No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Perceptual (roots of) core knowledge
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 27 June 2024
Abstract
Some core knowledge may be rooted in – or even identical to – well-characterized mechanisms of mid-level visual perception and attention. In the decades since it was first proposed, this possibility has inspired (and has been supported by) several discoveries in both infant cognition and adult perception, but it also faces several challenges. To what degree does What Babies Know reflect how babies see and attend?
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Bai, D. (2023). Intuitive physics in visual perception. PhD dissertation, l'Ecole Normale Supérieure.Google Scholar
Battaglia, P. W., Hamrick, J. B., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2013). Simulation as an engine of physical scene understanding. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 18327–18332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carey, S., & Xu, F. (2001). Infants knowledge of objects: Beyond object-files and object tracking. Cognition, 80, 179–213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, Y.-C., & Scholl, B. J. (2016). The perception of history: Seeing causal history in static shapes induces illusory motion perception. Psychological Science, 27, 923–930.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheries, E. W., Mitroff, S. R., Wynn, K., & Scholl, B. J. (2008). Cohesion as a constraint on object persistence in infancy. Developmental Science, 11, 427–432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheries, E. W., Mitroff, S. R., Wynn, K., & Scholl, B. J. (2009). Do the same principles constrain persisting object representations in infant cognition and adult perception?: The cases of continuity and cohesion. In Hood, B. & Santos, L. (Eds.), The origins of object knowledge (pp. 107–134). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Freitas, J., & Alvarez, G. (2018). Your visual system provides all the information you need to make moral judgments about generic visual events. Cognition, 178, 133–146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dewar, K., & Xu, F. (2007). Do 9-month-old infants expect distinct words to refer to kinds? Developmental Psychology, 43, 1227–1238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Firestone, C., & Scholl, B. J. (2016). Cognition does not affect perception: Evaluating the evidence for “top-down” effects. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39(E229), 1–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gao, T., McCarthy, G., & Scholl, B. J. (2010). The wolfpack effect: Perception of animacy irresistibly influences interactive behavior. Psychological Science, 21, 1845–1853.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gao, T., Newman, G. E., & Scholl, B. J. (2009). The psychophysics of chasing: A case study in the perception of animacy. Cognitive Psychology, 59, 154–179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gao, T., & Scholl, B. J. (2011). Chasing vs. stalking: Interrupting the perception of animacy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 37, 669–684.Google ScholarPubMed
Gergeley, G., Nadasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Biro, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56, 165–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, E. J., & Wang, S.-h. (2019). Comparison facilitates the use of height information by 5-month-olds in containment events. Developmental Psychology, 55, 2475–2482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature, 450, 557–559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hespos, S. J., & Baillargeon, R. (2001). Infants’ knowledge about occlusion and containment events: A surprising discrepancy. Psychological Science, 12, 141–147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huntley-Fenner, G., Carey, S., & Solimando, A. (2002). Objects are individuals but stuff doesn't count: Perceived rigidity and cohesiveness influence infants’ representations of small numbers of discrete entities. Cognition, 85, 203–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leslie, A. M., Xu, F., Tremoulet, P., & Scholl, B. J. (1998). Indexing and the object concept: Developing “what” and “where” systems. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 10–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitroff, S. R., Scholl, B. J., & Wynn, K. (2004). Divide and conquer: How object files adapt when a persisting object splits into two. Psychological Science, 15, 420–425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakayama, K., He, Z. J., & Shimojo, S. (1995). Visual surface representation: A critical link between lower-level and higher-level vision. In Kosslyn, S. (Ed.), Visual cognition (pp. 1–70). Volume 2 of An Invitation to Cognitive Science, 2nd ed. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Noles, N. S., Scholl, B. J., & Mitroff, S. R. (2005). The persistence of object file representations. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 324–334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ongchoco, J. D. K., Wong, K., & Scholl, B. J. (2023). The “unfinishedness” of dynamic events is spontaneously extracted in visual processing: A new “Visual Zeigarnik Effect”. Journal of Vision, 23, 4974.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scholl, B. J. (2001). Objects and attention: The state of the art. Cognition, 80, 1–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scholl, B. J. (2007). Object persistence in philosophy and psychology. Mind & Language, 22, 563–591.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scholl, B. J., & Leslie, A. M. (1999). Explaining the infant's object concept: Beyond the perception/cognition dichotomy. In Lepore, E. & Pylyshyn, Z. (Eds.), What is cognitive science? (pp. 26–73). Blackwell.Google Scholar
Southgate, V., & Csibra, G. (2009). Inferring the outcome of an ongoing novel action at 13 months. Developmental Psychology, 45, 1794–1798.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spelke, E. (1988). Where perceiving ends and thinking begins: The apprehension of objects in infancy. In Yonas, A. (Ed.), Perceptual development in infancy (pp. 197–234). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Spelke, E., & Van de Walle, G. (1993). Perceiving and reasoning about objects: Insights from infants. In Eilan, N., McCarthy, R. & Brewer, W. (Eds.), Spatial representation (pp. 132–161). Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Strickland, B., & Scholl, B. J. (2015). Visual perception involves “event type” representations: The case of containment vs. occlusion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 570–580.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turk-Browne, N. B., Scholl, B. J., & Chun, M. M. (2008). Babies and brains: Habituation in infant cognition and functional neuroimaging. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 2, Article 16.Google ScholarPubMed
Ullman, T. D., Spelke, E. S., Battaglia, P., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2017). Mind games: Game engines as an architecture for intuitive physics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21, 649–665.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Buren, B., Uddenberg, S., & Scholl, B. J. (2016). The automaticity of perceiving animacy: Goal-directed motion in simple shapes influences visuomotor behavior even when task-irrelevant. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 797–802.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
vanMarle, K., & Scholl, B. J. (2003). Attentive tracking of objects versus substances. Psychological Science, 14, 498–504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, S., Baillargeon, R., & Brueckner, L. (2004). Young infants’ reasoning about hidden objects: Evidence from violation-of-expectation tasks with test trials only. Cognition, 93, 167–198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wong, K. W., Bi, W., Soltani, A., Yildirim, I., & Scholl, B. J. (2023). Seeing soft materials draped over objects: A case study of intuitive physics in perception, attention, and memory. Psychological Science, 34, 111–119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, F. (2002). The role of language in acquiring object kind concepts in infancy. Cognition, 85, 223–250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Target article
Précis of What Babies Know
Related commentaries (25)
Concepts, core knowledge, and the rationalism–empiricism debate
Core knowledge and its role in explaining uniquely human cognition: Some questions
Core knowledge as a neuro-ethologist views it
Core knowledge, visual illusions, and the discovery of the self
Developmental origin of a language–cognition interface in infants: Gateway to advancing core knowledge?
Divisive language
Early pragmatic expectations in human infancy
Evidence for core social goal understanding (and, perhaps, core morality) in preverbal infants
How do babies come to know what babies know?
How important is it to learn language rather than create it?
Investigating infant knowledge with representational similarity analysis
Is core knowledge a natural subdivision of infant cognition?
Is there only one innate modular system for spatial navigation?
Learning in the social being system
More than language is needed to represent and combine different core knowledge components
Not all core knowledge systems are created equal, and they are subject to revision in both children and adults
Perceptual (roots of) core knowledge
Questioning the nature and origins of the “social agent” concept
Substances as a core domain
The brain origins of early social cognition
The key to understanding core knowledge resides in the fetus
The role of language in transcending core knowledge
What we don't know about what babies know: Reconsidering psychophysics, exploration, and infant behavior
Where is the baby in core knowledge?
Wired for society? From ego-logy to eco-logy
Author response
Response to commentaries on What Babies Know