Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T19:22:55.812Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

From extreme emotions to extreme actions: Explaining non-normative collective action and reconciliation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 November 2012

Allard R. Feddes
Affiliation:
University of Amsterdam, Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences, Social Psychology Department, 1018 XA Amsterdam, The Netherlands. [email protected]://home.medewerker.uva.nl/a.r.feddes/[email protected]://home.medewerker.uva.nl/l.mann/[email protected]://home.medewerker.uva.nl/e.j.doosje/
Liesbeth Mann
Affiliation:
University of Amsterdam, Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences, Social Psychology Department, 1018 XA Amsterdam, The Netherlands. [email protected]://home.medewerker.uva.nl/a.r.feddes/[email protected]://home.medewerker.uva.nl/l.mann/[email protected]://home.medewerker.uva.nl/e.j.doosje/
Bertjan Doosje
Affiliation:
University of Amsterdam, Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences, Social Psychology Department, 1018 XA Amsterdam, The Netherlands. [email protected]://home.medewerker.uva.nl/a.r.feddes/[email protected]://home.medewerker.uva.nl/l.mann/[email protected]://home.medewerker.uva.nl/e.j.doosje/

Abstract

A key argument of Dixon et al. in the target article is that prejudice reduction through intergroup contact and collective action work in opposite ways. We argue for a complementary approach focusing on extreme emotions to understand why people turn to non-normative collective action and to understand when and under what conditions extreme emotions may influence positive effects of contact on reconciliation.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bar-Tal, D. (2004) The necessity of observing real life situations: Palestinian-Israeli violence as a laboratory for learning about social behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology 34:677701.Google Scholar
BBC (2001, Sept.) Bin Laden's warning: Full text. Retrieved from http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/south_asia/1585636.stm.Google Scholar
De Wolf, A., & Doosje, B. (2010) Aanpak van radicalisme: Een psychologische analyse [Dealing with radicalization: A psychological analysis]. SWP.Google Scholar
Doosje, B., Branscombe, N. R., Spears, R. & Manstead, A. S. R. (1998) Guilty by association: When one's group has a negative history. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 75:872–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, A. H. (2011) Contempt: A hot feeling hidden under a cold jacket. In: Re-constructing emotional spaces: From experience to regulation, ed. Trnka, R., Balcar, K. & Kuška, M., pp. 7789. Prague College of Psychosocial Studies Press.Google Scholar
Fischer, A. H. & Roseman, I. J. (2007) Beat them or ban them: The characteristics and social functions of anger and contempt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 93:103–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartling, L. M. & Luchetta, T. (1999) Humiliation: Assessing the impact of derision, degradation, and debasement. The Journal of Primary Intervention 19:259–78.Google Scholar
Leyens, J.-P., Paladino, P. M., Rodriguez-Torres, R., Vaes, J., Demoulin, S., Rodriguez-Perez, A. & Gaunt, R. (2000) The emotional side of prejudice: The attribution of secondary emotions to ingroups and outgroups. Personality and Social Psychology Review 4:186–97.Google Scholar
Lindner, E. G. (2001) Humiliation as the source of terrorism: A new paradigm. Peace Research 33:5968.Google Scholar
Mackie, D. M., Smith, E. R. & Ray, D. G. (2008) Intergroup emotions and intergroup relations. Personality and Social Psychology Compass 2:1866–80.Google Scholar
Martin, J. (1986) The tolerance of injustice. In: Relative deprivation and social comparison: The Ontario Symposium, vol. 4, ed. Olson, J. M., Herman, C. P. & Zanna, M. P., pp. 217–42. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
O'Gorman, R. (2010) The evolutionary logic of terrorism. In: The Psychology of Counter-Terrorism, ed. Silke, A., pp. 6275. Routledge.Google Scholar
Rimé, B., Kanyangara, P., Yzerbyt, V. & Paezt, D. (2011) The impact of Gacaca tribunals in Rwanda: Psychosocial effects of participation in a truth and reconciliation process after a genocide. European Journal of Social Psychology 41:695706.Google Scholar
Smith, D. (2008) Globalization, degradation and the dynamics of humiliation. Current Sociology 56:371–79.Google Scholar
Staub, E. (1989) The roots of evil: The origins of genocide and other group violence. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2003) A duplex theory of hate: Development and application to terrorism, massacres, and genocide. Review of General Psychology 7:299328.Google Scholar
Tausch, N., Becker, J. C., Spears, R., Christ, O., Saab, R., Singh, P. & Siddiqui, R. N. (2011) Explaining radical group behavior: Developing emotion and efficacy routes to normative and nonnormative collective action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 101:129–48.Google Scholar
Wright, S. C., Taylor, D. M. & Moghaddam, F. M. (1990) Responding to membership in a disadvantaged group: From acceptance to collective protest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58:9941003.Google Scholar