Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-21T03:37:31.489Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Association between breakfast consumption and educational outcomes in 9–11-year-old children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 September 2015

Hannah J Littlecott*
Affiliation:
Centre for the Development and Evaluation of Complex Interventions for Public Health Improvement (DECIPHer), School of Social Sciences, Cardiff University, 1–3 Museum Place, Cardiff CF10 3BD, UK
Graham F Moore
Affiliation:
Centre for the Development and Evaluation of Complex Interventions for Public Health Improvement (DECIPHer), School of Social Sciences, Cardiff University, 1–3 Museum Place, Cardiff CF10 3BD, UK
Laurence Moore
Affiliation:
MRC/CSO Social and Public Health Sciences Unit, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, UK
Ronan A Lyons
Affiliation:
Centre for the Development and Evaluation of Complex Interventions for Public Health Improvement (DECIPHer), Centre for Health Information, Research and Evaluation, Swansea University, Swansea, UK
Simon Murphy
Affiliation:
Centre for the Development and Evaluation of Complex Interventions for Public Health Improvement (DECIPHer), School of Social Sciences, Cardiff University, 1–3 Museum Place, Cardiff CF10 3BD, UK
*
* Corresponding author: Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

Breakfast consumption has been consistently associated with health outcomes and cognitive functioning in schoolchildren. Evidence of direct links with educational outcomes remains equivocal. We aimed to examine the link between breakfast consumption in 9–11-year-old children and educational outcomes obtained 6–18 months later.

Design

Data on individual-level free school meal entitlement and educational outcomes (Statutory Assessment Tests (SATs) at Key Stage 2) were obtained via the SAIL databank and linked to earlier data collected on breakfast consumption. Multilevel modelling assessed associations between breakfast consumption and SATs.

Setting

Trial of the Primary School Free Breakfast Initiative in Wales.

Subjects

Year 5 and 6 students, n 3093 (baseline) and n 3055 (follow-up).

Results

Significant associations were found between all dietary behaviours and better performance in SATs, adjusted for gender and individual- and school-level free school meal entitlement (OR=1·95; CI 1·58, 2·40 for breakfast, OR=1·08; CI 1·04, 1·13 for healthy breakfast items). No association was observed between number of unhealthy breakfast items consumed and educational performance. Association of breakfast consumption with educational performance was stronger where the measure of breakfast consumption was more proximal to SATs tests (OR=2·02 measured 6 months prior to SATs, OR=1·61 measured 18 months prior).

Conclusions

Significant positive associations between self-reported breakfast consumption and educational outcomes were observed. Future research should aim to explore the mechanisms by which breakfast consumption and educational outcomes are linked, and understand how to promote breakfast consumption among schoolchildren. Communicating findings of educational benefits to schools may help to enhance buy-in to efforts to improve health behaviours of pupils.

Type
Research Papers
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2015

Interventions to improve child health are increasingly delivered via the school setting, in part because this provides opportunities to reach large numbers of children simultaneously. Such interventions range from health education programmes( Reference Brown and Summerbell 1 ) to holistic settings-based approaches( Reference Rothwell, Shepherd and Murphy 2 ). Reviews indicate that interventions based on education alone are typically ineffective( Reference Thomas, McLellan and Perera 3 ), while complex interventions including components at multiple levels often have greater overall effects( Reference Waters, de Silva Sanigorski and Hall 4 ). Hence, effective interventions often require significant changes to the structures and practices of schools. However, directing time and resources towards improving child health is commonly seen as diverting time away from schools’ core business of educating pupils, in part due to pressures from sources including regulatory bodies that emphasise educational attainment( Reference Bonell, Humphrey and Fletcher 5 ).

However, this resistance to delivery of health improvement interventions overlooks the potential synergy between health and education, and the growing evidence that improving health may also improve educational outcomes. In the UK, there is a growing policy emphasis on the need to understand links between health and education agendas in schools, in order to align such programmes with the ‘core business’ of the school and achieve buy-in( Reference Bonell, Humphrey and Fletcher 5 ). For example, Public Health England has produced a briefing for school staff highlighting the reciprocal relationship between pupil health and well-being and attainment, emphasising the importance of a whole-school approach( 6 ). Moreover, recent education reviews in Wales and Scotland have highlighted physical and mental health and well-being as a priority( Reference Donaldson 7 ).

The evidence base linking health behaviours to educational outcomes is at present underdeveloped; there is little prospective evidence, while few public health researchers evaluate the impacts of their interventions on education( Reference Langford, Bonell and Jones 8 ). However, strong cross-sectional associations have been found between educational outcomes and health behaviours( Reference Murray, Low and Hollis 9 ), such as physical activity( Reference Rasberry, Lee and Robin 10 ) and substance use( Reference Lynskey and Hall 11 ). Links between educational outcomes and dietary factors remain equivocal, in part due to the difficulty defining, identifying and measuring the key dietary factors( Reference Rampersaud, Pereira and Girard 12 ).

One dietary behaviour for which effects on education are often presumed, and which is commonly targeted by school-based interventions, is breakfast consumption. In one UK survey of fifty-nine schools, Hoyland et al.( Reference Hoyland, McWilliams and Duff 13 ) found that 14 % of 7–15-year-old children reported skipping breakfast, with one-third reporting that they did not eat anything until lunchtime. Breakfast consumption is also socially patterned, thus putting less affluent children at a higher risk of experiencing negative health outcomes associated with skipping breakfast( Reference Moore, Tapper and Murphy 14 , Reference O’Dea and Caputi 15 ). In the UK, breakfast clubs were offered in almost half of schools in England by 2012, with delivery concentrated in areas of deprivation( Reference Hoyland, McWilliams and Duff 13 ). In Wales, the Welsh Government’s Primary School Free Breakfast Initiative was implemented from 2005( Reference Moore, Moore and Tapper 16 , Reference Thomas 17 ).

However, while breakfast has been consistently associated with health outcomes( Reference Rampersaud, Pereira and Girard 12 , Reference Ells, Hillier and Shucksmith 18 ), evidence regarding links to educational outcomes remains equivocal. While a body of research examines impacts of breakfast consumption on acute cognitive performance in terms of episodic memory, visual searching and concentration( Reference Cooper, Bandelow and Nevill 19 Reference Wesnes, Pincock and Richardson 22 ), fewer studies have examined associations with direct measures of educational performance. In their review of breakfast habits and academic performance, Rampersaud et al.( Reference Rampersaud, Pereira and Girard 12 ) found significant improvements in achievement test scores for Jamaican children who consumed breakfast during two randomised controlled trials. A further randomised controlled trial found improved attendance rates for Peruvian children in the school breakfast arm( Reference Jacoby, Cueto and Pollitt 20 ). They concluded that future trials should investigate the longitudinal effects of breakfast consumption on academic outcomes and control for socio-economic status (SES)( Reference Rampersaud, Pereira and Girard 12 ). A recent review concluded that there remained insufficient evidence for the relationship between breakfast consumption and longer-term attainment( 23 ), while Adolphus et al.( Reference Adolphus, Lawton and Dye 24 ) reported that few such studies adopt longitudinal designs, adjusted adequately for confounding variables such as SES, or included large samples and validated measures of academic performance. In fact, no previous studies have examined the role of breakfast consumption in mediating SES inequalities in educational outcomes.

The current study uses data collected as part of the cluster-randomised controlled trial of the Welsh Government’s Primary School Free Breakfast Initiative in a large representative UK population, while also examining whether any effect observed differs by SES. A cluster-randomised trial of this scheme demonstrated effects on the quality of children’s breakfast foods and reduction in socio-economic inequality in breakfast skipping, despite a lack of main effect on breakfast skipping( Reference Moore, Murphy and Chaplin 25 ). It was not possible to link the intervention to educational outcomes, as control schools took up the scheme between completion of the trial and collection of educational performance data. However, within the present paper, we examine longitudinal associations between breakfast consumption (in terms of both whether children eat breakfast and the quality of foods children eat for breakfast) and subsequent exam results. A secondary analysis examines whether better educational outcomes were achieved in schools receiving the free school breakfast intervention during the trial period. The following research questions were addressed:

  1. 1. Is there a link between breakfast consumption and other dietary behaviours and educational outcomes in 9–11-year-old children?

  2. 2. Did any existing relationship differ in strength according to the proximity of the dietary recall measures to the collection of educational outcomes data?

  3. 3. Are socio-economic differences in educational performance impacted by SES differences in breakfast consumption?

Methods

Sampling and participants

Full details of methods and sampling procedures are described elsewhere( Reference Moore, Moore and Tapper 16 ). Primary schools in Wales were approached to take part in a cluster-randomised controlled trial of the Welsh Government’s Primary School Free Breakfast Initiative. A total of 4350 students in Years 5 and 6 (i.e. aged 9–11 years) at baseline and 4472 at 12-month follow-up completed classroom-based attitude and dietary recall questionnaires within the 111 schools that took part. Due to increased class sizes in some schools, the eligible pool of children increased slightly, although response rates were identical at baseline and follow-up (88·3 %). Of the 4350 and 4472 children who completed baseline and follow-up measures, individual-level free school meal (FSM) entitlement and educational outcomes data were obtained for 3093 (71·1 %) and 3055 (68·3 %). The trial used a repeated cross-sectional design, sampling Year 5 and 6 pupils at baseline and follow-up. However, a nested cohort of 1216 children (who were in Year 5 at baseline but Year 6 at follow-up) provided data at both baseline (16–18 months prior to collection of educational outcomes data in May) and follow-up (4–6 months prior to collection of educational outcomes data in May). A secondary analysis therefore examines links between reported breakfast consumption at these two time points and educational performance tests among this sub-sample.

Measures

Deprivation

This was assessed using data on FSM entitlement, including a school-level measure (percentage of children entitled to FSM within the whole school) and children’s own entitlement to FSM (yes or no).

Dietary recall questionnaire

The questionnaire was a modified version of the Day in the Life Questionnaire( Reference Edmunds and Ziebland 26 ). This measure has been validated against 24 h recall interviews with a sub-sample of children from the present study and offers an acceptable level of validity and reliability( Reference Moore, Tapper and Murphy 27 ). The measure covered a period slightly in excess of 24 h; children were asked to list all foods and drinks consumed at chronologically ordered time points throughout the previous day and for breakfast on the day of reporting. Outcome variables are the proportion of children consuming less than two breakfasts over 2 d, the number of healthy items (i.e. cereals, bread, fruits and milk products) consumed for breakfast, the number of unhealthy items (i.e. crisps and sweet snacks) consumed for breakfast, the number of fruit and vegetable portions consumed during the rest of the day and the number of unhealthy items (i.e. crisps and sweet snacks) consumed during the rest of the day.

Statutory Assessment Tests

Following the trial, children’s reports of their breakfast consumption behaviours were linked to their scores on Key Stage 2 Statutory Assessment Tests (SATs). SATs are mandatory tests taken by all Year 6 (aged 10–11 years) students in Wales and England in mid-May. In 2005, when this trial was conducted, students were assessed in English, Maths and Science and graded with levels 1–6.

Procedure

For the original trial, parents were informed of the research by means of a letter and information sheet sent home with children and were asked to contact the school if they did not wish their child to participate in the study. Parents of fifteen children requested that their child be excluded. At each data collection, children were also informed that they were under no obligation to participate. Class-level measures were completed in the morning (between 09.00 and 11.00 hours) as a supervised classroom exercise with a maximum class size of forty children. For the dietary recall measure and the cognitive measures, the researcher read out the instructions and asked children to complete the task independently from one another. If children had finished or needed help with spelling or further clarification, they were asked to put their hands up. Three members of the research team were present to assist children. The main trial design and results are presented in detail elsewhere( Reference Moore, Moore and Tapper 16 , Reference Murphy, Moore and Tapper 28 ). The SAIL (Secure Anonymised Information Linkage) databank is a data warehouse that was established at the Health Information Research Unit at the College of Medicine at Swansea University. It brings together a wide range of person-based data, using a split-file approach to anonymisation to overcome issues of confidentiality and disclosure( Reference Lyons, Jones and John 29 ), and operates within a robust series of guidelines in line with the Caldicott principles and the National Information Governance Board for Health and Social Care( Reference Ford, Jones and Verplancke 30 ). Participating children were each assigned an Anonymised Linking Field (ALF_E)( Reference Lyons, Jones and John 29 ) and thereby linked to the National Pupil Database for Wales, a version of which is held on the SAIL databank. This allowed the dietary data to be linked to the SATs and individual FSM entitlement data for each participant.

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics were conducted to describe the characteristics of participants. Mixed-effects binary logistic regression models, with pupils nested within the school, were used to assess the association between breakfast consumption and SATs results. A P value of 0·05 was considered a significant association. Whole-sample analyses were conducted using separate models for baseline and follow-up samples on dietary behaviour. Primary analysis focused on the baseline data, which were collected before any intervention related to breakfast was implemented. The first model included individual-level demographics (gender and FSM entitlement), while the second model combined individual-level demographics and school-level FSM entitlement. Being male and being ineligible for FSM were used as the reference groups for the independent variables within the multilevel models (i.e. male=0, female=1), whereas for school-level FSM entitlement, a higher score=a higher percentage of pupils entitled to FSM (i.e. higher deprivation). Subsequent models added terms for each breakfast consumption measure: ‘number of healthy breakfast items consumed’, ‘number of unhealthy breakfast items consumed’, ‘number of sweets and crisps consumed’ or ‘number of fruit and vegetable portions consumed’. These analyses were run twice for the nested cohort (n 1216), first examining links between breakfast consumption at baseline and educational performance, and second examining associations of reported behaviour at follow-up. A secondary analysis compared educational outcomes for children in intervention and control schools. Follow-up data collections for the school breakfast trial were completed by April 2006 in Phase 1 of the trial (Communities First schools) and by February 2007 in Phase 2 (after which wait-list controls were free to set up the intervention). Hence, to minimise the risk of contamination, the present analyses include only children taking their SATs in May 2006 for Communities First schools and in 2007 in non-Communities First schools (i.e. those children in Year 6 when the trial ended). All analyses were conducted using the statistical software package Stata version 13.

Results

The samples are described in Table 1. The nested cohort was comparable to the full sample in terms of FSM entitlement, gender, breakfast consumption behaviours and educational outcomes.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics of the samples of Year 5 and 6 students aged 9–11 years participating in the cluster-randomised controlled trial of the Welsh Government’s Primary School Free Breakfast Initiative

* FSM entitlement refers to those students from low-income families who are, therefore, eligible to receive free school meals at lunchtime.

Data are presented as median and interquartile range (IQR).

In 2005, when this trial was conducted, students were assessed in English, Maths and Science and graded with levels 1–6.

§ Data (all such values) are numbers.

|| Data (all such values) are percentages.

Dietary behaviours and educational outcomes

Table 2 shows the results of the model using the baseline and follow-up samples. Being female, not being entitled to FSM and a lower school-level FSM entitlement were all independently associated with higher SATs scores. The subsequent models, which included dietary behaviours, showed that breakfast consumption, number of healthy breakfast items consumed for breakfast, number of sweets and crisps consumed throughout the rest of the day and number of fruit and vegetable portions consumed throughout the rest of the day, adjusted for gender and individual- and school-level FSM entitlement, were all significantly and positively associated with educational performance. No association was observed between the number of unhealthy breakfast items consumed and educational performance. Associations of school- and individual-level measures of SES with education remained unchanged after inclusion of dietary variables, providing no evidence that their association with educational performance was mediated by SES differences for any outcome. The intra-cluster correlation coefficient declined from 0·8 in the model containing only individual-level demographic variables to 0·7 in models containing school-level FSM and dietary variables, indicating that only a small proportion of school-level variance is explained by these additional variables.

Table 2 Odds ratios and 95 % confidence intervals from multilevel binary logistic regression analysis using individual- and school-level demographics and baseline (n 3093) and follow-up data (N 3055) to investigate the associations between dietary behaviours and improved educational outcomes among Year 5 and 6 students aged 9–11 years participating in the cluster-randomised controlled trial of the Welsh Government’s Primary School Free Breakfast Initiative

ICC, intra-cluster correlation coefficient.

Significant results are highlighted in bold.

* Reference category for gender=male.

FSM entitlement refers to those students from low-income families who are, therefore, eligible to receive free school meals at lunchtime (for the individual-level FSM variable, no=0 and yes=1).

School-level FSM refers to the percentage of students within the whole school who are from low-income families and are, therefore, eligible to receive free school meals at lunchtime.

Proximity of the dietary measures and examination date

Table 3 shows that among the nested cohort of children who provided measures at baseline and follow-up, the association of breakfast consumption with educational performance was stronger where the measure of breakfast consumption was more proximal to the collection of educational outcomes data (OR=2·02 for follow-up compared with OR=1·61 for baseline). For number of healthy breakfast items consumed, number of unhealthy breakfast items consumed, number of sweets and crisps consumed and number of fruit and vegetable portions, associations were similar whether using the proximal or more distal measure. As with the whole group, all dietary measures were associated with educational performance except for the consumption of unhealthy breakfast items.

Table 3 Nested cohort analysis (n 1216) to investigate the associations between dietary behaviours and improved educational outcomes among Year 5 and 6 students aged 9–11 years participating in the cluster-randomised controlled trial of the Welsh Government’s Primary School Free Breakfast Initiative for whom dietary behaviour is available at baseline and follow-up

Significant results are highlighted in bold.

Intention-to-treat analysis of intervention effects on educational outcomes

Following the intervention, 71·2 % (n 632) of children in the intervention group and 73·7 % (n 691) in the control group achieved a ‘high’ level of educational outcome. Results from the multilevel analysis indicated that between-group differences were not significant (OR=0·87; 95 % CI 0·65, 1·16; n 1826). Hence, there was no evidence of an intervention effect on educational outcomes.

Discussion

In the present study, breakfast consumption, number of healthy breakfast items consumed, number of sweets and crisps consumed and number of fruit and vegetable portions consumed were all associated with significantly better educational performance at baseline and follow-up. The association of breakfast consumption with SATs scores was stronger at follow-up (i.e. 6 months prior to SATs) compared with baseline (i.e. 18 months prior to SATs), although a significant association remained in both analyses. No evidence of an intervention effect on educational outcomes was observed.

While a substantial body of existing literature focuses on links between breakfast eating behaviours and acute measures of concentration and memory( Reference Rampersaud, Pereira and Girard 12 ), the present analysis shows a meaningful link between dietary behaviours and concrete measures of academic attainment, which could have practical implications. The finding that associations between breakfast consumption behaviours and academic performance within the nested cohort analysis remained regardless of whether measured 6 or 18 months prior to examination perhaps indicates that breakfast consumption behaviours are relatively stable at this age, prior to moving to secondary school. Moreover, the finding that this association between dietary factors and SATs scores for the whole sample was significant at both baseline and follow-up provides further evidence that breakfast consumption behaviours may be relatively stable at this age and suggests that there may be a longitudinal effect of breakfast consumption on educational performance.

The association observed between healthy breakfast items and SATs scores v. the lack of association between unhealthy items and SATs scores at baseline and follow-up is consistent with an emerging body of research which suggests that a breakfast consisting of foods with a lower glycaemic index, which release energy steadily throughout the morning, may have a positive effect on students’ cognitive functioning, health, school attendance and academic outcomes( Reference Edefonti, Rosato and Parpinel 31 Reference Mahoney, Taylor and Kanarek 33 ). For example, Mahoney et al.( Reference Mahoney, Taylor and Kanarek 33 ) conducted a highly controlled experiment that manipulated breakfast content among 9–11-year-old children once per week for three weeks, finding that both boys and girls who had consumed breakfast foods with a higher glycaemic index had improved spatial memory and girls to have improved short-term memory. A further study by O’Dea and Mugridge( Reference O’Dea and Mugridge 34 ) observed an association between habitual breakfast quality and literacy scores, but not with numeracy scores.

However, there was also a significant association between consumption of sweets and crisps (eaten later in the day) and SATs scores in our analysis. One possible explanation for this could be an effect on more undernourished children. If children who are undernourished increase their energy intake, regardless of what they are eating, hunger is likely to be decreased and concentration and subsequent academic achievement improved. This is demonstrated by a 6-month school breakfast programme that observed significantly improved mathematics grades in undernourished children whose nutritional status improved to ‘adequate’( Reference Kleinman, Hall and Green 32 ). Notably, while only one in five children ate sweets or crisps at breakfast, demonstrating no association with academic performance, a vast majority of children ate these items later on in the day. Further research is required to investigate any difference in the effects of breakfast consumption on children from families of differing levels of deprivation and the mechanisms by which this may occur.

Earlier results from the trial, reported in this journal, indicate that the initiative played a significant role in reducing SES inequality in pupils’ consumption of breakfast, which has been shown to be a socially patterned behaviour( Reference Moore, Tapper and Murphy 14 ). Hence, universal as opposed to targeted free breakfast programmes could play an important role in reducing health and educational inequalities. For example, more affluent children are likely to simply exchange a healthy breakfast at home for a healthy breakfast at school, while poorer children may exchange a poorer-quality home breakfast for a healthy school breakfast, gaining more from this exchange( Reference Moore, Tapper and Murphy 14 , Reference Moore, Murphy and Chaplin 25 ). However, the current analysis provides no evidence that breakfast consumption mediated the relationship between SES and educational outcomes. Hence, while the study provides some support for the notion that promoting breakfast might improve overall academic performance, it provides no evidence that improving breakfast consumption would reduce inequality in educational outcomes.

Strengths and limitations

Major strengths of the present study include the large sample and the longitudinal design. The measure of diet used in the current study involved dietary recall of the whole previous day and breakfast on the day of reporting. There have been several problems identified with the measurement of breakfast due to ambiguity over how it is defined in terms of frequency, time of day and type of food( Reference Rampersaud, Pereira and Girard 12 ). However, the measure used in the present study has shown adequate validity and reliability by comparison to more expensive and labour-intensive 24 h recalls. A further strength of the study is the use of SATs scores as a measure of educational attainment. SATs scores provide a measure that may be comparable across developed countries where age-specific tests are consistently sat by schoolchildren. This is particularly important due to the fact that studies investigating the effect of breakfast have often relied on self-report( Reference Lien 35 ) or used varied and simplistic measures of academic achievement, such as tests of acute cognitive function( Reference Adolphus, Lawton and Dye 24 ). Intention-to-treat analysis was hampered by the fact that some control schools may have set up the free school breakfast scheme before SATs tests were taken. However, this would have diluted any potential intervention effect, rather than eliminating it altogether.

Conclusions

Overall, these results demonstrate the association between consumption of healthy breakfast items and academic outcomes among primary-school children, with a stronger association observed when breakfast consumption is more proximal to the SATs tests. They also demonstrate that the significant independent associations of school- and individual-level SES with educational outcomes are not altered by addition of terms of dietary behaviours. Hence, while the study provides some support for the notion that promoting breakfast might improve overall academic performance, it provides no evidence that improving breakfast consumption would reduce inequality in educational outcomes.

Future research should aim to understand the causal mechanisms by which breakfast consumption and other health behaviours may improve academic outcomes. While this survey was conducted with children of primary-school age, research has shown that breakfast consumption declines once children begin secondary school( Reference Hoyland, McWilliams and Duff 13 ). This suggests that an interesting avenue for further research would be to investigate the effects of any drop in breakfast eating behaviour in secondary school on trajectories in young people’s educational performance. In addition, measures of educational performance should be integrated into trials of interventions to promote breakfast consumption and other health behaviours, in order to understand how interventions might promote both educational and health benefits simultaneously( Reference Langford, Bonell and Jones 8 ). Such results could be vital in helping to align health improvement with the ‘core business’ of schools( Reference Bonell, Humphrey and Fletcher 5 , 6 ) and to argue for the implementation of universal as opposed to targeted interventions.

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements: The authors thank Janine Hale and Chris Roberts in the Welsh Government for their support. Financial support: H.J.L. is supported by a PhD studentship funded by the Medical Research Council and Cardiff University. G.F.M. is supported by an MRC Population Health Scientist Fellowship (MR/K021400/1). The study was funded via the National Preventive Research Initiative (http://www.npri.org.uk). Funding from the British Heart Foundation; Cancer Research UK; Department of Health; Diabetes UK; Economic and Social Research Council; Medical Research Council; Research and Development Office for the Northern Ireland Health and Social Services; Chief Scientist Office, Scottish Executive Health Department; The Stroke Association; Welsh Government; and World Cancer Research Fund is gratefully acknowledged. The work was undertaken with the support of the Centre for the Development and Evaluation of Complex Interventions for Public Health Improvement (DECIPHer), a UK CRC Public Health Research Centre of Excellence. Joint funding (MR/KO232331/1) from the British Heart Foundation, Cancer Research UK, Economic and Social Research Council, Medical Research Council, the Welsh Government and the Wellcome Trust, under the auspices of the UK Clinical Research Collaboration, is gratefully acknowledged. The funders had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article. Conflict of interest: None. Authorship: H.J.L. wrote the paper and conducted the statistics. G.F.M. wrote the paper and conducted the statistics. L.M. was involved in the study design and implementation and provided comments on the manuscript. R.A.L. was involved in the study design and implementation and provided comments on the manuscript. S.M. was involved in the study design and implementation and helped to prepare and provided comments on the manuscript. Ethics of human subject participation: This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures involving human subjects/patients were approved by the Cardiff University Social Science Ethics Committee. Opt-out consent was used for all parents/guardians and written assent was obtained from all subjects/patients.

References

1. Brown, T & Summerbell, C (2009) Systematic review of school‐based interventions that focus on changing dietary intake and physical activity levels to prevent childhood obesity: an update to the obesity guidance produced by the National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence. Obes Rev 10, 110141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
2. Rothwell, H, Shepherd, M, Murphy, S et al. (2010) Implementing a social-ecological model of health in Wales. Health Educ 110, 471489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
3. Thomas, R, McLellan, J & Perera, R (2013) School-based programmes for preventing smoking. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 4, CD001293.Google Scholar
4. Waters, E, de Silva Sanigorski, A, Hall, BJ et al. (2011) Interventions for preventing obesity in children. Cochrane Database Syst Rev issue 12, CD001871.Google ScholarPubMed
5. Bonell, C, Humphrey, N, Fletcher, A et al. (2014) Why schools should promote students’ health and wellbeing. BMJ 348, g3078.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6. Public Health England (2014) The Link Between Pupil Health and Wellbeing and Attainment: A Briefing for Head Teachers, Governors and Staff in Education Settings. London: Public Health England.Google Scholar
7. Donaldson, G (2015) Successful Futures: Independent Review of Curriculum and Assessment Arrangements in Wales. Cardiff: Welsh Government.Google Scholar
8. Langford, R, Bonell, CP, Jones, HE et al. (2014) The WHO Health Promoting School framework for improving the health and well‐being of students and their academic achievement. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 4, CD008958.Google Scholar
9. Murray, NG, Low, BJ, Hollis, C et al. (2007) Coordinated school health programs and academic achievement: a systematic review of the literature. J Sch Health 77, 589600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10. Rasberry, CN, Lee, SM, Robin, L et al. (2011) The association between school-based physical activity, including physical education, and academic performance: a systematic review of the literature. Prev Med 52, Suppl. 1, S10S20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11. Lynskey, M & Hall, W (2000) The effects of adolescent cannabis use on educational attainment: a review. Addiction 95, 16211630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12. Rampersaud, GC, Pereira, MA, Girard, BL et al. (2005) Breakfast habits, nutritional status, body weight, and academic performance in children and adolescents. J Am Diet Assoc 105, 743760.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13. Hoyland, A, McWilliams, KA, Duff, RJ et al. (2012) Breakfast consumption in UK schoolchildren and provision of school breakfast clubs. Nutr Bull 37, 232240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14. Moore, GF, Tapper, K, Murphy, S et al. (2007) Associations between deprivation, attitudes towards eating breakfast and breakfast eating behaviours in 9–11-year-olds. Public Health Nutr 10, 582589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15. O’Dea, JA & Caputi, P (2001) Association between socioeconomic status, weight, age and gender, and the body image and weight control practices of 6- to 19-year-old children and adolescents. Health Educ Res 16, 521532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16. Moore, L, Moore, GF, Tapper, K et al. (2007) Free breakfasts in schools: design and conduct of a cluster randomised controlled trial of the Primary School Free Breakfast Initiative in Wales [ISRCTN18336527]. BMC Public Health 7, 258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17. Thomas, M (2012) Free school breakfasts in Wales. The Guardian, 20 June; available at http://www.theguardian.com/teacher-network/teacher-blog/2012/jun/20/free-school-breakfasts-wales Google Scholar
18. Ells, LJ, Hillier, FC, Shucksmith, J et al. (2008) A systematic review of the effect of dietary exposure that could be achieved through normal dietary intake on learning and performance of school-aged children of relevance to UK schools. Br J Nutr 100, 927936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19. Cooper, SB, Bandelow, S & Nevill, ME (2011) Breakfast consumption and cognitive function in adolescent schoolchildren. Physiol Behav 103, 431439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20. Jacoby, E, Cueto, S & Pollitt, E (1996) Benefits of a school breakfast programme among Andean children in Huaraz, Peru. Food Nutr Bull 17, 5464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
21. Vaisman, N, Voet, H, Akivis, A et al. (1996) Effect of breakfast timing on the cognitive functions of elementary school students. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 150, 10891092.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22. Wesnes, KA, Pincock, C, Richardson, D et al. (2003) Breakfast reduces declines in attention and memory over the morning in schoolchildren. Appetite 41, 329331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23. Public Health England (2013) Breakfast and Cognition: Review of the Literature. London: Public Health England.Google Scholar
24. Adolphus, K, Lawton, CL & Dye, L (2013) The effects of breakfast on behavior and academic performance in children and adolescents. Front Hum Neurosci 7, 425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25. Moore, GF, Murphy, S, Chaplin, K et al. (2014) Impacts of the Primary School Free Breakfast Initiative on socio-economic inequalities in breakfast consumption among 9–11-year-old schoolchildren in Wales. Public Health Nutr 17, 12801289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26. Edmunds, LD & Ziebland, S (2002) Development and validation of the Day in the Life Questionnaire (DILQ) as a measure of fruit and vegetable questionnaire for 7–9 year olds. Health Educ Res 17, 211220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
27. Moore, GF, Tapper, K, Murphy, S et al. (2006) Validation of a self-completion measure of breakfast foods, snacks and fruits and vegetables consumed by 9- to 11-year-old schoolchildren. Eur J Clin Nutr 61, 420430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28. Murphy, S, Moore, GF, Tapper, K et al. (2011) Free healthy breakfasts in primary schools: a cluster randomised controlled trial of a policy intervention in Wales, UK. Public Health Nutr 14, 219226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29. Lyons, R, Jones, K, John, G et al. (2009) The SAIL databank: linking multiple health and social care datasets. BMC Med Inform Decis 9, 3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30. Ford, D, Jones, K, Verplancke, J-P et al. (2009) The SAIL databank: building a national architecture for e-health research and evaluation. BMC Health Serv Res 9, 157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31. Edefonti, V, Rosato, V, Parpinel, M et al. (2014) The effect of breakfast composition and energy contribution on cognitive and academic performance: a systematic review. Am J Clin Nutr 100, 626656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32. Kleinman, RE, Hall, S, Green, H et al. (2002) Diet, breakfast, and academic performance in children. Ann Nutr Metab 46, 2430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33. Mahoney, CR, Taylor, HA, Kanarek, RB et al. (2005) Effect of breakfast composition on cognitive processes in elementary school children. Physiol Behav 85, 635645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34. O’Dea, JA & Mugridge, AC (2012) Nutritional quality of breakfast and physical activity independently predict the literacy and numeracy scores of children after adjusting for socioeconomic status. Health Educ Res 27, 975985.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35. Lien, L (2007) Is breakfast consumption related to mental distress and academic performance in adolescents? Public Health Nutr 10, 422428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Descriptive statistics of the samples of Year 5 and 6 students aged 9–11 years participating in the cluster-randomised controlled trial of the Welsh Government’s Primary School Free Breakfast Initiative

Figure 1

Table 2 Odds ratios and 95 % confidence intervals from multilevel binary logistic regression analysis using individual- and school-level demographics and baseline (n 3093) and follow-up data (N 3055) to investigate the associations between dietary behaviours and improved educational outcomes among Year 5 and 6 students aged 9–11 years participating in the cluster-randomised controlled trial of the Welsh Government’s Primary School Free Breakfast Initiative

Figure 2

Table 3 Nested cohort analysis (n 1216) to investigate the associations between dietary behaviours and improved educational outcomes among Year 5 and 6 students aged 9–11 years participating in the cluster-randomised controlled trial of the Welsh Government’s Primary School Free Breakfast Initiative for whom dietary behaviour is available at baseline and follow-up