Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T09:24:38.391Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Developing and applying a socio-ecological model to the promotion of healthy eating in the school

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 October 2011

Nick Townsend*
Affiliation:
British Heart Foundation Health Promotion Research Group, University of Oxford and National Obesity Observatory, BHF HPRG, Old Road Campus, Headington, Oxford OX3 7LF, UK
Charlie Foster
Affiliation:
British Heart Foundation Health Promotion Research Group, University of Oxford and National Obesity Observatory, BHF HPRG, Old Road Campus, Headington, Oxford OX3 7LF, UK
*
*Corresponding author: Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

To develop and apply a socio-ecological model (SEM) for healthy eating in school students, to better understand the association between factors at different levels of the SEM and pupils’ dietary choices.

Design

Student-level data, collected through anonymised questionnaires, included reported dietary choices and correlates to these; data on school approaches to food were collected through postal surveys. We used multilevel analysis to study the association of each level of the SEM on student dietary choice while controlling for factors found at other levels.

Setting

Data were collected from secondary schools in Wales that were a part of the 2005/2006 Health Behaviour in School-aged Children (HBSC) study.

Subjects

The final sample for analysis included data from 6693 students aged 11–16 years and 289 teachers from sixty-four secondary schools in Wales.

Results

Student interpersonal factors, an individual's social environment, had a greater association with the dietary choices students made for lunch than student intrapersonal characteristics, those that reside within the person, which were found to have a greater association with the dietary choices made outside school. School organisational factors, such as rules and policies, had a greater association with whether students ate unhealthy foods, whereas the community nature of the school had a greater association with the choosing of healthy foods.

Conclusions

Using the SEM and multilevel analysis allowed us to study how factors were associated with the choice of different foods at different times of the day by students. Interventions can use an SEM to target specific correlates and change health outcomes in the school.

Type
HOT TOPIC – School food
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2011 

Schools are a popular area for health promotion for young people and adolescents as they can target a large population in an economically efficient manner(Reference Pyle, Sharkey and Yetter1). As children spend more time in schools than any other environment outside the home(Reference Ball, Timperio and Crawford2, Reference Story, Kaphingst and French3), they provide unparalleled access through continuous and intensive contact.

Current school health promotion approaches, developed in light of the failure of traditional didactic approaches of health education, are implemented through the ‘settings’ approach(Reference Lister-Sharp, Chapman and Stewart-Brown4Reference Lynagh, Schofield and Sanson-Fisher8). This settings approach recognises the impact the relationship between individuals and their environment has on their health(Reference Green and Kreuter9). This is articulated in schools through the health promoting school concept(1013) which has been embraced internationally as an effective method for promoting the health of children, adolescents and the wider school community(10, 11, Reference Mũkoma and Flisher14, Reference West, Sweeting and Leyland15).

The conceptual base for a settings approach is the socio-ecological model (SEM) of health promotion that was advocated in the Ottawa Charter for Health Promotion(16). There are two key concepts of this approach: (i) behaviour affects and is affected by multiple levels of influence; and (ii) individual behaviour shapes and is shaped by the social environment (reciprocal causation). The SEM emphasises the interaction between, and interdependence of, factors within and across all levels of health behaviour, recognising that most public health challenges are too complex to be adequately understood and addressed from single-level analyses(Reference Stokols17). In doing so it demands that individuals are not viewed in isolation from the larger social units in which they live their lives, pointing to the need to create environmental conditions that support, and promote, effective and sustainable behaviour change.

SEM for health have been adopted in dealing with many health behaviours(Reference Stokols17Reference King, Stokols and Talen22), including healthy eating and childhood obesity(Reference Kumanyika, Jeffery and Morabia23), as they provide useful frameworks for achieving a better understanding of the multiple factors and barriers that impact on dietary behaviours. Although past studies have used SEM in developing conceptual models to investigate the impact of interventions on healthy eating(Reference Story, Kaphingst and Robinson-O'Brien19, Reference Thomas, Sutcliffe and Harden20, Reference King, Stokols and Talen22, Reference Klepp, Pérez-Rodrigo and De Bourdeaudhuij24, Reference Dzewaltowski, Estabrooks and Johnston25), as far as the authors are aware none have investigated the relative association of the different levels found in these models through multilevel analysis. Where studies have used multilevel analysis to investigate school effects on student dietary choice they have found no significant school-level variance once controlling for student-level characteristics(Reference Maes and Lievens5, Reference West, Sweeting and Leyland15, Reference Krolner, Due and Rasmussen26, Reference van der Horst, Timperio and Crawford27), suggesting that differences in student dietary choice between schools is due to the composition of the individuals who attended the schools and not the school approaches to food. These studies incorporated few school-level factors that targeted student dietary choice in their analyses, either focusing on a very small number of factors such as the availability of unhealthy snacks and drinks(Reference Maes and Lievens5, Reference Krolner, Due and Rasmussen26, Reference van der Horst, Timperio and Crawford27), the availability of healthy food(Reference Krolner, Due and Rasmussen26), the distance from school to the nearest food store(Reference van der Horst, Timperio and Crawford27) and whether students were allowed to leave the school grounds during lunch(Reference Maes and Lievens5), or by considering general school-level characteristics(Reference West, Sweeting and Leyland15) that are not directly related to dietary choice. In doing so it was not possible for the studies to consider the multifactorial approach to the promotion of healthy eating as described in the SEM and promoted through the whole school approach to healthy eating developed from a settings perspective(Reference Townsend, Murphy and Moore28).

Within the present paper we develop an SEM that considers the multiple levels of influence on the promotion of healthy eating in the school as well as the interaction between these levels. We then apply this model to the multilevel analysis of the promotion of healthy eating within secondary schools in Wales to study how factors at different levels of the SEM are associated with dietary behaviour of students, the use of the SEM informing our data collection, analysis and understanding of healthy eating within schools.

Methods

Developing a socio-ecological model

We developed an SEM for health promotion in the school through adapting previous models. Our model was based largely on McLeroy and colleagues’(Reference McLeroy, Bibeau and Steckler29) variation of Brofenbrenner's(Reference Bronfenbrenner30, Reference Bronfenbrenner31) ecological model that consisted of five levels of influence for health-related behaviours and conditions. We also reviewed other frameworks such as Dahlgren and Whiteheads’(Reference Dahlgren and Whitehead32) ‘main determinants of health from a socio-ecological perspective’ and Barton and Grants’ health map(Reference Barton and Grant33) that represented the determinants of health behaviours at the different system levels that the SEM describes, as well as suggesting complex interconnections among these determinants.

Within the SEM developed for the present study we included six levels of influence, encompassing both student- and school-level influences (Table 1), which should be considered when developing health promotion approaches within schools or when investigating the health behaviour of students.

Table 1 A socio-ecological framework for the investigation of the promotion of health behaviour in schools

By applying this SEM to the promotion of healthy eating within schools, we were able to identify the important aspects that influence healthy eating and the promotion of healthy dietary choice. This subsequently informed both the collection of data and the analysis. The SEM was used to target an investigation into the influences on student dietary choice in schools by providing areas to examine in case studies completed in two schools in Wales and through an extensive review of the literature for each level of the SEM. Data collection in the case studies involved focus groups with pupils, semi-structured interviews with staff and observations of the school food environment; some trialling of dietary recall questions was also carried out during case studies to examine if students related to eating foods included in the questions. Data were then collected on factors identified in this preliminary research in the main data collection, with variables describing these factors included in the final analyses.

Main data collection

Data at the student level were collected from school students aged 11–16 years through the Wales sample of the 2005/2006 Health Behaviour in School-aged Children (HBSC) study. The student survey was administered between January and April 2006, as a self-completion in-school questionnaire to all sampled students attending school on the day of the survey; absent students were not followed up. Sampling and data collection protocol for the 2005/2006 HBSC followed that for the 2001/2002 survey(Reference Currie, Samdal and Boyce34).

School-level data on the school approaches to promote healthy eating were collected through postal questionnaires sent to members of teaching staff within the schools in which the HBSC study was carried out, alongside information provided by the Welsh Assembly Government. The majority of school-level variables came from pre-coded close-ended questions that provided variable categories as responses. Data collection from staff in schools aimed to achieve two or more completed questionnaires from each school, allowing modal responses of staff within the school to be used in the derivation of variables. Further details on data collection can be found elsewhere(Reference Townsend, Murphy and Moore28).

Multilevel analysis

Multilevel analysis assists in efforts to investigate school approaches to healthy eating from the perspective of the SEM, by enabling the analysis of the relative contribution of different levels of determinants and also of their cross-level interactions(Reference Ball, Timperio and Crawford2). Indeed socio-ecological and multilevel approaches are recommended for research into healthy eating in schools(Reference Story, Kaphingst and Robinson-O'Brien19, Reference Story, Giles-Corti and Yaroch35). Multilevel analysis allowed us to carry out analysis incorporating student- and school-level variables and enabled us to investigate where and how effects were occurring(Reference Sellström and Bremberg36, Reference Rasbash, Steele and Browne37).

Dependent variables

Two types of self-report dietary behaviour measure were used as dependent variables in analysis. First, summary measures of the frequency of consumption of (i) healthy and (ii) unhealthy food items; and second, measures of whether or not three different foods were eaten at lunch time. These types of dependent variables were chosen to investigate any difference in the association of factors from the various levels of the SEM for food eaten during the school day, i.e. lunch time, and that eaten throughout the whole day, including food eaten outside school.

For the frequency measures, students were asked: ‘How many times a week do you usually eat….?’ for two different healthy food items (fruit/vegetables) and four different unhealthy food items (cola and other soft drinks/sweets and chocolate/crisps/chips). For each food item, there were seven possible responses (‘never’, ‘less than once a week’, ‘once a week’, ‘2–4 days a week’, ‘5–6 days a week’, ‘once a day every day’, ‘every day more than once’). Each of the two summary measures was obtained by counting, for each student, the number of healthy (0–2) or unhealthy (0–4) items reported as consumed at least daily.

For the foods eaten at lunch time, dichotomous variables were derived to indicate those students who agreed (1) or disagreed (0) that they ate the food for lunch. This was based on responses to questions which asked students to indicate their agreement with the statement: ‘I eat (food) for lunch’, which was asked for three food items (fruit/chips/sweets and chocolate) on a 7-point scale. Those indicating that they ‘agree very strongly’, ‘agree strongly’ or ‘agree slightly’ were coded as agreeing that they ate that food item for lunch. These questions were taken from a study on adolescent food choice(Reference Currie, Samdal and Boyce34).

Analytical approach

Two-level random effects models were run in the software MlWin 2·1 (Centre for Multilevel Modelling, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK) for each of the dependent variables: level one corresponded to students and included student-level variables, level two to schools and included school-level variables. These models were logistic for the three binary lunch variables and ordinal logistic for the healthy and unhealthy frequency variables. The independent variables derived from the data collection were assigned to the level of influence at which they can be found from the SEM (Table 2). In Table 2 variable values are provided in brackets. Many variables were dichotomous; those that had more complex values or construction are described in footnotes below the table. Variables in normal font were derived from the student questionnaires, variables in normal bold were derived from the teacher questionnaire, variables in italic bold were derived from information supplied by the Welsh Assembly Government, underlined variables were not included in final analyses as data were not collected on them. More information on variables and can be found elsewhere(Reference Townsend, Murphy and Moore28).

Table 2 Summary of factors included in the analysis, showing variables of influence on student dietary choice in relation to level within the socio-ecological model applied to our data collection

TV, television; SES, socio-economic status; WNHSS, Welsh Network of Healthy School Schemes; HBSC, Health Behaviour in School-aged Children.

†Variables used a 7-point Likert scale from ‘agree very strongly’ to ‘disagree very strongly’. For interpersonal variables students were asked ‘Do you agree that your family/friend think you should eat…?’ or ‘…your friends eat…?’ for each food measured in the dependent variables.

‡Engagement with school variable is a four-level composite variable derived from three questions in the HBSC questionnaire(Reference Dennison and Shepherd40). The higher the score, the greater the engagement with school.

§Family Affluence Scale is a four-item composite score to judge individual SES(Reference Hox38, 39).

∣∣Within the WNHSS schools complete phases in which they implement a number of health promoting actions. Once schools are adjudged to have completed one phase, through inspection by outside parties, they move on to the next(Reference Rasmussen, Krølner and Klepp41).

The percentage of school-level variance attributed to each level of the SEM for the dependent variables was calculated by presenting the difference between the residual error variances for the full model and those for the model without all variables from that level of influence from the SEM, expressed as a percentage of residual error variance of the full model(Reference Hox38).

Results

In the final sample seventy schools completed the HBSC survey, with 7300 students completing the questionnaire. Of these seventy schools, sixty-four completed two or more teacher questionnaires (total n 289) and thus could be included in the analysis. This resulted in 90 % of the state schools from the HBSC sample being retained for the analysis, equating to 26 % of the state schools in Wales, with a final sample for analysis involving the school-level variables of 6693 students in sixty-four schools in Wales.

There were no significant differences between schools that did and did not return two or more questionnaires in terms of location, language medium of school, number of students in the school and the proportion of students eligible for free school meals. Wald tests indicated a significant variation at the school level once controlling for all student-level variables, indicating the data were applicable for multilevel analysis (Table 3).

Table 3 Student and school sample sizes for final models and significant school-level variation once controlling for student-level factors for each dependent variable

Data were collected from secondary schools in Wales that were a part of the 2005/2006 Health Behaviour in School-aged Children (HBSC) study; the final sample for analysis included data from 6693 students aged 11–16 years and 289 teachers from sixty-four secondary schools in Wales.

As the SEM describes, interactions can occur within the same level and across the levels. Thus many of the variables could be assigned to, and interact with, a number of levels. For example, although the ‘number of four main subjects useful in learning about healthy eating’ is representative of an individual's attitude towards the classroom education on healthy eating, this can influenced by the school approaches to health education, found at the school organisation level. These in turn will be influenced by the National Curriculum, found at the macro level. Indeed the macro level will influence many of the variables included in the analysis, such as the subjects within which schools teach healthy eating, the food served within the school and the students eligible for free school meals.

The only macro-level influence included in our SEM was school participation in the Welsh Network of Healthy School Schemes (WNHSS) which is a national initiative that demands schools undertake certain health promoting actions to be considered ‘healthy’ schools. Although macro-level factors will influence many of the factors at a number of levels of the SEM, differences between schools in these factors will only occur due to the extent the school adopts national policies or differences between local authorities in which schools are based. A means of measuring the association of public policies, therefore, is to measure how much schools adhere to these macro-level factors, as was done in the present study through the number of phases schools had completed in the WNHSS.

A number of factors identified in the SEM developed for the present study were not included in the final analysis (Table 2). Ethnicity was not included as the student sample (n 7300) was mainly white British (94 %) and the low numbers for other ethnic groups were not large enough to make a statistical impact. Food preparation knowledge and skill along with family eating behaviour were not measured as a part of the HBSC within Wales, while schools did not report on links to parents in generating consistent messages around healthy eating or local authority approaches to school food. Local authorities were not included in the data collection.

Amount of variance explained by levels of influence

Multilevel analysis of the data allowed us to estimate the percentage of school-level variance in the dietary choices of students explained by the different levels of influence of the SEM (Table 4).

Table 4 Percentage of school-level variance in dependent variables explained by levels of influence of the socio-ecological framework adopted by the present study

Data were collected from secondary schools in Wales that were a part of the 2005/2006 Health Behaviour in School-aged Children (HBSC) study; the final sample for analysis included data from 6693 students aged 11–16 years and 289 teachers from sixty-four secondary schools in Wales.

The variance explained by the total models was greater than the sum of the variance explained by each level for each dependent variable. This difference was greatest for the healthy sum variable for which the sum of the variance explained by each level was 22·9 % of the variance explained by the total model; the difference was smallest for the sweets for lunch dependent variable for which the sum of the variance explained by each level was 56·7 % of the variance explained by the total model.

Factors from the macro level explained the least amount of school-level variance for each dependent variable except for the unhealthy frequency variable, for which factors at the demographic level explained the least.

Factors at the student intrapersonal level explained more variance for the food frequency dependent variables, whereas student interpersonal factors explained more for the lunch dependent variables. School as an organisation explained more of the variance of the unhealthy food dependent variables (chips for lunch, sweets for lunch and the unhealthy frequency variable) than the factors at the school community level, which explained more of the variance for the healthy sum variable. The two school levels explained similar amounts of variance for the fruit for lunch variable.

Discussion

At the student level, student intrapersonal characteristics explained more of the variance for food eaten throughout the day than that eaten for lunch, while a greater proportion of the variance in lunch choices was explained by student interpersonal factors. At the school level organisational factors explained a greater proportion of variance in the dietary choice of unhealthy foods than community factors, which explained more of the variance in the dietary choice of food eaten throughout the day than those eaten for lunch only.

The main strength of the present study is that it had access to data from a large sample of secondary-school students from many schools within Wales. Using these data in multilevel analysis alongside those collected from teachers within the schools, analysis could be performed which incorporated a number of individual-level and school-level factors. By calculating the variance explained by variables from each level of the SEM for each dependent variable, the association between different levels of influence from the SEM and varied dietary choice could be studied.

The main limitation of the present study is that most of the variables come from self-report questionnaires. Both sets of dependent variables were derived from self-reported questions in the HBSC student survey. The sum variables came from the selected ‘method of choice’(39) for the HBSC survey that has been validated(39) while the lunch variables came from questions asking about the choice of specific foods for lunch(Reference Dennison and Shepherd40). Although these lunch variables came from a paper published in 1995, leading to concerns about applicability to the reporting of current adolescent lunch behaviour, these were trialled in case studies prior to the data collection. The student-level independent variables all came from the HBSC survey and were self-report. Many of the school-level variables are crude, unvalidated measures, which are dependent upon teacher responses and provide no measure of quality for these actions. For both student-level and school-level variables, unless there is substantial variation across schools in reporting bias, this should not affect the estimated association in the models. The data for the present study are cross-sectional; as they are not longitudinal no causality can be inferred between levels of the SEM and the dependent variables, only a significant association can be derived.

Although other studies have developed conceptual models based on SEM for studying healthy eating in schools, none were found that investigated the relative association of these levels on the behaviour of students. Studies that have investigated the school effect on health behaviours, including healthy eating, using multilevel analysis found no significant school effect on student dietary choice once controlling for student-level characteristics(Reference Maes and Lievens5, Reference West, Sweeting and Leyland15, Reference Krolner, Due and Rasmussen26, Reference van der Horst, Timperio and Crawford27). The significant school-level variation on controlling for student-level factors and the amount of variance explained by school-level characteristics in the present study contradict these earlier findings. One explanation for this difference may be due to the greater school and student sample size within the present study. The largest sample size in previous multilevel studies identified by the authors was fifty-nine schools(Reference Krolner, Due and Rasmussen26) and 3225 students(Reference Maes and Lievens5) while the smallest was fifteen schools(Reference van der Horst, Timperio and Crawford27) and 1293 students(Reference van der Horst, Timperio and Crawford27), compared with 6693 students in sixty-three schools included in the present study. We also built on previous multilevel studies in student dietary choice in schools(Reference Maes and Lievens5, Reference West, Sweeting and Leyland15, Reference Krolner, Due and Rasmussen26, Reference van der Horst, Timperio and Crawford27) by including more variables at both the student and school levels and by separating determinants operating at the different levels of the SEM to estimate the association they had with the reported dietary choice of students.

The small amount of between-school variance in the dependent variables attributed to factors at the student demographic level of influence highlight some differences between individuals of different ages and sexes which explained small amounts of school-level variance due to slight differences in ratios of students from different year groups and between sexes within the schools. A small percentage of schools were single sex.

The greater percentage of variance explained by the student interpersonal factors for the lunch variables than the food frequency variables highlights the association of norms of behaviour with lunch choices. As these are lunch choices made during the school day it would be expected that these reflect norms of peers with whom students eat lunch(Reference Rasmussen, Krølner and Klepp41Reference Neumark-Sztainer, Wall and Perry43); other interpersonal factors may be important, however, such as family norms(Reference Rasmussen, Krølner and Klepp41, Reference Perez-Pastor, Metcalf and Hosking44, Reference Pearson, Biddle and Gorely45) and family setup(Reference Rasmussen, Krølner and Klepp41) and this finding would benefit from further investigation. The greater percentage of variance explained by the student intrapersonal factors for the sum variables than the lunch variables indicates the importance of the behaviour(Reference Rasmussen, Krølner and Klepp41, Reference Giovannini, Agostoni and Shamir46) and attitude(Reference Martens, van Assema and Brug47Reference Kann, Telljohann and Wooley50) of individuals in dietary choices made throughout the whole day.

The greater percentage of variance explained by the school organisational factors, than those at the school community level, for all the unhealthy dependent variables may indicate the importance of school rules and policies(Reference Neumark-Sztainer, Wall and Perry43, Reference Nelson, Bradbury and Poulter51Reference Neumark-Sztainer, French and Hannan53) on controlling unhealthy eating. It may be that these school rules and approaches are more effective at controlling unhealthy eating than they are at encouraging healthy eating. It must be recognised, however, that there was a greater number of variables in models from this level of influence (eleven variables) than for school community level (four variables); whereas the school community level explained a greater percentage of variance for the food frequency variables, highlighting the importance of the community environment(Reference Gould, Russell and Barker54, Reference Resnicow, Davis-Hearn and Smith55) in student dietary choices throughout the day.

Factors from the macro level were not found to explain a large amount of the variance in dietary choice, although this may be due to the fact that only one variable was included in models from this level of influence (number of phases school had completed as part of the WNHSS). Despite the relatively low variance explained by this level in comparison to other levels, the 1·16 % of the variance in unhealthy food choices made throughout the day explained by the number of phases schools had completed in the WNHSS indicates the association of this programme with student dietary choice. Larger studies comparing between local authorities, or even countries, may allow more factors at the macro level to be investigated.

The total variance explained by the final models was greater than the sum of the variances explained by each level of influence, highlighting the synergistic nature of these levels in influencing dietary choice, as proposed in the SEM. This supports the use of the SEM in investigating and developing school approaches to healthy eating. To develop this, further objective measurements of school and student actions would provide more confidence in the findings while a longitudinal approach would allow an inference of causality.

This model helps in both the targeting of interventions to promote behaviour change and in focusing research into approaches. This practice can be moved forward by using the SEM to identify which factors should be targeted in making schools more health promoting. The large amounts of variance explained by the student interpersonal level on reported lunch choices indicates the importance of social influences on student choices at lunch and the need for tackling social norms along with individual behaviour within schools. School approaches to food must involve both organisational and community aspects of the school, implementing policies and rules towards food and influencing the social networks within the school and between schools and the community.

Acknowledgements

The data collection was completed as part of a PhD funded by the Medical Research Council, UK; N.T.'s position at the University of Oxford is funded by the National Obesity Observatory. The PhD the data came from was undertaken at Cardiff University and was supervised by Professor Laurence Moore and Dr Simon Murphy. There are no conflicts of interest to report. N.T. was responsible for data collection, statistical analysis and writing the manuscript; he is the guarantor. C.F. contributed to the development of the study and provided critical review of the manuscript.

References

1.Pyle, SA, Sharkey, J, Yetter, Get al. (2006) Fighting an epidemic: the role of schools in reducing childhood obesity. Psychol Sch 43, 361376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
2.Ball, K, Timperio, A & Crawford, D (2006) Understanding environmental influences on nutrition and physical activity behaviors: where should we look and what should we count? Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 3, 33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3.Story, M, Kaphingst, KM & French, S (2006) The role of schools in obesity prevention. Future Child 16, 109142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4.Lister-Sharp, D, Chapman, S, Stewart-Brown, Set al. (1999) Health promoting schools and health promotion in schools: two systematic reviews. Health Technol Assess 3, issue 22, 1207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5.Maes, L & Lievens, J (2003) Can the school make a difference? A multilevel analysis of adolescent risk and health behaviour. Soc Sci Med 56, 517529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6.Bennett, P & Hodgson, R (1992) Psychology and health promotion. In Health Promotion: Disciplines and Diversity, pp. 2238 [R Bunton and G Macdonald, editors]. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
7.Macdonald, G & Bunton, R (editors) (1992) Health promotion: discipline or disciplines? In Health Promotion: Disciplines and Diversity, pp. 621. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
8.Lynagh, M, Schofield, MJ & Sanson-Fisher, RW (1997) School health promotion programs over the past decade: a review of the smoking, alcohol and solar protection literature. Health Promot Int 12, 4360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9.Green, LW & Kreuter, MW (1999) Health Promotion Planning: An Educational and Ecological Approach, 3rd ed. Mountain View, CA: Mayfield Publishing Co.Google Scholar
10.World Health Organization (1997) Promoting Health Through Schools. Report of a WHO Expert Committee on Comprehensive School Health Education and Promotion. WHO Technical Report Series no. 870. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
11.World Health Organization (2002) The Egmond Agenda: Establishing Health Promotion in Schools: Components for Success. Copenhagen: WHO Regional Office for Europe.Google Scholar
12.Dooris, M (2009) Holistic and sustainable health improvement: the contribution of the settings-based approach to health promotion. Perspect Public Health 129, 2936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13.World Health Organization (1997) The Health Promoting School: An Investment in Education, Health and Democracy: Conference Report on the First Conference of the European Network of Health Promoting Schools, Thessaloniki, Greece. Copenhagen: WHO Regional Office for Europe.Google Scholar
14.Mũkoma, W & Flisher, AJ (2004) Evaluations of health promoting schools: a review of nine studies. Health Promot Int 19, 357368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
15.West, P, Sweeting, H & Leyland, A (2004) School effects on pupils’ health behaviours: evidence in support of the health promoting school. Res Papers Educ 19, 261291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
16.World Health Organization (1986) Ottawa Charter for Health Promotion. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
17.Stokols, D (1996) Translating social ecological theory into guidelines for community health promotion. Am J Health Promot 10, 282293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18.Story, M, Neumark-Sztainer, D & French, S (2002) Individual and environmental influences on adolescent eating behaviors. J Am Diet Assoc 102, Suppl. 1, S40S51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19.Story, M, Kaphingst, KM, Robinson-O'Brien, Ret al. (2008) Creating healthy food and eating environments: policy and environmental approaches. Annu Rev Public Health 29, 253272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20.Thomas, J, Sutcliffe, K, Harden, Aet al. (2003) Children and Healthy Eating: A Systematic Review of Barriers and Facilitators. London: EPPI-Centre, Social Science Research Unit, Institute of Education, University of London.Google Scholar
21.Wind, M, Bjelland, M, Pérez-Rodrigo, Cet al. (2008) Appreciation and implementation of a school-based intervention are associated with changes in fruit and vegetable intake in 10- to 13-year old schoolchildren – the Pro Children study. Health Educ Res 23, 9971007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22.King, AC, Stokols, D, Talen, Eet al. (2002) Theoretical approaches to the promotion of physical activity: forging a transdisciplinary paradigm. Am J Prev Med 23, Suppl. 1, 1525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
23.Kumanyika, S, Jeffery, RW, Morabia, Aet al. (2002) Public Health Approaches to the Prevention of Obesity (PHAPO) Working Group of the International Obesity Task Force (IOTF). Obesity prevention: the case for action. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 93, 11681173.Google Scholar
24.Klepp, KI, Pérez-Rodrigo, C, De Bourdeaudhuij, Iet al. (2005) Promoting fruit and vegetable consumption among European schoolchildren: rationale, conceptualization and design of the Pro Children project. Ann Nutr Metab 49, 212220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25.Dzewaltowski, DA, Estabrooks, PA & Johnston, JA (2002) Healthy Youth Places promoting nutrition and physical activity. Health Educ Res 17, 541551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26.Krolner, R, Due, P, Rasmussen, Met al. (2009) Does school environment affect 11-year-olds’ fruit and vegetable intake in Denmark? Soc Sci Med 68, 14161424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27.van der Horst, K, Timperio, A, Crawford, Det al. (2008) The school food environment: associations with adolescent soft drink and snack consumption. Am J Prev Med 35, 217223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28.Townsend, N, Murphy, S & Moore, L (2011) The more schools do to promote healthy eating, the healthier the dietary choices by students. J Epidemiol Community Health 65, 889895.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29.McLeroy, KR, Bibeau, D, Steckler, Aet al. (1988) An ecological perspective on health promotion programs. Health Educ Behav 15, 351377.Google ScholarPubMed
30.Bronfenbrenner, U (1979) The Ecology of Human Development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
31.Bronfenbrenner, U (1977) Toward an experimental ecology of human development. Am Psychol 32, 513531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
32.Dahlgren, G & Whitehead, M (1991) Policies and Strategies to Promote Social Equity in Health. Stockholm: Institute for Future Studies.Google Scholar
33.Barton, H & Grant, M (2006) A health map for the local human habitat. J R Soc Promot Health 126, 252253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34.Currie, C, Samdal, O, Boyce, et al. (2002) Research Protocol for the 2001/2002 Survey. Edinburgh: HBSC.Google Scholar
35.Story, M, Giles-Corti, B, Yaroch, ALet al. (2009) Work Group IV: future directions for measures of the food and physical activity environments. Am J Prev Med 36, Suppl. 1, S182S188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36.Sellström, E & Bremberg, S (2006) Is there a ‘school effect’ on pupil outcomes? A review of multilevel studies. J Epidemiol Community Health 60, 149155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
37.Rasbash, J, Steele, F, Browne, Wet al. (2004) A User's Guide to MLwiN. Bristol: Centre for Multilevel Modelling, University of Bristol.Google Scholar
38.Hox, J (2002) Multilevel Analysis Techniques and Applications. London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
39.HBSC (2002) Overview: Health Behaviour in School-aged Children. http://www.hbsc.org/overview.html (accessed February 2008).Google Scholar
40.Dennison, CM & Shepherd, R (1995) Adolescent food choice: an application of the Theory of Planned Behaviour. J Hum Nutr Diet 8, 923.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
41.Rasmussen, M, Krølner, R, Klepp, K-Iet al. (2006) Determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among children and adolescents: a review of the literature. Part I: quantitative studies. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 3, 22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42.Kalavana, TV, Maes, S & De Gucht, V (2010) Interpersonal and self-regulation determinants of healthy and unhealthy eating behavior in adolescents. J Health Psychol 15, 4452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
43.Neumark-Sztainer, D, Wall, M, Perry, Cet al. (2003) Correlates of fruit and vegetable intake among adolescents. Findings from Project EAT. Prev Med 37, 198208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
44.Perez-Pastor, EM, Metcalf, BS, Hosking, Jet al. (2009) Assortative weight gain in mother–daughter and father–son pairs: an emerging source of childhood obesity. Longitudinal study of trios (EarlyBird 43). Int J Obes (Lond) 33, 727735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45.Pearson, N, Biddle, SJH & Gorely, T (2009) Family correlates of fruit and vegetable consumption in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Public Health Nutr 12, 267283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
46.Giovannini, M, Agostoni, C & Shamir, R (2010) Symposium overview: do we all eat breakfast and is it important? Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr 50, 9799.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
47.Martens, MK, van Assema, P & Brug, J (2005) Why do adolescents eat what they eat? Personal and social environmental predictors of fruit, snack and breakfast consumption among 12–14-year-old Dutch students. Public Health Nutr 8, 12581265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
48.Nutbeam, D, Smith, C, Moore, Let al. (1993) Warning! schools can damage your health: alienation from school and its impact on health behaviour. J Paediatr Child Health 29, Suppl. 1, S25S30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
49.Brug, J (2008) Determinants of healthy eating: motivation, abilities and environmental opportunities. Fam Pract 25, Suppl. 1, i50i55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
50.Kann, L, Telljohann, SK & Wooley, SF (2007) Health education: results from the School Health Policies and Programs Study 2006. J Sch Health 77, 408434.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
51.Nelson, M, Bradbury, J, Poulter, Jet al. (2004) School Meals in Secondary Schools in England. London: King's College London, National Centre for Social Research, Nutrition works!.Google Scholar
52.Kubik, MY, Lytle, LA, Hannan, PJet al. (2003) The association of the school food environment with dietary behaviors of young adolescents. Am J Public Health 93, 11681173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
53.Neumark-Sztainer, D, French, SA, Hannan, PJet al. (2005) School lunch and snacking patterns among high school students: associations with school food environment and policies. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 2, 14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
54.Gould, R, Russell, J & Barker, ME (2006) School lunch menus and 11 to 12 year old children's food choice in three secondary schools in England-are the nutritional standards being met? Appetite 46, 8692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
55.Resnicow, K, Davis-Hearn, M, Smith, Met al. (1997) Social-cognitive predictors of fruit and vegetable intake in children. Health Psychol 16, 272276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 A socio-ecological framework for the investigation of the promotion of health behaviour in schools

Figure 1

Table 2 Summary of factors included in the analysis, showing variables of influence on student dietary choice in relation to level within the socio-ecological model applied to our data collection

Figure 2

Table 3 Student and school sample sizes for final models and significant school-level variation once controlling for student-level factors for each dependent variable

Figure 3

Table 4 Percentage of school-level variance in dependent variables explained by levels of influence of the socio-ecological framework adopted by the present study