No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Magnitude, numerosity, and development of number: Implications for mathematics disabilities
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 27 July 2017
Abstract
Leibovich et al. challenge the prevailing view that non-symbolic number sense (e.g., sensing number the same way one might sense color) is innate, that detection of numerosity is distinct from detection of continuous magnitude. In the present commentary, the authors' viewpoint is discussed in light of the integrative theory of numerical development along with implications for understanding mathematics disabilities.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017
References
Butterworth, B. (1999) Perspectives: Neuroscience – A head for figures. Science
284(5416):928–29. doi: 10.1126/science.284.5416.928.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, B. (2005) The development of arithmetical abilities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
46(1):3–18. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00374.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butterworth, B. & Reigosa-Crespo, V. (2007) Information processing deficits in dyscalculia. In: Why is math so hard for some children? The nature and origins of mathematical learning difficulties and disabilities, ed. Berch, D. B. & Mazzocco, M. M. M., pp. 107–20. Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Clarke, B. & Shinn, M. R. (2004) A preliminary investigation into the identification and development of early mathematics curriculum-based measurement. School Psychology Review
33(22):234–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S. (1997) The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Frye, D., Baroody, A. J., Burchinal, M., Carver, S. M., Jordan, N. C. & McDowell, J. (2013) Teaching math to young children: A practice guide (NCEE 2014-4005). U.S. Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences, National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance. Available at: http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/ pdf/practice_guides/early_math_pg_111313.pdf.Google Scholar
Hansen, N., Jordan, N. C., Fernandez, E., Siegler, R. S., Fuchs, L. S., Gersten, R. & Micklos, D. A. (2015) General and math-specific predictors of sixth-graders' knowledge of fractions. Cognitive Development
35:34–49. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2015.02.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hassinger-Das, B., Jordan, N. C., Glutting, J., Irwin, C. & Dyson, N. (2014) Domain general mediators of the relation between kindergarten number sense and first-grade mathematics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
118:78–92. doi: 10.1016/jcep.2013.09.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jordan, N. C. & Dyson, N. (2016) Catching math problems early: Findings from the number sense intervention project. In Continuous issues in numerical cognition: How many or how much? ed. Henik, A., pp. 60–81. Elsevier.Google Scholar
Jordan, N. C., Hansen, N., Fuchs, L. S., Siegler, R. S., Gersten, R. & Micklos, D. (2013) Developmental predictors of fraction concepts and procedures. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
116:45–58. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.02.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landerl, K., Bevan, A. & Butterworth, B. (2004) Developmental dyscalculia and basic numerical capacities: A study of 8–9–year-old students. Cognition
93:99–125. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.11.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, P. G., Lewis, M. R. & Hubbard E. M. (2016) Individual differences in nonsymbolic ratio processing predict symbolic math performance. Psychological Science
27(2):191–202. doi: 10.1177/0956797615617799.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazzocco, M. M. M. & Thompson, R. E. (2005) Kindergarten predictors of math learning disability. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice
20(3):142–55. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5826.2005.00129.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McMullen, J., Hannula-Sormunen, M. M. & Lehtinen, E. (2015) Preschool spontaneous focusing on numerosity predicts rational number conceptual knowledge 6 years later. ZDM Mathematics Education
47(5):813–24. doi: 10.1007/s11858-015-0669-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Research Council (2009) Mathematics learning in early childhood: Paths toward excellence and equity, ed. Committee on Early Childhood Mathematics, Cross, C.T., Woods, T.A. & Schweingruber, H.. Center for Education, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education. National Academies Press.Google Scholar
Resnick, I., Jordan, N. C., Hansen, N., Rajan, V., Rodrigues, J., Siegler, R. S. & Fuchs, L. (2016) Developmental growth trajectories in understanding of fraction magnitude from fourth through sixth grade. Developmental Psychology
52(5):746–57. doi: 10.1037/dev0000102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rinne, L., Ye, A. & Jordan, N. C. (2017) Development of fraction comparison strategies: A latent transition analysis. Developmental Psychology
53(4):713–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rousselle, L. & Noel, M. P. (2007) Basic numerical skills in children with mathematics learning disabilities: A comparison of symbolic vs non-symbolic number magnitude processing. Cognition
102:361–95. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.01.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegler, R. S. & Lortie-Forgues, H. (2014) An integrative theory of numerical development. Child Development Perspectives
8(3):144–50. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12077.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zentall, S. S., Smith, Y. N., Lee, Y. B. & Wieczorek, C. (1994) Mathematical outcomes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Learning Disabilities
27(8):510–19. doi:10.1177/002221949402700806.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Target article
From “sense of number” to “sense of magnitude”: The role of continuous magnitudes in numerical cognition
Related commentaries (29)
A “sense of magnitude” requires a new alternative for learning numerical symbols
Approximate number sense theory or approximate theory of magnitude?
Commentary on Leibovich et al.: What next?
Computational foundations of the visual number sense
Controlling for continuous variables is not futile: What we can learn about number representation despite imperfect control
Direct and rapid encoding of numerosity in the visual stream
Evidence for a number sense
From continuous magnitudes to symbolic numbers: The centrality of ratio
How not to develop a sense of number
Infants discriminate number: Evidence against the prerequisite of visual object individuation and the primacy of continuous magnitude
Infants, animals, and the origins of number
Innateness of magnitude perception? Skill can be acquired and mastered at all ages
Is the ANS linked to mathematics performance?
Magnitude rather than number: More evidence needed
Magnitude, numerosity, and development of number: Implications for mathematics disabilities
Multitudes are adaptable magnitudes in the estimation of number
Number faculty is alive and kicking: On number discriminations and number neurons
Numerical intuitions in infancy: Give credit where credit is due
Numerical magnitude evaluation as a foundation for decision making
Perceiving numerosity from birth
Right idea, wrong magnitude system
Selecting the model that best fits the data
The contribution of fish studies to the “number sense” debate
The contributions of non-numeric dimensions to number encoding, representations, and decision-making factors
The evolvement of discrete representations from continuous stimulus properties: A possible overarching principle of cognition
The number sense is neither last resort nor of primary import
What is a number? The interplay between number and continuous magnitudes
What is the precise role of cognitive control in the development of a sense of number?
Why try saving the ANS? An alternative proposal
Author response
Toward an integrative approach to numerical cognition