwozdz 26 ). The consumption frequency of foods high in sugar was calculated in times per week, including fruit juice, jam, honey, chocolate or nut-based spread, and sweet snacks, as well as the following items if they contained added sugar: fresh fruit, drinks, milk, yoghurt and breakfast cereals. A propensity to consume foods rich in sugar was defined as the weekly frequency of foods high in sugar divided by the total frequency of all foods assessed in this FFQ. For instance, a value of 25 % for sugar propensity means that a quarter of all food frequencies reported were from sugar-rich foods. A similarly defined fat propensity was based on intake of fried potatoes, high-fat milk and yoghurt, fried fish, meat products, fried eggs, mayonnaise, cheese, chocolate or nut-based spread, butter and margarine, nuts, savoury snacks, chocolate, cake and biscuits, and ice cream. These food groups as well as the propensities have shown relative validity in relation to repeated 24 h recalls( Reference Lissner, Lanfer and Gwozdz 26 ). In spite of some common food items, the mutual correlation between sugar and fat propensities was small in magnitude (r=0·12, P<0·001). In addition to continuous variables we examined categories of high sugar or high fat propensity by dichotomizing the respective propensities at the median percentage. This was done to facilitate interaction and mediation analyses. Finally, a composite indicator was defined comparing the category of both high sugar and high fat propensity with the combination of the three remaining categories: high sugar and low fat, low sugar and high fat, and low sugar and low fat. Sensitivity analyses based on tertiles were performed to demonstrate independence of results on specific cut-off points. Six years after the baseline examination, the adolescents (now aged 11–16 years) were asked to complete an FFQ with questions almost identical to the baseline FFQ, and propensities were calculated that are comparable to those at baseline. The FFQ for adolescents also included one item for alcoholic beverages( Reference Arvidsson, Bogl and Eiben 27 ). Use of alcohol was defined as at least weekly consumption v. less than weekly use, including no use. Dietary information in both childhood and adolescence was complete in 2263 individuals.

Assessment of confounding variables

To adjust for weight status, we included age- and sex-standardized Z-scores of BMI measured at baseline and at follow-up, and overweight at baseline was defined using International Obesity Task Force cut-off points( Reference Cole and Lobstein 28 ). Parental education was classified based on the International Standard Classification of Education( 29 ) and dichotomized at medium level (post-secondary v. less education) reflecting the maximum educational level of both parents. Family income was dichotomized into high v. medium or low income based on standardized country-specific income categories( Reference Bammann, Gwozdz and Lanfer 30 ). Also at baseline, parents were asked to what extent they agreed with the statement ‘At our home it is laid down quite clearly what is allowed and what is not’ and parenting style was categorized into ‘strict’, somewhat strict’ and ‘permissive or rather permissive’( Reference Bammann, Williams and Wolters 31 ). A psychological quality-of-life score (HRQoL) was defined based on four dimensions of the parent- and self-report versions of the health-related quality-of-life KINDL-R questionnaire( Reference Ravens-Sieberer and Bullinger 32 ) (emotional well-being, self-esteem, relations with family, relations with peers) for children and adolescents, respectively, with a range of 12–48, and higher values indicating better HRQoL. At the time of the follow-up, parents were asked to complete the same FFQ as the adolescents and the number of drinks per week was recorded for the parent who completed the FFQ, or the maximum number if both parents answered. In addition to the HRQoL, teens answered questions on their tendency to rash action that were combined into a score (range 12–48) with higher values indicating higher impulsivity( Reference Smith, Fischer and Cyders 33 ). Both HRQoL and impulsivity scores were included as potential correlates of exposure and outcome variables.

Statistical methods

Descriptive statistics are presented as mean and standard deviation for continuous variables and as frequency and percentage for binary variables. Alcohol consumption at follow-up in relation to baseline predictors was assessed using log-binomial regression( Reference Spiegelman and Hertzmark 34 ); the result is given in terms of the relative risk (RR) for alcohol consumption together with the 95 % confidence interval. Exposure variables, namely sugar and fat propensities at baseline, were analysed as (i) continuous variables and categorical variables at (ii) the median or (iii) tertile values. The basic model for alcohol consumption as a function of sugar and fat propensities included age and sex as fixed effects as well as country as a random effect, to account for the low prevalence of ≥weekly alcohol consumption in this age group. Additional models tested the impact of potential confounders, as well as the interaction between high sugar and high fat propensity. To test for country-specific associations between alcohol consumption and earlier dietary exposure we added an interaction term between exposure and country as a random effect. Potential collinearity between predictors was assessed by calculating the variance inflation factor using a linear model for the prevalence of alcohol consumption. All regression models presented here showed variance inflation factor values less than 2. The log-binomial regression models included a random effect to allow for overdispersion and goodness-of-fit was assessed in terms of the scaled Pearson statistics, with values close to 1 indicating a good fit. Complete case analysis was applied for covariates with missing values (<10 % of total sample size, except for parental alcohol use which was available for 1627 adolescents only). Mediation analysis was performed to examine to what extent the prospective association between a preference for foods high in fat and sugar in childhood and alcohol consumption in adolescence was due to sustained consumption of these foods in adolescence. The mediation analysis was conducted using a SAS macro published by Valeri and VanderWeele( Reference Valeri and Vanderweele 35 ) consisting of two regression steps assessing: (i) the association of alcohol use at follow-up with diet at baseline and at follow-up; and (ii) the association between diet at follow-up and diet at baseline. Statistical analyses were performed using the statistical software package SAS version 9.4. Statistical significance was set at P<0·05 (two-sided tests).

Results

Descriptive characteristics of the sample at baseline and at follow-up

At baseline, the children were 5–9 years old and 51 % of them were girls (n 1155). Weight status in terms of age- and sex-specific BMI Z-score did not differ between time points. Half of the families reported a high level of parental education and 37 % reported high income. Compared with childhood, adolescents reported higher fat and lower sugar propensities, and 5 % reported weekly alcohol consumption or more. Additional characteristics for potential confounders or mediators of the association between diet and alcohol consumption are given in Table 1.

Table 1 Participant characteristics at baseline and follow-up among children from eight European countries examined at ages 5–9 years and followed up at ages 11–16 years (n 2263 if not indicated otherwise), IDEFICS/I.Family cohort study

IDEFICS, Identification and prevention of Dietary- and lifestyle-induced health EFfects In Children and infantS; HRQoL, health-related quality of life.

Data presented are mean and sd unless indicated otherwise.

n<2263 due to missing values (cf. online supplementary material, Tables S1 and S2).

Cross-sectional characteristics associated with sugar and fat propensities, and with alcohol consumption

Sugar and fat propensities were dichotomized at their median values of 24·0% and 24·6%, respectively, to examine characteristics of high sugar or high fat propensity (see online supplementary material, Table S1). There were no differences by age or sex between categories of sugar or fat propensity in childhood. Children with high sugar or high fat propensity had lower BMI Z-score compared with reference. High sugar propensity was associated with lower parental education and a less strict parenting style, while high fat propensity was associated with lower HRQoL. Compared with overall averages, children in Italy, Belgium and Germany were most likely to consume foods high in sugar or fat, while children from the Swedish survey centre were least likely to do so (data not shown).

Follow-up characteristics are presented by alcohol consumption status (see online supplementary material, Table S2). Participants were between 11·0 and 16·2 years old, with similar age distribution in both alcohol categories. Boys were more likely to report weekly alcohol intake than girls. Alcohol consumption was associated with higher values of concurrent sugar and fat propensities, but not with weight status. Adolescents consuming alcohol showed lower HRQoL, higher impulsivity and were more likely to report smoking than their non-drinking peers. Parents of alcohol-consuming adolescents reported higher alcohol consumption compared with parents of adolescents who did not drink (point-biserial correlation coefficient=0·06, P=0·01). The country-specific prevalence of ≥weekly alcohol consumption was 7 % (Italy), 6 % (Cyprus), 5 % (Hungary), 4 % (Belgium and Estonia), 2 % (Germany and Spain) and 1 % (Sweden).

Consumption of sugar and fat in childhood and alcohol consumption in adolescence

A higher value of sugar propensity in childhood predicted use of alcohol in adolescence, RR=1·20 (95 % CI 1·01, 1·43) for 1 SD, adjusted for age, sex and country. The corresponding association between fat propensity and alcohol use was somewhat stronger than that for sugar, RR=1·27 (95 % CI 1·09, 1·49) per 1 sd. In a mutually adjusted model, fat propensity was independently predictive of alcohol use, RR=1·25 (95 % CI 1·06, 1·47), but sugar propensity was not, RR=1·16 (95 % CI 0·98, 1·38). A prediction of alcohol use was also observed for the dichotomized sugar and fat propensities, RR=1·62 (95 % CI 1·11, 2·37) for high sugar and RR=1·78 (95 % CI 1·22, 2·59) for high fat propensity, and both associations were observed in the mutually adjusted model, RR=1·54 (95 % CI 1·06, 2·26) and RR=1·71 (95 % CI 1·17, 2·49), respectively.

Table 2 shows the risk of alcohol use as a function of dichotomized propensities including their interaction. The risk of alcohol consumption was more than doubled for the combination of high sugar and high fat propensity compared with the combination of low sugar and low fat propensity (Table 2A), but the interaction between high fat and high sugar consumption was not statistically significant (P=0·4). The association between categories of fat and sugar propensity and later alcohol use was independent of baseline measures of HRQoL, parental education, income and parenting style (Table 2B). Among the latter, only HRQoL was significantly associated with alcohol in the multivariable model, such that higher values were protective for later alcohol use, RR=0·81 (95 % CI 0·67, 0·98) per 1 sd. High parental income at baseline was associated with reduced risk of alcohol use in adolescence, RR=0·55 (95 % CI 0·34, 0·89), adjusted for diet, age, sex and country. Also, a stricter parenting style predicted less alcohol use: RR=0·69 (95 % CI 0·43, 1·13) and RR=0·56 (95 % CI 0·32, 0·97) for somewhat strict and strict v. permissive, respectively, adjusted for diet, age, sex and country.

Table 2 Associations between sugar and fat propensity at baseline and alcohol consumption in adolescence at follow-up among children from eight European countries examined at ages 5–9 years and followed up at ages 11–16 years (n 2263 if not indicated otherwise), IDEFICS/I.Family cohort study

IDEFICS, Identification and prevention of Dietary- and lifestyle-induced health EFfects In Children and infantS; RR, relative risk; HRQoL, health-related quality of life; Ref., reference category.

*P<0·05, ***P<0·001.

n 1931 due to missing values for covariates.

Sensitivity analyses

Figure 1 illustrates the risk of alcohol use as a function of tertiles of sugar and fat propensities. Being in the combination of highest tertiles more than doubled the risk of alcohol use, RR=2·30 (95 % CI 1·16, 4·57) compared with reference. The associations of sugar and fat propensities with alcohol use did not differ by sex, country or other covariates listed in Table 1 (see online supplementary material, Tables S3 and S4). Baseline BMI Z-score did not predict later alcohol use, RR=0·98 (95 % CI 0·84, 1·15) adjusted for diet, age, sex and country, and the positive association between the combination of high sugar and high fat propensity with alcohol consumption was observed in both overweight and non-overweight children (Table S3). Finally, parental alcohol consumption was positively associated with adolescents’ alcohol consumption but not with the children’s diet at baseline or at follow-up. Consequently, adjusting for parental alcohol consumption did not change the associations between childhood diet and later alcohol consumption reported in Table 2.

Fig. 1 (colour online) Relative risk (RR) for use of alcohol in adolescence by sugar and fat propensity tertiles and their combinations at baseline (log-binomial regression adjusted for age, sex and country) among children from eight European countries examined at ages 5–9 years and followed up at ages 11–16 years (n 2263), IDEFICS/I.Family cohort study. *P<0·05 (Ref, reference category (lowest tertile in sugar propensity and in fat propensity); IDEFICS, Identification and prevention of Dietary- and lifestyle-induced health EFfects In Children and infantS)

Is the prospective association between diet and alcohol mediated by diet at follow-up?

Table 2 showed that the combination of high sugar and high fat propensity was the strongest predictor of later alcohol consumption. For the purpose of mediation analysis, we defined a binary variable distinguishing the combination of high sugar and high fat propensity from all other categories combined, both for diets in childhood and in adolescence (Table 2). Both dietary indicators were associated with alcohol consumption. However, mediation analysis showed that the direct effect of diets high in fat and sugar in childhood on later alcohol use was stronger than the indirect effect through sustained preference for these foods in adolescence (Table 3).

Table 3 Mediation analysis: decomposition of the total effect of high sugar and high fat propensity at baseline on subsequent alcohol consumption into direct and indirect effects among children from eight European countries examined at ages 5–9 years and followed up at ages 11–16 years (n 2256), IDEFICS/I.Family cohort study

IDEFICS, Identification and prevention of Dietary- and lifestyle-induced health EFfects In Children and infantS; RR, relative risk.

**P<0·01, ***P<0·001 (adjusted for sex of the child and survey country).

Discussion

In the European, multicentre, IDEFICS/I.Family cohort study we observed that children consuming sugar-rich or high-fat foods were at higher risk of alcohol consumption in adolescence. The associations were independent of age, sex and country, in spite of notable country differences in both exposure and outcome, and of male teenagers being more likely to report alcohol consumption. The highest risk for later alcohol consumption was observed for the combination of above-median sugar and above-median fat propensity, compared with all other combinations. An analysis based on tertiles of sugar and fat propensities confirmed the independence of the result on specific cut-off points. The associations between dietary preferences in childhood and later alcohol use were not explained by strict parenting style, which does not support the assumption that parents restricting their children’s sugar and fat intake would later limit their alcohol consumption. The results were not confounded by health-related quality of life, parental education and income, or parental alcohol use. Although the prevalence of high sugar and high fat consumption did not decline between childhood and adolescence, mediation analysis showed that the direct effect of sugar and fat consumption in childhood was larger than the indirect effect mediated through sustained consumption of sugar and fat into adolescence.

Adolescence represents a period of unique vulnerability for developing addiction( Reference Pagan, Rose and Viken 4 ), with both parents and peers acting as role models and contributing to risk or resilience( Reference Huurre, Lintonen and Kaprio 5 , Reference Latendresse, Rose and Viken 36 , Reference Latendresse, Rose and Viken 37 ). It is a challenge in observational epidemiology to distinguish the effect of an exposure such as dietary preference in childhood from covarying influences such as parenting style or socio-economic factors. On the family level, high parental income was associated with reduced risk of adolescent alcohol consumption, but the risk due to above-median values for sugar and fat propensity was not explained by parental income. Given the strong inter-country differences in both dietary preferences and alcohol consumption, it is remarkable that their mutual association did not differ by country. The survey centre in southern Italy showed the highest prevalence of alcohol consumption among adolescents, while the prevalence in the Swedish survey was lowest. This illustrates the cultural differences between countries, with higher social acceptability of alcohol drinking in minors in some areas, and the influence of more restrictive alcohol policies in others( Reference Babor 38 ). Italian children also showed a high propensity to consume sugar, while Swedish children had lowest scores for both sugar and fat propensity.

Despite this new evidence for an effect of childhood diet on later alcohol consumption, the identification of causal pathways remains difficult. In contrast to animal models it is difficult to determine the effects of single nutrients in human studies as they are often consumed together. As a candidate for a physiological link between consumption of diet in childhood and alcohol use, we mention the strong main effect of high fat propensity on alcohol consumption that was independent of sugar propensity. Mutual reinforcement of ethanol and fat consumption has been demonstrated in rodents( Reference Barson, Karatayev and Chang 16 ) and our findings may constitute a similar phenomenon in man. However, the large combined effect of high sugar and high fat propensity underlines the importance of taste preference( Reference Levine, Kotz and Gosnell 14 , Reference Gearhardt, Davis and Kuschner 17 , Reference Blumenthal and Gold 18 ) that might predispose overconsumers of palatable foods to regular use of alcohol. Future studies on this cohort, using techniques ranging from genotyping to brain imaging, may shed more light on the addictive potential of palatable foods in adolescents and families.

Among the strengths of the present study is the comprehensive assessment of lifestyle factors of incipient users of alcohol in their different cultural contexts. The consistency of positive associations between sugar, fat and alcohol in spite of large between-country differences in consumption patterns underlines the robustness of the results. The longitudinal design with clear separation between measurements in childhood and in adolescence six years later strengthens the level of evidence that can be attributed to this observational study. The validity of food groups in the FFQ was reported at baseline( Reference Lanfer, Hebestreit and Ahrens 23 , Reference Huybrechts, Bornhorst and Pala 25 ), except for the question on alcohol that was added at follow-up. However, the plausibility of the latter is supported by the concomitant correlation with higher impulsivity, smoking and parental alcohol consumption. A main study limitation is sample size, with just over 100 regular users of alcohol among the adolescents, restricting the possibility for subgroup and interaction analyses. Because attrition at follow-up was associated with overweight, lower well-being scores and lower parental education and income( Reference Ahrens, Siani and Adan 20 , Reference Hense, Michels and Mårild 22 ), prevalence of alcohol use as well as of specific dietary patterns may be under- or overestimated. However, non-participation should not affect the size of associations between childhood diet and alcohol use in adolescence, as confirmed by adjustment for these measured confounding factors. Another important limitation is residual confounding due to unmeasured factors that could drive both ingestive behaviours we are studying. For instance, weak associations with parenting style could indicate that the variable was not specifically assessing supervision of dietary intake but capturing other aspects of family life. It is thus still possible that strict norms on both palatable foods and alcohol could explain their mutual association observed here. The lack of an age effect on alcohol consumption could be viewed as another limitation, possibly related to the fact that the age distribution is narrow compared with the entire course of adolescent development, when initiation to alcohol typically occurs( Reference Kosterman, Hawkins and Guo 39 ). Finally, we note that higher propensities for sugar and fat intake were associated with lower BMI Z-scores. While this direction of associations may be a sign of reverse causation, i.e. parents of overweight children under-reporting intakes of sugar and fat, we showed that weight status did not confound or modify any of the findings reported here.

Conclusion

The present study explored the relationship between children’s consumption of sugar-rich and high-fat foods and their alcohol consumption in adolescence. The results suggest a positive association, which was only partly explained by important confounders measured in the study, or by sugar and fat consumption sustained into adolescence. The combined effect of fat and sugar might suggest habituation to palatability rather than effects of specific substances, although our method could not identify effects of single macronutrients. Covariation with poor mental health and lower parental income suggests that there are societal groups of particular vulnerability, in times when the abundance of cheap, unhealthy foods encourages unhealthy coping mechanisms. This calls for interventions increasing the awareness about the long-term impact of healthy eating early in life( Reference Gearhardt, Grilo and DiLeone 40 ), which may ultimately lead to legislative changes that influence reformulation, production, advertisement and distribution of unhealthy food products.

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements: This work was done as part of the IDEFICS (http://www.idefics.eu) and I.Family studies (http://www.ifamilystudy.eu/). Financial support: The authors gratefully acknowledge the financial support of the European Commission within the Sixth RTD Framework Programme (Contract No. 016181 (FOOD)) and the Seventh RTD Framework Programme (Contract No. 266044), and funding within the EpiLife research programme (Swedish Research Council for Working Life and Social Research (grant number 2006-1506); Swedish Research Council for Environment, Agricultural Sciences, and Spatial Planning (grant number 2012-00038); Swedish Research Council (grant number 2015-02508)). The funding agencies had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article. Conflict of interest: None. Authorship: W.A., S.D.H., D.M., V.P., P.R., M.T., T.V., J.K. and L.L. designed and conducted the study; L.L. and J.K. developed the research question; K.M. analysed the data and drafted the manuscript; K.M., L.L., M.H., W.A., L.H.B., J.K., I.I., H.J., V.P. and P.R. wrote the manuscript; K.M. and L.L. had primary responsibility for the final content. All authors read and approved the final manuscript. Ethics of human subject participation: This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures involving human subjects were approved by the relevant local or national ethics committee for each survey centre. Parents and adolescents gave written informed consent for all examinations as well as for the collection and storage of personal data and biological samples. The Pan-European IDEFICS/I.Family children cohort is registered at the ISRCTN clinical trials registry (ID ISRCTN62310987; https://doi.org/10.1186/ISRCTN62310987).

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980018001829

References

1. Guerri, C & Pascual, M (2010) Mechanisms involved in the neurotoxic, cognitive, and neurobehavioral effects of alcohol consumption during adolescence. Alcohol 44, 1526.Google Scholar
2. Maldonado-Devincci, AM, Badanich, KA & Kirstein, CL (2010) Alcohol during adolescence selectively alters immediate and long-term behavior and neurochemistry. Alcohol 44, 5766.Google Scholar
3. Jacobus, J & Tapert, SF (2013) Neurotoxic effects of alcohol in adolescence. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 9, 703721.Google Scholar
4. Pagan, JL, Rose, RJ, Viken, RJ et al. (2006) Genetic and environmental influences on stages of alcohol use across adolescence and into young adulthood. Behav Genet 36, 483497.Google Scholar
5. Huurre, T, Lintonen, T, Kaprio, J et al. (2010) Adolescent risk factors for excessive alcohol use at age 32 years. A 16-year prospective follow-up study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 45, 125134.Google Scholar
6. Vendruscolo, LF, Gueye, AB, Darnaudery, M et al. (2010) Sugar overconsumption during adolescence selectively alters motivation and reward function in adult rats. PLoS One 5, e9296.Google Scholar
7. Murray, SM, Tulloch, AJ, Chen, EY et al. (2015) Insights revealed by rodent models of sugar binge eating. CNS Spectr 20, 530536.Google Scholar
8. Avena, NM, Rada, P & Hoebel, BG (2009) Sugar and fat bingeing have notable differences in addictive-like behavior. J Nutr 139, 623628.Google Scholar
9. Fortuna, JL (2010) Sweet preference, sugar addiction and the familial history of alcohol dependence: shared neural pathways and genes. J Psychoactive Drugs 42, 147151.Google Scholar
10. Ahmed, SH, Guillem, K & Vandaele, Y (2013) Sugar addiction: pushing the drug–sugar analogy to the limit. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care 16, 434439.Google Scholar
11. Bray, GA (2016) Is sugar addictive? Diabetes 65, 17971799.Google Scholar
12. Lustig, RH (2013) Fructose: it’s ‘alcohol without the buzz’. Adv Nutr 4, 226235.Google Scholar
13. Benton, D (2010) The plausibility of sugar addiction and its role in obesity and eating disorders. Clin Nutr 29, 288303.Google Scholar
14. Levine, AS, Kotz, CM & Gosnell, BA (2003) Sugars and fats: the neurobiology of preference. J Nutr 133, issue 3, 831S834S.Google Scholar
15. Eiler, WJA 2nd, Dzemidzic, M, Soeurt, CM et al. (2018) Family history of alcoholism and the human brain response to oral sucrose. Neuroimage Clin 17, 10361046.Google Scholar
16. Barson, JR, Karatayev, O, Chang, GQ et al. (2009) Positive relationship between dietary fat, ethanol intake, triglycerides, and hypothalamic peptides: counteraction by lipid-lowering drugs. Alcohol 43, 433441.Google Scholar
17. Gearhardt, AN, Davis, C, Kuschner, R et al. (2011) The addiction potential of hyperpalatable foods. Curr Drug Abuse Rev 4, 140145.Google Scholar
18. Blumenthal, DM & Gold, MS (2010) Neurobiology of food addiction. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care 13, 359365.Google Scholar
19. Hebebrand, J, Albayrak, O, Adan, R et al. (2014) ‘Eating addiction’, rather than ‘food addiction’, better captures addictive-like eating behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 47, 295306.Google Scholar
20. Ahrens, W, Siani, A, Adan, R et al. (2017) Cohort profile: the transition from childhood to adolescence in European children – how I.Family extends the IDEFICS cohort. Int J Epidemiol 46, 1394j1395j.Google Scholar
21. Ahrens, W, Bammann, K, Siani, A et al. (2011) The IDEFICS cohort: design, characteristics and participation in the baseline survey. Int J Obes (Lond) 35, Suppl. 1, S3S15.Google Scholar
22. Hense, SPH, Michels, N, Mårild, S et al. (2013) Determinants of attrition to follow-up in a multicentre cohort study in children – results from the IDEFICS study. Epidemiol Res Int 2013, 936365.Google Scholar
23. Lanfer, A, Hebestreit, A, Ahrens, W et al. (2011) Reproducibility of food consumption frequencies derived from the Children’s Eating Habits Questionnaire used in the IDEFICS study. Int J Obes (Lond) 35, Suppl. 1, S61S68.Google Scholar
24. Bel-Serrat, S, Mouratidou, T, Pala, V et al. (2014) Relative validity of the Children’s Eating Habits Questionnaire-food frequency section among young European children: the IDEFICS Study. Public Health Nutr 17, 266276.Google Scholar
25. Huybrechts, I, Bornhorst, C, Pala, V et al. (2011) Evaluation of the Children’s Eating Habits Questionnaire used in the IDEFICS study by relating urinary calcium and potassium to milk consumption frequencies among European children. Int J Obes (Lond) 35, Suppl. 1, S69S78.Google Scholar
26. Lissner, L, Lanfer, A, Gwozdz, W et al. (2012) Television habits in relation to overweight, diet and taste preferences in European children: the IDEFICS study. Eur J Epidemiol 27, 705715.Google Scholar
27. Arvidsson, L, Bogl, LH, Eiben, G et al. (2015) Fat, sugar and water intakes among families from the IDEFICS intervention and control groups: first observations from I.Family. Obes Rev 16, Suppl. 2, 127137.Google Scholar
28. Cole, TJ & Lobstein, T (2012) Extended international (IOTF) body mass index cut-offs for thinness, overweight and obesity. Pediatr Obes 7, 284294.Google Scholar
29. United Nations Educational Scientific and Cultural Organization (2006) ISCED 1997 International Standard Classification of Education. Montreal: UNESCO Institute for Statistics.Google Scholar
30. Bammann, K, Gwozdz, W, Lanfer, A et al. (2013) Socioeconomic factors and childhood overweight in Europe: results from the multi-centre IDEFICS study. Pediatr Obes 8, 112.Google Scholar
31. Bammann, KRL, Williams, GD & Wolters, M (2018) B7: core questionnaires, IDEFICS & I.Family instruments. In Instruments for Health Surveys in Children and Adolescents. Springer Series Epidemiology & Public Health. Heidelberg: Springer (In the Press).Google Scholar
32. Ravens-Sieberer, U & Bullinger, M (1998) Assessing health-related quality of life in chronically ill children with the German KINDL: first psychometric and content analytical results. Qual Life Res 7, 399407.Google Scholar
33. Smith, GT, Fischer, S, Cyders, MA et al. (2007) On the validity and utility of discriminating among impulsivity-like traits. Assessment 14, 155a70.Google Scholar
34. Spiegelman, D & Hertzmark, E (2005) Easy SAS calculations for risk or prevalence ratios and differences. Am J Epidemiol 162, 199200.Google Scholar
35. Valeri, L & Vanderweele, TJ (2013) Mediation analysis allowing for exposure-mediator interactions and causal interpretation: theoretical assumptions and implementation with SAS and SPSS macros. Psychol Methods 18, 137150.Google Scholar
36. Latendresse, SJ, Rose, RJ, Viken, RJ et al. (2008) Parenting mechanisms in links between parents’ and adolescents’ alcohol use behaviors. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 32, 322330.Google Scholar
37. Latendresse, SJ, Rose, RJ, Viken, RJ et al. (2010) Examining the etiology of associations between perceived parenting and adolescents’ alcohol use: common genetic and/or environmental liabilities? J Stud Alcohol Drugs 71, 313325.Google Scholar
38. Babor, T (2010) Alcohol: No Ordinary Commodity: Research and Public Policy, 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
39. Kosterman, R, Hawkins, JD, Guo, J et al. (2000) The dynamics of alcohol and marijuana initiation: patterns and predictors of first use in adolescence. Am J Public Health 90, 360366.Google Scholar
40. Gearhardt, AN, Grilo, CM, DiLeone, RJ et al. (2011) Can food be addictive? Public health and policy implications. Addiction 106, 12081212.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Participant characteristics at baseline and follow-up among children from eight European countries examined at ages 5–9 years and followed up at ages 11–16 years (n 2263 if not indicated otherwise), IDEFICS/I.Family cohort study

Figure 1

Table 2 Associations between sugar and fat propensity at baseline and alcohol consumption in adolescence at follow-up among children from eight European countries examined at ages 5–9 years and followed up at ages 11–16 years (n 2263 if not indicated otherwise), IDEFICS/I.Family cohort study

Figure 2

Fig. 1 (colour online) Relative risk (RR) for use of alcohol in adolescence by sugar and fat propensity tertiles and their combinations at baseline (log-binomial regression adjusted for age, sex and country) among children from eight European countries examined at ages 5–9 years and followed up at ages 11–16 years (n 2263), IDEFICS/I.Family cohort study. *P<0·05 (Ref, reference category (lowest tertile in sugar propensity and in fat propensity); IDEFICS, Identification and prevention of Dietary- and lifestyle-induced health EFfects In Children and infantS)

Figure 3

Table 3 Mediation analysis: decomposition of the total effect of high sugar and high fat propensity at baseline on subsequent alcohol consumption into direct and indirect effects among children from eight European countries examined at ages 5–9 years and followed up at ages 11–16 years (n 2256), IDEFICS/I.Family cohort study

Supplementary material: PDF

Mehlig et al. supplementary material

Table S1-S4

Download Mehlig et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 76.7 KB