Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-l4dxg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-21T10:47:29.364Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effect of egg consumption on early childhood development: evidence from Un Oeuf study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 December 2024

Helen Ernyey
Affiliation:
College of Medicine, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
Chhavi Tiwari
Affiliation:
Department of Environmental and Global Health, College of Public Health and Health Professions, University of Florida, Gainesville 32611-7011, FL, USA
Heather Stark
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology, College of Public Health and Health Professions, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
Emma Hunniford
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology, College of Public Health and Health Professions, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
Aissata Wereme N’Diaye
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Recherche en Production et Santé Animales (LaRePSA), Institut de l’Environnement et de Recherches Agricoles (INERA), Centre National de Recherches Scientifique et Technologique–(CNRST), Ouagadougou, Burkina Faso
Yacouba Zare
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Recherche en Production et Santé Animales (LaRePSA), Institut de l’Environnement et de Recherches Agricoles (INERA), Centre National de Recherches Scientifique et Technologique–(CNRST), Ouagadougou, Burkina Faso
Anteneh Omer
Affiliation:
Independent Nutrition Consultant, Hawassa, Ethiopia
Sarah Lindley McKune*
Affiliation:
Department of Environmental and Global Health, College of Public Health and Health Professions, University of Florida, Gainesville 32611-7011, FL, USA The Center for African Studies, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
*
Corresponding author: Sarah Lindley McKune; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

Recent studies have shown that inclusion of eggs in young children’s diet can help meet nutritional requirements associated with cognitive development. This study aims to investigate the effect of egg consumption on early childhood development using Ages and Stages Questionnaire-3 in Burkina Faso.

Design:

The study presented here uses data collected during a follow-up of the Un Oeuf-a three-arm clustered randomised controlled trial (RCT), conducted roughly 4 months after the end of the RCT.

Setting:

This research was conducted in eighteen rural villages within the Kaya Department of the Sanmatenga Province in Burkina Faso.

Participants:

Participants of this study include a total of 244 children aged between 18 and 33 months, with seventy-eight children in the full intervention group, eighty-three in the partial group, and eighty-three in the control group.

Results:

Results show that children with consistent egg consumption (in all months) had a lower odd of falling below the cut-off scores in gross motor (${\rm{OR}} = {\rm 0\!\cdot\!13},{\it P} = \rm{0\!\cdot\!02}$) and personal social skills (${\rm{OR}} = 0\!\cdot\!34,{\it P} =0 \!\cdot\!05$). And a dose–response was established; for each additional egg/week, a 1·9 % increase in scores for problem-solving skills was observed.

Conclusions:

Findings from this study contribute to a growing body of evidence that increasing egg consumption among children in low- and middle-income countries (LMIC) can improve growth and development. The study highlights the need for additional research in LMIC to better understand the multifactorial relationship between diet and childhood development.

Type
Research Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Early childhood development (ECD) was included in the UN’ Sustainable Development Goals in 2015, signifying a global commitment to ensure equitable access to early learning opportunities and childhood development for all children(1). Early childhood offers a critical window of opportunity that can shape the lifetime trajectory of a child’s holistic development, including academic achievement, occupational success and social adjustment(2,Reference Singh, Yeh and Boone Blanchard3) . Delay in ECD is more common in low- and middle-income countries (LMIC), with 250 million children under five at risk of not achieving their developmental potential(Reference Black, Walker and Fernald4), and around 42 % of children experiencing childhood delays living in West Africa(Reference Gil, Ewerling and Ferreira5).

ECD is impacted by myriad factors, including diet, environmental, and social risk exposures(Reference Ibáñez-Alfonso, Company-Córdoba and García De La Cadena6,Reference Larson, Martorell and Bauer7) . One of the important underlying reasons for the high prevalence of ECD delays is underlying poor nutrition and food insecurity in LMIC, which emerges from lack of access to resources alongside inadequate and inequitable food production(Reference Company-Córdoba, Sianes and Simpson8). Undernutrition during childhood leads to adverse health effects, including stunting, decreased cognitive functioning, and increased morbidity and mortality(Reference Larson, Martorell and Bauer7,Reference Nurliyana, Mohd Shariff and Mohd Taib9,Reference Larson and Yousafzai10) . In Burkina Faso, over 672 000 children suffer from chronic malnutrition,(11) with 22·6 % children under 5 suffering from stunting(12). The high prevalence of stunting in Burkina Faso raises significant concerns for policymakers and necessitates additional research on nutrition and ECD in this context.

While meeting the nutritional requirement of early age is a challenge in most LMIC due to lack of resources, access to nutritious food(Reference Asare, Rosi and Faber13) and suboptimal feeding practices(Reference Arabi, Frongillo and Avula14), it has been shown that including animal source foods such as eggs, meat, fish, or dairy in otherwise typical diets has the potential to improve a child’s nutritional and health outcomes(Reference Eaton, Rothpletz-Puglia and Dreker15). However, the consumption of animal source foods in LMIC has remained low, likely due to poor availability, high relative cost(Reference Headey16), and restrictive food norms and taboos(Reference Adesogan, Havelaar and McKune17). Nutritional interventions and behaviour change campaigns have been shown to reduce or remove the social and cultural barriers to consuming certain food items that may significantly impact early childhood growth and development(Reference Gallegos-Riofrío, Waters and Salvador18Reference Moore, Singh and Serra20).

Like other animal source foods, eggs are rich in macro and micronutrients(Reference Iannotti, Lutter and Bunn21,Reference Stewart22) . Consuming one egg per day for a child aged 6–24 months can provide adequate nutrients that are essential for child nutrition and brain development(Reference Lutter, Iannotti and Stewart23).

Based on evidence from Ecuador(Reference Iannotti, Lutter and Stewart24) that found significant improvements in child growth when infants were fed eggs, the Un Oeuf study – a clustered randomised controlled trial – was conducted in eighteen rural villages of Burkina Faso in 2018–2019, with the primary objective of increasing egg consumption among infants and young children through a behaviour change communication intervention(Reference McKune, Stark and Sapp19). In Kaya Department, the Un Oeuf study randomly selected eighteen non-urban villages and then randomly assigned (1:1:1) the villages to one of the three intervention arms. In the full intervention arm, children were gifted three chickens from a community champion and a fourth chicken from their fathers and caregivers received a behaviour change communication (BCC) package, including monthly trainings on integrated nutrition and agriculture. At a gifting ceremony, where the child received the chickens, village leaders presented the chickens as gifts to the child’s flock, reinforcing an understanding that the chickens and any eggs produced belonged to the child. One egg per day from these chickens was advised to be fed to the child. While the project did not intervene on the sale or sharing of eggs, mothers were trained on the significance of diet during the first 1000 d and asked to see the egg as a treatment for or investment in the targeted child. They were also advised that only after the child’s one egg/day ‘dose’ had been achieved, might the mother consider feeding other children or selling the eggs. The partial intervention group received only the BCC package and did not receive any chickens. The control group did not receive any intervention; neither BCC nor chickens. The intervention, which included monthly BCC trainings for the full and partial intervention groups, continued for 9 months with baseline data collection in the month of July 2018 and the end line data collection in April 2019. The trial was registered at clinicaltrials.gov: NCT04135625. For a detailed protocol of the study design, methods and baseline characteristics, see Stark et al., 2021. The Un Oeuf study population ate little to no eggs at baseline; however, the intervention significantly increased egg consumption in the full and partial groups; the full intervention, in which children consumed around six eggs per week by end line, also significantly decreased wasting and underweight(Reference McKune, Stark and Sapp19). As the Un Oeuf study significantly improved egg consumption and nutritional status of children, additional funding was secured to investigate the potential impact of the study on ECD. The study presented here uses data collected during a follow-up of the Un Oeuf study, conducted roughly 4 months after the end of the randomised controlled trial and engaging the same children who participated in the initial study. This study aims to investigate the effect of egg consumption on ECD using the Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ-3; Squires & Bricker, 2009), modified by the study team for the local environment.

Methods

Study design

The follow-up study was conducted 4 months after the completion of the Un Oeuf study to assess the impact of egg consumption on ECD in Burkina Faso(Reference Stark, Omer and Wereme N’Diaye25). During the 9-month intervention period of the Un Oeuf study, monthly surveys were used to collect data on egg consumption and anthropometric measurements of enrolled children. At the beginning of the study, children of age 6–12 months were enrolled. Upon conclusion, these children were assessed for development outcomes in the follow-up study, using a culturally modified ASQ-3 tool and anthropometric measurements of height, length, weight and head circumference.

Variables

Child development measures

ASQ-3 is a widely used standardised screening instrument to measure development progress in children between the ages of 1 month to 66 months (Reference Squires and Bricker26Reference Miller, Neupane and Joshi30). The research team selected and adapted the ASQ tool to assess ECD in Burkina Faso in collaboration with scientists at the Anita Zucker Center for Excellence in Early Child Development Studies. Guided by these experts, changes in the ASQ-3 tool were made to allow for appropriate assessment of an indicator, while utilising materials that were more familiar to children in the Burkinabe context. These included substituting a stick that the children could use to write/draw in the dirt, since they did not have access to pencils and papers; using small pebbles instead of cheerios and stepping onto logs instead of climbing steps. Researchers then trained the enumerators from the Un Oeuf study to use the modified ASQ-3 tool, after working with them to review and revise the proposed tool for cultural salience. The questionnaire was then pilot-tested with children of appropriate ages using set criteria established to assess the various questions to improve internal reliability and validity prior to administering the ASQ-3 assessment. The ASQ-3 assessment was verbally administered to the child’s mother. In accord with ASQ-3 instruction, the enumerator asked the mother to respond to the questions and, in some cases, asked the child to perform an activity. These data were recorded by the enumerator. ASQ items were observed by the enumerator or reported by a parent and consist of thirty items scored as yes, sometimes, or not yet across five domains: communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem-solving and personal social skills. Within each domain, individuals can score from 0 to 60, with specific scores above or below the cut-off for expected childhood development. We examined cut-off scores in each domain of development. These domain-specific cut-off scores align with ASQ-3 guidelines, which use ASQ score and age of the child to determine the cut-offs. A binary indicator was used to describe whether a child’s score was below the cutoff value (1) or not (0). Following methodological approach of previous ECD research(Reference Alvik and Grøholt27), we looked at domain-specific scores, as well as the overall total score (sum of the five domain-specific scores).

Egg consumption measures

After the baseline survey, the first 2 months’ data quality was compromised due to heavy rains and inaccessibility to the study site. This study, therefore, used longitudinal information for egg consumption for 7 months (months 3–9) with baseline variables measured at the beginning (month 0) and child development scores measured in the follow-up (at month 12). In each month of the intervention period (month 3 through month 9), respondents were asked if they fed eggs to the child in the previous month. We created a categorical variable for consistency in egg consumption as: 1 = Never, 2 = 1–3 months; 3 = 4–6 months and 4 = in all 7 months. Further, if/when respondents indicated that eggs were fed to the child, they were also asked how many eggs were fed in the previous week. Based on their responses, an average weekly egg consumption is calculated using 7 months of data. The measure calculates on average, how many eggs the child ate per week in the last 7 months. Therefore, we check for the effect of both consistency over time and quantity of egg consumption on child development outcomes.

As child development scores were only assessed after the trial ended, the analysis adjusted available confounding factors at baseline that are identified as determinants of child development in existing literature(Reference Mbabazi, Pesu and Mutumba31). These confounders include child’s gender, birth order, baseline weight and height, mother’s age at first birth, mother’s education, household size and economic status (wealth index). The wealth index was calculated based on possession of assets and quality of housing using principal component analysis following Demographic and Health Survey guidelines and treated as tercile for modelling purposes. The items in the asset score included flooring material, cooking fuel, electricity, radio, television, cellphone, table, chair, mattress, solar panel, lamp, cycle, bike and cart. Information for asset ownership was collected only in the seventh month of data collection. The study does not include child’s own age as a confounder since ages and stages accounts for the age itself in score as well as the cut-off values.

Statistical analysis

All analysis was performed in STATA, v.17. Two separate regression analyses have been performed contingent on the nature of the dependent variable. First, logistic regressions were employed to calculate the odds of children falling below the domain-specific cut-off scores while accounting for potential confounders to egg consumption and child growth. Further, linear regression models were used for domain-specific scores and for the total scores. Logarithmic transformations of these scores were used to ensure normality in the distribution and to look at the percent change in scores due to egg consumption. In all models, se were clustered at village level to account for village-level randomisation and heterogeneity across villages. We also tested these models by including intervention arms, but since the intervention of the Un Oeuf study was specifically designed to increase egg consumption in children through behaviour change, gifting chickens and integrated nutrition and agriculture training, the intervention arms (full and partial) were found to be highly correlated with the egg consumption (correlation coefficients, r = 0·92 and r = 0·70, respectively), thereby, could not be included in the regression model. In addition, propensity score matching is used to ensure robustness of the results and deal with selection bias.

Results

Descriptive statistics

The final analysis included a total of 244 children aged between 18 and 33 months, with seventy-eight children in the full intervention group, eighty-three in the partial group, and eighty-three in the control group. Descriptive statistics for baseline characteristics, egg consumption and child development outcomes across the three research arms are presented in Table 1.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics by intervention groups

* Captured only in month 7.

In egg consumption variables, it was observed that children in the full intervention group consumed eggs more regularly than the partial intervention group and control group. In comparison, very few children consumed eggs in the control group. Average weekly egg consumption was also significantly higher in the full intervention group (6·4) as compared to the partial (1·76) and control group (0·22). Figure 1 shows a pattern of egg consumption across research arms in follow-up months.

Figure 1. Pattern of egg consumption in follow-up months across research arms.

Table 2 shows a comparison of average total ASQ score across research arms, and the score was highest in the full intervention group (239·36), as compared with the partial intervention group (223·13) and the control group (223·37). Considering domain-specific outcomes, children in the full intervention group scored higher in almost all developmental domains (except personal social) than the children in the partial or control groups. Independent t test showed that children in the full intervention arm had significantly higher mean scores in gross motor, fine motor and problem-solving domains and in overall total scores compared with those in the control. However, in partial intervention arm, no significant difference in the development score was found when compared with the control arm.

Table 2. Early child development (ECD) scores by intervention arm

ASQ, Ages and Stages Questionnaire.

Impact of egg consumption on early childhood development

Table 3 reports adjusted odds ratios from logistic regressions (Col. 1–5) and coefficients (β) of linear regression models (Col. 6–11). After adjusting for baseline covariates (child’s gender, birth order, child’s anthropometry, mother’s education, mother’s age, household size, wealth index and baseline egg consumption), consistency in egg consumption was found to be statistically significant with lower odds of falling below the cut-off in communication skills ( ${\rm{OR}} =0\!\cdot\!079,{\it P} = 0\!\cdot\!06$ ), gross motor skills ( ${\rm{OR}} = 0\!\cdot\!125, {\it P} \lt0\!\cdot\!05$ ) and for personal social skills ( ${\rm{OR}} = 0\!\cdot\!34,{\it P}=0\!\cdot\!05$ ). Among the covariates, child’s height at baseline is found to be associated with decreasing odds of falling below the cut-off scores in fine motor ( ${\rm{OR}}=0\!\cdot\!69,{\it P} \lt 0\!\cdot\!001$ ), problem-solving ( ${\rm{OR}} = 0\!\cdot\!82,{\it P} = 0\!\cdot\!02$ ) and personal social ( ${\rm{OR}}=0\!\cdot\!82,{\it P}= 0\!\cdot\!06$ ) domains, and higher weight at baseline was associated with lower odds of falling below the cut-off in communication skills ( ${\rm{OR}}=0\!\cdot\!37,{\it P}=0\!\cdot\!008$ ). The highest wealth index group was associated with a significantly lower odds of falling below the cut-off scores in gross motor ( ${\rm{OR}} ={\rm}0\!\cdot\!19,{\it P} =0\!\cdot\!09)$ .

Table 3. Impact of consistency of egg consumption on ECD

ECD, early childhood development.

* Captured only in month 7. *** P < 0·01, ** P < 0·05, * P < 0·1. Clustered se in parentheses.

In multivariate regression models, there was no significant impact of consistency of egg consumption on domain-specific ASQ scores or on total ASQ scores. Among the covariates, as in previous cases, child height at the baseline remained a significant predictor of ASQ scores in children in all domains except problem-solving, with a 1 cm increase in height associated with 1 to 4 % increase in development scores.

In the second stage of the analysis, we tested the quantity of egg consumption per week (Table 4). Weekly egg consumption was not found to be significantly associated with falling below the cut-off scores in any of the domain. Height was a significant predictor of ECD. Height was found to be significantly associated with lower odds of falling below the cut-off in fine motor, problem-solving and personal social skills.

Table 4. Impact of quantity of egg consumption on ECD

ECD, early childhood development.

* Captured only in month 7.*** P < 0·01, ** P < 0·05, * P < 0·1. Clustered se in parentheses.

For linear regression results (col 6–11), egg consumption was found to be significantly associated with problem-solving skills, where each increase in weekly egg consumption led to 1·9 % increase in scores for problem-solving skills.

In Fig. 2, we present the predicted probability of falling below the cut-off scores with respect to average weekly egg consumption based on logistic regressions. Results show that as weekly egg consumption increases, the probability of falling below the cut-off scores for each domain-specific ASQ scores declines. Noticeably, the impact is largest for gross motor, problem-solving and personal social scores, with the steepest decline and with the largest negative change in the probability between 0 and 9 eggs per week corroborating our previous findings with consistency in egg consumption in Table 3.

Figure 2. Probability of falling below domain specific ASQ cut-off by weekly egg consumption, ASQ, Ages and Stages Questionnaire.

Robustness check

Propensity score matching

Although we use data from a clustered randomised controlled trial where households in the two treatment arms and the control arm were similar at baseline with no significant differences in egg consumption among children, this study investigates the impact of higher egg consumption resulting from the intervention, not the intervention itself. Thus, there could be a potential of selection bias as the previous model does not account for the determinants of feeding eggs itself and if there are systematic differences between households feeding eggs to the children and those who do not (e.g. more engagement with children and better educated). Therefore, to overcome this potential selection bias, the study uses propensity score matching to estimate the effect of treatment (egg consumption) on ECD. The treatment here is defined as binary and equal to ‘1’ if the respondent reported feeding eggs to the child in all the 7 months, and ‘0’ otherwise. Propensity scores were generated using a multivariate logistic regression model with egg consumption in all the months as outcome variable and the baseline predictors as follows; produce enough eggs to feed child one egg per day, mother’s age at first birth, mother’s educational status, household size and wealth index. We also checked for some other determinants of feeding eggs related to mother’s knowledge, attitude and practices towards egg consumption. These include knowledge of nutritional importance of eggs, confidence in preparing eggs, difficulty in getting eggs, whether received nutritional knowledge about eggs, etc., but these factors did not appear to be statistically significant of feeding eggs, therefore were dropped from the model. Propensity score matching one to one matching was performed using nearest neighbour matching. The matching ensures that groups of households who fed their children and those who did not were systematically identical. For brevity (matching results are available on request)), here we only report the results for average treatment effect on treated in Table 5. The coefficients in Table 5 indicate the difference in the scores among the treatment group (children who ate eggs in all the months) due to the treatment (egg consumption in all months). The significant increase in gross motor skills and fine motor skills aligns with our previously reported results in Table 3. However, we do not find significant increase in communication and personal social skills. Hence, the findings confirm the significant contribution of egg consumption on ECD outcomes after ensuring selection issues were controlled for.

Table 5. Effect of egg consumption on child development outcomes – average treatment effect on the treated (ATET)

** P < 0·05, * P < 0·1.

Discussion

The empirical analysis in this article provides supporting evidence for the promotion and inclusion of eggs in child diet to improve ECD. The challenges of food insecurity and malnutrition have been directly linked to the nutrient deficiencies and their contribution to learning and development deficits(Reference Rosales, Reznick and Zeisel32). Delays in ECD contribute to cognitive and motor development deficits, low educational attainment and intergenerational transmission of poverty(Reference Drago, Scharf and Maphula33). Addressing malnutrition, especially in early childhood, is critical to the prevention of poor cognitive and health outcomes. Findings highlighted the impact of full intervention and partial intervention as part of Un Oeuf trial on ECD scores. Children in the full intervention group who received chickens and BCC package reported highest ASQ scores in almost all domains (except personal social) than the children in partial intervention group (who only receive the BCC package) and the control group. Though the difference across partial and control group was not statistically significant indicating the importance of accessibility and availability of eggs over behaviour change communication. Furthermore, the impact of eggs consumption in improving ECD is highlighted with this study with implications in LMIC.

The key findings of the paper hold statistical significance after baseline adjustment for three domains (communication, gross motor and personal social) for consistency of egg consumption and for one domain (problem-solving) for quantity of egg consumption. Result showed that each additional egg per week led to a 1·9 % increase in problem-solving scores in children establishing a dose–response relationship between egg consumption and problem-solving skills scores.

Another important result was surrounding consistency of egg consumption. We find that children who consumed eggs consistently in all months (7) of intervention period were significantly less likely to fall below the cut-off score for communication, gross motor and personal social skills, all of which are crucial elements of overall child development.

Our findings are consistent with Miller et al. (2020), who found egg consumption to be associated with lower odds of having ASQ score in the bottom quartile in Nepal. A study in Ethiopia also reported that children who had increased egg intake attained gross motor skills at a significantly earlier age compared with controls(Reference Omer, Hailu and Whiting34). Similar to ours, the study by Omer et al. (2022) focused on child owned poultry as compared to some other trials that distributed eggs directly to the children(Reference Stewart22,Reference Iannotti, Lutter and Stewart24) . Similarly, a meta-analysis also found that micronutrients played a crucial role in cognitive performance among children aged 6–11 years(Reference Meli, Ali and Mhd Jalil35), which suggests a possible mechanism underlying the findings reported here.

Our findings show that consistent consumption of eggs led to improved ECD in the domains of communication, gross motor and personal social. Eggs contain numerous nutrients that play a vital role in neurocognitive development including Fe, Zn, choline, folate, iodine and long-chain PUFA such as docosahexanoic acid(Reference Faber, Malan and Kruger36). The improved nutritional status as manifested by decreased wasting and underweight(Reference McKune, Stark and Sapp19) among the full intervention group might have also contributed to better ECD. Nutritional status is found to have significant contribution on gross motor and fine motor skills though the findings have been mixed and not strong and consistent(Reference Cavagnari, Guerrero-Vaca and Carpio-Arias37,Reference Zulkarnaen38) . Improved anthropometry is significantly associated with motor development and language skills(Reference Rico-González, Ardigò and Ramírez-Arroyo39). Additionally previous studies(Reference Larson, Martorell and Bauer7) have noted significant association of motor development with executive functions, memory and later cognitive development in children. Therefore, the results of the study indicate the importance of better childhood nutrition and underscoring eggs as a complementary food option.

In Burkina Faso, undernutrition is one of the leading causes of morbidity in children-under-five (Reference Erismann, Knoblauch and Diagbouga40). Behavioural and nutrition interventions, such as the Un Oeuf project, can be effective among vulnerable population for improved decision-making, enhancing knowledge of nutrition, and improving access to livestock production resources. However, there are prevalent gaps in research as more information is needed to find appropriate and effective approaches to improve children’s developmental and nutritional status at specific intervals of childhood development. Research conducted by Miller et al. (2020) states nutritional intervention not only affect the development outcomes immediately but also may be evident and permanent later in life. Thus, the extension of the study can be done by following the children in the later stages of childhood and adolescence and check how and if the effect of intervention continued over time.

This study also has a few limitations. First, ASQ-3 has its own reported rules(Reference Rubio-Codina, Araujo and Attanasio41) in capturing information and indicators of child development, including a stated purpose as a screening tool designed to recognise developmental delays but not affirm child development status. Like others who have used or modified the ASQ for research in LMIC, the research team believes that the ASQ provides meaningful information about different domains of development in children(Reference Justino, Leone and Rolla28,Reference Miller, Neupane and Joshi30,Reference Shrestha, Ranjitkar and Hysing42) . Second, there could have been unique distractions or curiosities for children completing the evaluation that led to differences or biases in their recorded responses. For example, some children in these villages never had access to a mirror. Thus, for many, it was their first time seeing their reflection and could impact the ability to accurately assess childhood development. Third, as this study was a behaviour change intervention, there is possibility of social desirability bias in reporting egg consumption especially in the full intervention group, although the questionnaire was administered to control for the same. Additionally, the weekly egg consumption measure is based on a 7-day dietary recall with a potential to recall bias in the dataset. Finally, other variables, such as environmental exposure, illness, other diet, maternal factors and water and sanitation, could have influenced child development during the study period and were not directly measured in our study.

Conclusion

ECD is closely related to the diets and nutritional status of children. Leveraging data collected during and just after the Un Oeuf intervention in Burkina Faso, this study found that both consistency and quantity of egg consumption led to observed improvements: greater consistency of egg consumption led to lower odds of falling below the ASQ-3 cut-off score for gross motor, personal social skills and the total development score. The study also found that increase in the quantity of egg consumption led to an increase in scores in problem-solving domain. These results underscore the importance of further research on dietary interventions in LMIC, with particular focus on complexity of nutrition as well as culturally appropriate tools that can measure child development in low-resource settings in LMIC. Given the relative affordability and accessibility of eggs, integrating them into nutrition-focused public health programmes could serve as a feasible and effective strategy to improve ECD outcomes in these countries.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980024002490

Acknowledgements

We would like to acknowledge Drs. Patricia Snyder and Maureen Conroy from the University of Florida’s Anita Zucker Center for Excellence in Early Child Development Studies for their contributions in modifying the ASQ for implementation in Burkina Faso. We would also like to acknowledge our study team and trained enumerators in Burkina Faso for their dedication and commitment to this study: Mahamoudoudo Ibriga, Zare Yacouoba, Iliasse Tiemtore, Christian Oueoraogo and Aida Sanou.

Authorship

H.S., S.M., A.O. and A.W. conceived of the study. H.S., S.M., A.O. and A.W. initiated the study design, and H.S., S.M., A.O., A.W. and E.M. were involved in implementation. C.T. conducted statistical analyses and finalised Tables. H.E. led the write up of this manuscript, with significant contribution by S.M., H.S., A.O., C.T., Y.Z. and E.H. All authors contributed to the refinement of the manuscript and approved the final manuscript.

Financial support

This work was funded in whole or part by the United States Agency for International Development (USAID) Bureau for Resilience, Environment and Food Security under Agreement # AID-OAA-L-15–00003 as part of Feed the Future Innovation Lab for Livestock Systems. Additional funding was received from Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation OPP#060115. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations expressed here are those of the authors alone.

Competing interests

We wish to confirm that there are no known conflicts of interest associated with this publication and that the manuscript has been read and approved by all named authors and that there are no other persons who satisfied the criteria for authorship but are not listed. We further confirm that the order of authors listed in the manuscript has been approved by all of us.

Ethics of human subject participation

This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures involving research study participants were approved by the University of Florida Institutional Review Board Committee of Ethics of the Government of Burkina. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

References

UNICEF (2023) Early Childhood Development Index 2030: A New Tool to Measure SDG Indicator 4.2.1. https://data.unicef.org/resources/early-childhood-development-index-2030-ecdi2030/ (accessed February 2024).Google Scholar
MAL-ED Network Investigators (2018) Early childhood cognitive development is affected by interactions among illness, diet, enteropathogens and the home environment: findings from the MAL-ED birth cohort study. BMJ Glob Health 3, e000752.Google Scholar
Singh, A, Yeh, CJ & Boone Blanchard, S (2017) Ages and Stages Questionnaire: a global screening scale. Boletín Médico Hosp Infantil México (English Edition) 74, 512.Google Scholar
Black, MM, Walker, SP, Fernald, LCH et al. (2017) Early childhood development coming of age: science through the life course. Lancet 389, 7790.Google Scholar
Gil, JD, Ewerling, F, Ferreira, LZ et al. (2020) Early childhood suspected developmental delay in 63 low- and middle-income countries: large within- and between-country inequalities documented using national health surveys. J Global Health 10, 010427.Google Scholar
Ibáñez-Alfonso, JA, Company-Córdoba, R, García De La Cadena, C et al. (2021) How living in vulnerable conditions undermines cognitive development: evidence from the pediatric population of Guatemala. Children 8, 90.Google Scholar
Larson, LM, Martorell, R & Bauer, PJ (2018) A path analysis of nutrition, stimulation, and child development among young children in Bihar, India. Child Dev 89, 18711886.Google Scholar
Company-Córdoba, R, Sianes, A, Simpson, IC et al. (2021) Cognitive interventions in children and adolescents from low socioeconomic status backgrounds: a systematic review protocol of randomized controlled trials. Syst Rev 10, 187.Google Scholar
Nurliyana, AR, Mohd Shariff, Z, Mohd Taib, MN et al. (2016) Early nutrition, growth and cognitive development of infants from birth to 2 years in Malaysia: a study protocol. BMC Pediatr 16, 160.Google Scholar
Larson, LM & Yousafzai, AK (2017) A meta-analysis of nutrition interventions on mental development of children under-two in low- and middle-income countries. Matern Child Nutr 13, e12229.Google Scholar
USAID (2018) Nutrition Profile. Burkina Faso: USAID.Google Scholar
World Bank (2021) UNICEF, WHO, World Bank: Joint Child Malnutrition Estimates (JME). https://data.unicef.org/resources/jme-report-2023/ (accessed February 2024).Google Scholar
Asare, H, Rosi, A, Faber, M et al. (2022) Animal-source foods as a suitable complementary food for improved physical growth in 6 to 24-month-old children in low- and middle-income countries: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Br J Nutr 128, 24532463.Google Scholar
Arabi, M, Frongillo, EA, Avula, R et al. (2012) Infant and young child feeding in developing countries. Child Dev 83, 3245.Google Scholar
Eaton, JC, Rothpletz-Puglia, P, Dreker, MR et al. (2019) Effectiveness of provision of animal-source foods for supporting optimal growth and development in children 6 to 59 months of age. Cochrane Database of Syst Rev issue 2, CD012818.Google Scholar
Headey, D (2018) Animal Sourced Foods and Child Nutrition in South Asia: Policy Priorities. LANSA: Leveraging Institutions for Nutrition in South Asia. https://ebrary.ifpri.org/digital/collection/p15738coll16/id/1027/ (accessed February 2024).Google Scholar
Adesogan, AT, Havelaar, AH, McKune, SL et al. (2020) Animal source foods: sustainability problem or malnutrition and sustainability solution? Perspective matters. Global Food Secur 25, 100325.Google Scholar
Gallegos-Riofrío, CA, Waters, WF, Salvador, JM et al. (2018) The Lulun Project’s social marketing strategy in a trial to introduce eggs during complementary feeding in Ecuador. Maternal Child Nutr 14, e12700.Google Scholar
McKune, SL, Stark, H, Sapp, AC et al. (2020) Behavior change, egg consumption, and child nutrition: a cluster randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics 146, e2020007930.Google Scholar
Moore, EV, Singh, N, Serra, R et al. (2022) Household decision-making, women’s empowerment, and increasing egg consumption in children under five in rural Burkina Faso: observations from a cluster randomized controlled trial. Front Sustain Food Syst 6, 1034618.Google Scholar
Iannotti, LL, Lutter, CK, Bunn, DA et al. (2014) Eggs: the uncracked potential for improving maternal and young child nutrition among the world’s poor. Nutr Rev 72, 355368.Google Scholar
Stewart, ME (2018) The Effects of Consuming Eggs on the Physical and Cognitive Development of Food-Insecure Haitian Children. Fayetteville, AR: University of Arkansas.Google Scholar
Lutter, CK, Iannotti, LL & Stewart, CP (2018) The potential of a simple egg to improve maternal and child nutrition. Matern Child Nutr 14, e12678.Google Scholar
Iannotti, LL, Lutter, CK, Stewart, CP et al. (2017) Eggs in early complementary feeding and child growth: a randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics 140, e20163459.Google Scholar
Stark, H, Omer, A, Wereme N’Diaye, A et al. (2021) The Un Oeuf study: design, methods and baseline data from a cluster randomised controlled trial to increase child egg consumption in Burkina Faso. Matern Child Nutr 17, e13069.Google Scholar
Squires, J & Bricker, D (2009) Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ): A Parent Completed Child Monitoring System, 3rd ed. Baltimore, MD: Brooks Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Alvik, A & Grøholt, B (2011) Examination of the cut-off scores determined by the Ages and Stages Questionnaire in a population-based sample of 6 month-old Norwegian infants. BMC Pediatr 11, 117.Google Scholar
Justino, P, Leone, M, Rolla, P et al. (2023) Improving parenting practices for early child development: experimental evidence from Rwanda. J Eur Econ Assoc 21, 15101550.Google Scholar
Miller, AC, Rumaldo, N, Soplapuco, G et al. (2021) Success at scale: outcomes of Community-Based Neurodevelopment Intervention (CASITA) for Children Ages 6–20 months With Risk of Delay in Lima, Peru. Child Dev 92, e1275e1289.Google Scholar
Miller, LC, Neupane, S, Joshi, N et al. (2020) Diet quality over time is associated with better development in rural Nepali children. Matern Child Nutr 16, e12964.Google Scholar
Mbabazi, J, Pesu, H, Mutumba, R et al. (2024) Correlates of early child development among children with stunting: a cross-sectional study in Uganda. Matern Child Nutr 20, e13619.Google Scholar
Rosales, FJ, Reznick, JS & Zeisel, SH (2009) Understanding the role of nutrition in the brain and behavioral development of toddlers and preschool children: identifying and addressing methodological barriers. Nutr Neurosci 12, 190202.Google Scholar
Drago, F, Scharf, RJ, Maphula, A et al. (2020) Psychosocial and environmental determinants of child cognitive development in rural South Africa and Tanzania: findings from the mal-ed cohort. BMC Public Health 20, 505.Google Scholar
Omer, A, Hailu, D & Whiting, SJ (2022) Egg consumption of children under two years of age through a child-owned poultry and nutrition intervention in rural Ethiopia: a community-based randomized controlled trial. J Agric Food Res 9, 100354.Google Scholar
Meli, AM, Ali, A, Mhd Jalil, AM et al. (2021) Effects of physical activity and micronutrients on cognitive performance in children aged 6 to 11 years: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Medicina 58, 57.Google Scholar
Faber, M, Malan, L, Kruger, HS et al. (2022) Potential of egg as complementary food to improve nutrient intake and dietary diversity. Nutrients 14, 3396.Google Scholar
Cavagnari, BM, Guerrero-Vaca, DJ, Carpio-Arias, TV et al. (2023) The double burden of malnutrition and gross motor development in infants: a cross-sectional study. Clin Nutr 42, 11811188.Google Scholar
Zulkarnaen, Z (2019) The influence of nutritional status on gross and fine motor skills development in early childhood. ASS 15, 75.Google Scholar
Rico-González, M, Ardigò, LP, Ramírez-Arroyo, AP et al. (2024) Anthropometric influence on preschool children’s physical fitness and motor skills: a systematic review. J Funct Morphol Kinesiol 9, 95.Google Scholar
Erismann, S, Knoblauch, AM, Diagbouga, S et al. (2017) Prevalence and risk factors of undernutrition among schoolchildren in the Plateau Central and Centre-Ouest regions of Burkina Faso. Infect Dis Poverty 6, 17.Google Scholar
Rubio-Codina, M, Araujo, MC, Attanasio, O et al. (2016) Concurrent validity and feasibility of short tests currently used to measure early childhood development in large scale studies. PLoS ONE 11, e0160962.Google Scholar
Shrestha, M, Ranjitkar, S, Hysing, M et al. (2023) The Relationship between the Ages and Stages Questionnaire, 3rd Edition Scores in Early Childhood and Future Cognitive Abilities in Young Nepalese Children. SSRN. https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=4508978 (accessed February 2024).Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics by intervention groups

Figure 1

Figure 1. Pattern of egg consumption in follow-up months across research arms.

Figure 2

Table 2. Early child development (ECD) scores by intervention arm

Figure 3

Table 3. Impact of consistency of egg consumption on ECD

Figure 4

Table 4. Impact of quantity of egg consumption on ECD

Figure 5

Figure 2. Probability of falling below domain specific ASQ cut-off by weekly egg consumption, ASQ, Ages and Stages Questionnaire.

Figure 6

Table 5. Effect of egg consumption on child development outcomes – average treatment effect on the treated (ATET)

Supplementary material: File

Ernyey et al. supplementary material

Ernyey et al. supplementary material
Download Ernyey et al. supplementary material(File)
File 25.2 KB