Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T05:27:07.483Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

How the minimalist model of ownership psychology can aid in explaining moral behaviors under resource constraints

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 October 2023

Panagiotis Mitkidis
Affiliation:
Department of Management, Aarhus University, Fuglesangs alle 4, Aarhus V, Denmark [email protected]; https://pure.au.dk/portal/en/persons/panagiotis-mitkidis(ca495362-7cd4-473c-99a9-aad26088e872).html [email protected]; https://pure.au.dk/portal/en/persons/christian-truelsen-elbaek(24b06155-fef3-417b-8dc2-f33b86d1e117).html
Christian T. Elbaek
Affiliation:
Department of Management, Aarhus University, Fuglesangs alle 4, Aarhus V, Denmark [email protected]; https://pure.au.dk/portal/en/persons/panagiotis-mitkidis(ca495362-7cd4-473c-99a9-aad26088e872).html [email protected]; https://pure.au.dk/portal/en/persons/christian-truelsen-elbaek(24b06155-fef3-417b-8dc2-f33b86d1e117).html

Abstract

The model of ownership psychology as a cognitive adaptation proposes that people flexibly navigate cognitive systems of cooperation and competition, thus enabling them to justify unethical behavior. We discuss how this model captures previous accounts of unethical behavior and propose that a disengagement heuristic can help us understand recent findings in the interconnection between scarcity psychology and unethical behavior.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Central to the model of ownership psychology as a cognitive adaptation is the assumption about general respect of ownership as a consequence of cooperation expectations (Boyer, Reference Boyer2022). Boyer suggests that ownership is derived from the interaction of two cognitive systems, one that handles competition for resources and another that handles anticipated cooperation. These systems are argued to account for the flexibility of people's intuitions about ownership. This flexibility in turn allows criminal agents not to be inconsistent in their ownership intuitions, but simply to not include their unethical behavior “in the range of cooperation intuitions” (Boyer, Reference Boyer2022, p. 32). As Boyer argues: “It is a familiar observation that most criminals think in terms of Us vs. Them, explicitly differentiate their world from the ‘regular’ world of their victims” (Boyer, Reference Boyer2022, p. 32).

There are two previous accounts on (im)moral behavior that we see as interestingly connected by Boyer's theoretical framework; (1) the theory of morality-as-cooperation, which argues that morality promotes cooperation (Greene, Reference Greene2015) and has evolved to solve recurrent problems of cooperation and therefore that certain moral behaviors (e.g., reciprocating, helping kin) are considered universally “good” across societies, as they facilitate cooperation (Curry, Mullins, & Whitehouse, Reference Curry, Mullins and Whitehouse2019) and (2) moral disengagement, a cognitive heuristic, which influences peoples justifications for engaging in (im)moral behavior by reducing anticipatory guilt, when individuals distance themselves from the potential consequences of their decisions and act in ways that do not involve their own identity (Bandura, Reference Bandura2017). Boyer's framework on ownership links these two accounts of moral decision-making, by identifying that ownership cognition handles cooperation versus competition problems, while at the same time allowing for some degree of flexibility between behaving ethically (vs. unethically), which in turn allows for justifications and disengagement to kick in.

A general limitation of the model is not in the perspective it presents, but rather in what it leaves out. Here, we provide a perspective on how Boyer's model of ownership could explain how and why people decide to engage in unethical behavior by using a disengagement heuristic to rationalize their unethical behavior (Shu, Gino, & Bazerman, Reference Shu, Gino and Bazerman2011), particularly when contextual cues of resource scarcity (vs. abundance) (Elbaek, Mitkidis, Aarøe, & Otterbring, Reference Elbaek, Mitkidis, Aarøe and Otterbring2021a, Reference Elbaek, Mitkidis, Aarøe and Otterbring2021b; Elbæk, Mitkidis, Aarøe, & Otterbring, Reference Elbæk, Mitkidis, Aarøe and Otterbring2022; Mitkidis et al., Reference Mitkidis, Lindeløv, Elbaek, Porubanova, Grzymala-Moszczynska and Ariely2022) and competition (vs. cooperation) (Kilduff, Galinsky, Gallo, & Reade, Reference Kilduff, Galinsky, Gallo and Reade2016; Schurr & Ritov, Reference Schurr and Ritov2016) are available and influence people's downstream unethical behavior. For example, if the situation is one in which resources are scarce and/or competition is high, agents may adopt a competitive maximizing mindset (Goldsmith, Roux, & Ma, Reference Goldsmith, Roux and Ma2018), making them more likely to prioritize their own interests and possessions over those of others, thus discounting the potential benefits of cooperation. Conversely, if the situation is one in which resources are abundant and/or cooperation appears beneficial, people may be more willing to share resources.

In relation to resource scarcity (vs. abundance) it should here be noted that material resource scarcity can be categorized in different states (chronic vs. acute) and types (i.e., financial, physiological, low socioeconomic status [SES]) with a recent meta-analysis stating the mixed nature of results on the impact of material scarcity on unethical behavior and finding that acute (vs. chronic) scarcity can increase unethical economic behavior (Elbaek et al., Reference Elbaek, Mitkidis, Aarøe and Otterbring2021a). Boyer's theoretical framework might be able to theoretically explain this finding, as acute scarcity could function as a disengagement heuristic, in turn activating competitive attitudes for scarce resources. That is, as the acute experience of scarcity can trigger a competitive mindset aimed at regaining resources in the short-term, this allows individuals to morally disengage from unethical actions as such can serve to restore resources instantly, which cooperation might not. Previous research on how competition might increase agent's propensity to engage in unethical behavior, and provides justifications for such, corroborates this hypothesis (Goldsmith et al., Reference Goldsmith, Roux and Ma2018; Kilduff et al., Reference Kilduff, Galinsky, Gallo and Reade2016; Schurr & Ritov, Reference Schurr and Ritov2016).

At the same time, recent large-scale cross-cultural research, across 67 countries, on how chronic material scarcity affects moral judgment and decision-making suggests that low SES and income inequality, as forms of chronic material scarcity on the individual and macro-level, respectively, are associated with increased focus on acting according to universal moral behaviors as outlined morality-as-cooperation (e.g., reciprocating, helping kin) and increased prosocial intentions aimed toward others (i.e., donating to charities) (Elbaek, Mitkidis, Aarøe, & Otterbring, Reference Elbaek, Mitkidis, Aarøe and Otterbring2021b). This line of work highlights that when resources are persistently scarce, cooperative attitudes might be activated to deal with the chronic form of scarcity, as cooperation can aid in generating better outcomes for resource-deprived individuals. This concurs with previous research on how chronically resource-deprived individuals increase their social orientation and act less individualistic in contexts of resource scarcity, as an adaptation to harsh environments substantiates this hypothesis (Kraus, Piff, Mendoza-Denton, Rheinschmidt, & Keltner, Reference Kraus, Piff, Mendoza-Denton, Rheinschmidt and Keltner2012; Piff, Kraus, Côté, Cheng, & Keltner, Reference Piff, Kraus, Côté, Cheng and Keltner2010).

In sum, we propose that Boyer's (Reference Boyer2022) model of ownership psychology as a cognitive adaptation can aid in further understanding inconsistencies in the current state-of-the art on how experiences of resource scarcity might influence people's propensity to either engage in immoral behaviors (e.g., stealing resources) or cooperative behaviors (e.g., sharing resources). That is, acute resource scarcity can highlight competition and in turn probe the activation of a maximizing mindset (Goldsmith et al., Reference Goldsmith, Roux and Ma2018), which can lead individuals to morally disengage from unethical actions aimed at regaining resources (Elbaek et al., Reference Elbaek, Mitkidis, Aarøe and Otterbring2021a, Reference Elbaek, Mitkidis, Aarøe and Otterbring2021b), while chronic resource scarcity can probe cooperative behaviors, because agents can observe that such investments can aid prospective resource acquisition.

Yet further work is needed in the field of psychology of scarcity and behavioral ethics, exploring, for example, how different states (chronic vs. acute) or forms (financial scarcity, physiological scarcity, and lower social class) of resource scarcity interact with ownership intuitions and correspond to different behavioral outcomes. We believe it is worth expanding on possible interactions of the formal model's predictive outcomes, to propose both theoretical and practical recommendations for future research and policy making.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Competing interest

None.

References

Bandura, A. (2017). Moral disengagement in the perpetration of inhumanities. In Recent developments in criminological theory (pp. 135152). Routledge.10.4324/9781315089089-12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyer, P. (2022). Ownership psychology as a cognitive adaptation: A minimalist model. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 135.10.1017/S0140525X22002527CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curry, O. S., Mullins, D. A., & Whitehouse, H. (2019). Is it good to cooperate? Testing the theory of morality-as-cooperation in 60 societies. Current Anthropology, 60(1), 4769.10.1086/701478CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elbaek, C., Mitkidis, P., Aarøe, L., & Otterbring, T. (2021a). Material scarcity and unethical economic behavior: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Research Square. https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-800481/v2.Google Scholar
Elbaek, C., Mitkidis, P., Aarøe, L., & Otterbring, T. (2021b). Social class and income inequality is associated with morality: Empirical evidence from 67 countries. Research Square. https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-1082570/v1.Google Scholar
Elbæk, C. T., Mitkidis, P., Aarøe, L., & Otterbring, T. (2022). Honestly hungry: Acute hunger does not increase unethical economic behaviour. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 101, 104312.10.1016/j.jesp.2022.104312CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldsmith, K., Roux, C., & Ma, J. (2018). When seeking the best brings out the worst in consumers: Understanding the relationship between a maximizing mindset and immoral behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 28(2), 293309.10.1002/jcpy.1017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greene, J. D. (2015). The rise of moral cognition. Cognition, 135, 3942.10.1016/j.cognition.2014.11.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kilduff, G. J., Galinsky, A. D., Gallo, E., & Reade, J. J. (2016). Whatever it takes to win: Rivalry increases unethical behavior. Academy of Management Journal, 59(5), 15081534.10.5465/amj.2014.0545CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kraus, M. W., Piff, P. K., Mendoza-Denton, R., Rheinschmidt, M. L., & Keltner, D. (2012). Social class, solipsism, and contextualism: How the rich are different from the poor. Psychological Review, 119(3), 546.10.1037/a0028756CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitkidis, P., Lindeløv, J. K., Elbaek, C. T., Porubanova, M., Grzymala-Moszczynska, J., & Ariely, D. (2022). Morality in the time of cognitive famine: The effects of memory load on cooperation and honesty. Acta Psychologica, 228, 103664.10.1016/j.actpsy.2022.103664CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piff, P. K., Kraus, M. W., Côté, S., Cheng, B. H., & Keltner, D. (2010). Having less, giving more: The influence of social class on prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(5), 771.10.1037/a0020092CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schurr, A., & Ritov, I. (2016). Winning a competition predicts dishonest behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(7), 17541759.10.1073/pnas.1515102113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shu, L. L., Gino, F., & Bazerman, M. H. (2011). Dishonest deed, clear conscience: When cheating leads to moral disengagement and motivated forgetting. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(3), 330349.10.1177/0146167211398138CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed